Skip to main content
Top
Gepubliceerd in: Psychological Research 1/2017

30-09-2015 | Original Article

Naturally together: pitch-height and brightness as coupled factors for eliciting the SMARC effect in non-musicians

Auteurs: Marco Pitteri, Mauro Marchetti, Konstantinos Priftis, Massimo Grassi

Gepubliceerd in: Psychological Research | Uitgave 1/2017

Log in om toegang te krijgen
share
DELEN

Deel dit onderdeel of sectie (kopieer de link)

  • Optie A:
    Klik op de rechtermuisknop op de link en selecteer de optie “linkadres kopiëren”
  • Optie B:
    Deel de link per e-mail

Abstract

Pitch-height is often labeled spatially (i.e., low or high) as a function of the fundamental frequency of the tone. This correspondence is highlighted by the so-called Spatial–Musical Association of Response Codes (SMARC) effect. However, the literature suggests that the brightness of the tone’s timbre might contribute to this spatial association. We investigated the SMARC effect in a group of non-musicians by disentangling the role of pitch-height and the role of tone-brightness. In three experimental conditions, participants were asked to judge whether the tone they were listening to was (or was not) modulated in amplitude (i.e., vibrato). Participants were required to make their response in both the horizontal and the vertical axes. In a first condition, tones varied coherently in pitch (i.e., manipulation of the tone’s F0) and brightness (i.e., manipulation of the tone’s spectral centroid); in a second condition, pitch-height varied whereas brightness was fixed; in a third condition, pitch-height was fixed whereas brightness varied. We found the SMARC effect only in the first condition and only in the vertical axis. In contrast, we did not observe the effect in any of the remaining conditions. The present results suggest that, in non-musicians, the SMARC effect is not due to the manipulation of the pitch-height alone, but arises because of a coherent change of pitch-height and brightness; this effect emerges along the vertical axis only.
Bijlagen
Alleen toegankelijk voor geautoriseerde gebruikers
Literatuur
go back to reference Adair, J. C., Williamson, D. J., Jacobs, D. H., Na, D. L., & Heilman, K. M. (1995). Neglect of radial and vertical space: importance of the retinotopic reference frame. Journal of Neurology, Neurosurgery and Psychiatry, 58(6), 724–728.CrossRefPubMedPubMedCentral Adair, J. C., Williamson, D. J., Jacobs, D. H., Na, D. L., & Heilman, K. M. (1995). Neglect of radial and vertical space: importance of the retinotopic reference frame. Journal of Neurology, Neurosurgery and Psychiatry, 58(6), 724–728.CrossRefPubMedPubMedCentral
go back to reference Berch, D. B., Foley, E. J., Hill, R. J., & Ryan, P. M. (1999). Extracting parity and magnitude from Arabic numerals: developmental changes in number processing and mental representation. Journal of Experimental Child Psychology, 74(4), 286–308.CrossRefPubMed Berch, D. B., Foley, E. J., Hill, R. J., & Ryan, P. M. (1999). Extracting parity and magnitude from Arabic numerals: developmental changes in number processing and mental representation. Journal of Experimental Child Psychology, 74(4), 286–308.CrossRefPubMed
go back to reference Berlucchi, G., Tassinari, G., & Aglioti, S. (1994). Callosal pathways for simple visuomotor control in man. Rendiconti Lincei, 5(2), 191–201.CrossRef Berlucchi, G., Tassinari, G., & Aglioti, S. (1994). Callosal pathways for simple visuomotor control in man. Rendiconti Lincei, 5(2), 191–201.CrossRef
go back to reference Borchert, E. M. O., Micheyl, C., & Oxenham, A. J. (2011). Perceptual grouping affects pitch judgments across time and frequency. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 37(1), 257–269.PubMedPubMedCentral Borchert, E. M. O., Micheyl, C., & Oxenham, A. J. (2011). Perceptual grouping affects pitch judgments across time and frequency. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 37(1), 257–269.PubMedPubMedCentral
go back to reference Cappelletti, M., Freeman, E. D., & Cipolotti, L. (2007). The middle house or the middle floor: bisecting horizontal and vertical mental number lines in neglect. Neuropsychologia, 45(13), 2989–3000.CrossRefPubMedPubMedCentral Cappelletti, M., Freeman, E. D., & Cipolotti, L. (2007). The middle house or the middle floor: bisecting horizontal and vertical mental number lines in neglect. Neuropsychologia, 45(13), 2989–3000.CrossRefPubMedPubMedCentral
go back to reference Cho, Y. S., Bae, G. Y., & Proctor, R. W. (2012). Referential coding contributes to the horizontal SMARC effect. Journal of Experimental Psychology Human Perception and Performance, 38(3), 726–734.CrossRefPubMed Cho, Y. S., Bae, G. Y., & Proctor, R. W. (2012). Referential coding contributes to the horizontal SMARC effect. Journal of Experimental Psychology Human Perception and Performance, 38(3), 726–734.CrossRefPubMed
go back to reference Cho, Y. S., & Proctor, R. W. (2003). Stimulus and response representations underlying orthogonal stimulus–response compatibility effects. Psychonomic Bulletin and Review, 10(1), 45–73.CrossRefPubMed Cho, Y. S., & Proctor, R. W. (2003). Stimulus and response representations underlying orthogonal stimulus–response compatibility effects. Psychonomic Bulletin and Review, 10(1), 45–73.CrossRefPubMed
go back to reference Cohen Kadosh, R., & Henik, A. (2006). A common representation for semantic and physical properties. Experimental Psychology, 53(2), 87–94.CrossRefPubMed Cohen Kadosh, R., & Henik, A. (2006). A common representation for semantic and physical properties. Experimental Psychology, 53(2), 87–94.CrossRefPubMed
go back to reference Cohen Kadosh, R., Sagiv, N., Linden, D. E. J., Robertson, L. C., Elinger, G., & Henik, A. (2005). When blue is larger than red: colors influence numerical cognition in synesthesia. Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience, 17(11), 1766–1773.CrossRefPubMed Cohen Kadosh, R., Sagiv, N., Linden, D. E. J., Robertson, L. C., Elinger, G., & Henik, A. (2005). When blue is larger than red: colors influence numerical cognition in synesthesia. Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience, 17(11), 1766–1773.CrossRefPubMed
go back to reference Dehaene, S., Bossini, S., & Giraux, P. (1993). The mental representation of parity and number magnitude. Journal of Experimental Psychology General, 122(3), 371–396.CrossRef Dehaene, S., Bossini, S., & Giraux, P. (1993). The mental representation of parity and number magnitude. Journal of Experimental Psychology General, 122(3), 371–396.CrossRef
go back to reference Fias, W., Lammertyn, J., Reynvoet, B., Dupont, P., & Orban, G. A. (2003). Parietal representation of symbolic and nonsymbolic magnitude. Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience, 15(1), 47–56.CrossRefPubMed Fias, W., Lammertyn, J., Reynvoet, B., Dupont, P., & Orban, G. A. (2003). Parietal representation of symbolic and nonsymbolic magnitude. Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience, 15(1), 47–56.CrossRefPubMed
go back to reference Fischer, M. H., Riello, M., Giordano, B. L., & Rusconi, E. (2013). Singing numbers…in cognitive space–a dual-task study of the link between pitch, space, and numbers. Topics in Cognitive Science, 5(2), 354–366.CrossRefPubMed Fischer, M. H., Riello, M., Giordano, B. L., & Rusconi, E. (2013). Singing numbers…in cognitive space–a dual-task study of the link between pitch, space, and numbers. Topics in Cognitive Science, 5(2), 354–366.CrossRefPubMed
go back to reference Fischer, M. H., & Shaki, S. (2014). Spatial associations in numerical cognition–from single digits to arithmetic. Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology (2006), 67(8), 1461–1483.CrossRef Fischer, M. H., & Shaki, S. (2014). Spatial associations in numerical cognition–from single digits to arithmetic. Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology (2006), 67(8), 1461–1483.CrossRef
go back to reference Geldmacher, D. S., & Heilman, K. M. (1994). Visual field influence on radial line bisection. Brain and Cognition, 26(1), 65–72.CrossRefPubMed Geldmacher, D. S., & Heilman, K. M. (1994). Visual field influence on radial line bisection. Brain and Cognition, 26(1), 65–72.CrossRefPubMed
go back to reference Gevers, W., Reynvoet, B., & Fias, W. (2003). The mental representation of ordinal sequences is spatially. Cognition, 87(3), B87–B95.CrossRefPubMed Gevers, W., Reynvoet, B., & Fias, W. (2003). The mental representation of ordinal sequences is spatially. Cognition, 87(3), B87–B95.CrossRefPubMed
go back to reference Gevers, W., Reynvoet, B., & Fias, W. (2004). The mental representation of ordinal sequences is spatially organized: evidence from days of the week. Cortex, 40(1), 171–172.CrossRefPubMed Gevers, W., Reynvoet, B., & Fias, W. (2004). The mental representation of ordinal sequences is spatially organized: evidence from days of the week. Cortex, 40(1), 171–172.CrossRefPubMed
go back to reference Gevers, W., Verguts, T., Reynvoet, B., Caessens, B., & Fias, W. (2006). Numbers and space: a computational model of the SNARC effect. Journal of Experimental Psychology Human Perception and Performance, 32(1), 32–44.CrossRefPubMed Gevers, W., Verguts, T., Reynvoet, B., Caessens, B., & Fias, W. (2006). Numbers and space: a computational model of the SNARC effect. Journal of Experimental Psychology Human Perception and Performance, 32(1), 32–44.CrossRefPubMed
go back to reference Gibson, J. J. (1963). The useful dimensions of sensitivity. American Psychologist, 18(1), 1–15.CrossRef Gibson, J. J. (1963). The useful dimensions of sensitivity. American Psychologist, 18(1), 1–15.CrossRef
go back to reference Giordano, B. L., Rocchesso, D., & McAdams, S. (2010). Integration of acoustical information in the perception of impacted sound sources: the role of information accuracy and exploitability. Journal of Experimental Psychology Human Perception and Performance, 36(2), 462–476.CrossRefPubMed Giordano, B. L., Rocchesso, D., & McAdams, S. (2010). Integration of acoustical information in the perception of impacted sound sources: the role of information accuracy and exploitability. Journal of Experimental Psychology Human Perception and Performance, 36(2), 462–476.CrossRefPubMed
go back to reference Gobel, S. M., Shaki, S., & Fischer, M. H. (2011). The cultural number line: a review of cultural and linguistic influences on the development of number processing. Journal of Cross-Cultural Psychology, 42(4), 543–565.CrossRef Gobel, S. M., Shaki, S., & Fischer, M. H. (2011). The cultural number line: a review of cultural and linguistic influences on the development of number processing. Journal of Cross-Cultural Psychology, 42(4), 543–565.CrossRef
go back to reference Gordon, M. S., Russo, F. A., & MacDonald, E. (2013). Spectral information for detection of acoustic time to arrival. Attention, Perception, and Psychophysics, 75(4), 738–750.CrossRef Gordon, M. S., Russo, F. A., & MacDonald, E. (2013). Spectral information for detection of acoustic time to arrival. Attention, Perception, and Psychophysics, 75(4), 738–750.CrossRef
go back to reference Grade, S., Lefèvre, N., & Pesenti, M. (2013). Influence of gaze observation on random number generation. Experimental Psychology, 60(2), 122–130.CrossRefPubMed Grade, S., Lefèvre, N., & Pesenti, M. (2013). Influence of gaze observation on random number generation. Experimental Psychology, 60(2), 122–130.CrossRefPubMed
go back to reference Grassi, M. (2005). Do we hear size or sound? Balls dropped on plates. Perception and Psychophysics, 67(2), 274–284.CrossRefPubMed Grassi, M. (2005). Do we hear size or sound? Balls dropped on plates. Perception and Psychophysics, 67(2), 274–284.CrossRefPubMed
go back to reference Grassi, M., Pastore, M., & Lemaitre, G. (2013). Looking at the world with your ears: how do we get the size of an object from its sound? Acta Psychologica, 143(1), 96–104.CrossRefPubMed Grassi, M., Pastore, M., & Lemaitre, G. (2013). Looking at the world with your ears: how do we get the size of an object from its sound? Acta Psychologica, 143(1), 96–104.CrossRefPubMed
go back to reference Grassi, M., & Soranzo, A. (2009). MLP: a MATLAB toolbox for rapid and reliable auditory threshold estimation. Behavior Research Methods, 41(1), 20–28.CrossRefPubMed Grassi, M., & Soranzo, A. (2009). MLP: a MATLAB toolbox for rapid and reliable auditory threshold estimation. Behavior Research Methods, 41(1), 20–28.CrossRefPubMed
go back to reference Grey, J. M., & Gordon, J. W. (1978). Perceptual effects of spectral modifications on musical timbres. The Journal of the Acoustical Society of America, 63(5), 1493–1500.CrossRef Grey, J. M., & Gordon, J. W. (1978). Perceptual effects of spectral modifications on musical timbres. The Journal of the Acoustical Society of America, 63(5), 1493–1500.CrossRef
go back to reference Haas, E. C., & Edworthy, J. (1996). Designing urgency into auditory warnings using pitch, speed and loudness. Computing and Control Engineering Journal, 7(4), 193.CrossRef Haas, E. C., & Edworthy, J. (1996). Designing urgency into auditory warnings using pitch, speed and loudness. Computing and Control Engineering Journal, 7(4), 193.CrossRef
go back to reference Hartmann, M., Gashaj, V., Stahnke, A., & Mast, F. W. (2014). There is more than “more is up”: hand and foot responses reverse the vertical association of number magnitudes. Journal of Experimental Psychology Human Perception and Performance, 40(4), 1401–1414.CrossRefPubMed Hartmann, M., Gashaj, V., Stahnke, A., & Mast, F. W. (2014). There is more than “more is up”: hand and foot responses reverse the vertical association of number magnitudes. Journal of Experimental Psychology Human Perception and Performance, 40(4), 1401–1414.CrossRefPubMed
go back to reference Hartmann, M., Grabherr, L., & Mast, F. W. (2012). Moving along the mental number line: interactions between whole-body motion and numerical cognition. Journal of Experimental Psychology Human Perception and Performance, 38(6), 1416–1427.CrossRefPubMed Hartmann, M., Grabherr, L., & Mast, F. W. (2012). Moving along the mental number line: interactions between whole-body motion and numerical cognition. Journal of Experimental Psychology Human Perception and Performance, 38(6), 1416–1427.CrossRefPubMed
go back to reference Hartmann, M., Mast, F. W., & Fischer, M. H. (2015). Spatial biases during mental arithmetic: evidence from eye movements on a blank screen. Frontiers in Psychology, 6, 1–8.CrossRef Hartmann, M., Mast, F. W., & Fischer, M. H. (2015). Spatial biases during mental arithmetic: evidence from eye movements on a blank screen. Frontiers in Psychology, 6, 1–8.CrossRef
go back to reference Holmes, K. J., & Lourenco, S. F. (2012). Orienting numbers in mental space: horizontal organization trumps vertical. Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology, 65(6), 1044–1051.CrossRef Holmes, K. J., & Lourenco, S. F. (2012). Orienting numbers in mental space: horizontal organization trumps vertical. Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology, 65(6), 1044–1051.CrossRef
go back to reference Ishihara, M., Keller, P. E., Rossetti, Y., & Prinz, W. (2008). Horizontal spatial representations of time: evidence for the STEARC effect. Cortex, 44(4), 454–461.CrossRefPubMed Ishihara, M., Keller, P. E., Rossetti, Y., & Prinz, W. (2008). Horizontal spatial representations of time: evidence for the STEARC effect. Cortex, 44(4), 454–461.CrossRefPubMed
go back to reference Ito, Y., & Hatta, T. (2004). Spatial structure of quantitative representation of numbers: evidence from the SNARC effect. Memory and Cognition, 32(4), 662–673.CrossRefPubMed Ito, Y., & Hatta, T. (2004). Spatial structure of quantitative representation of numbers: evidence from the SNARC effect. Memory and Cognition, 32(4), 662–673.CrossRefPubMed
go back to reference Lega, C., Cattaneo, Z., Merabet, L. B., Vecchi, T., & Cucchi, S. (2014). The effect of musical expertise on the representation of space. Frontiers in Human Neuroscience, 8, 250.CrossRefPubMedPubMedCentral Lega, C., Cattaneo, Z., Merabet, L. B., Vecchi, T., & Cucchi, S. (2014). The effect of musical expertise on the representation of space. Frontiers in Human Neuroscience, 8, 250.CrossRefPubMedPubMedCentral
go back to reference Lewkowicz, D. J. & Minar, N. (2014). Infants are not sensitive to synesthetic cross-modality correspondences. A comment to Walker et al. (2010). Psychological Science, 25, 832–834.CrossRefPubMedPubMedCentral Lewkowicz, D. J. & Minar, N. (2014). Infants are not sensitive to synesthetic cross-modality correspondences. A comment to Walker et al. (2010). Psychological Science, 25, 832–834.CrossRefPubMedPubMedCentral
go back to reference Lidji, P., Kolinsky, R., Lochy, A., & Morais, J. (2007). Spatial associations for musical stimuli: a piano in the head? Journal of Experimental Psychology Human Perception and Performance, 33(5), 1189–1207.CrossRefPubMed Lidji, P., Kolinsky, R., Lochy, A., & Morais, J. (2007). Spatial associations for musical stimuli: a piano in the head? Journal of Experimental Psychology Human Perception and Performance, 33(5), 1189–1207.CrossRefPubMed
go back to reference Loetscher, T., Bockisch, C. J., Nicholls, M. E. R., & Brugger, P. (2010). Eye position predicts what number you have in mind. Current Biology CB, 20(6), R264–R265.CrossRefPubMed Loetscher, T., Bockisch, C. J., Nicholls, M. E. R., & Brugger, P. (2010). Eye position predicts what number you have in mind. Current Biology CB, 20(6), R264–R265.CrossRefPubMed
go back to reference Ludwig, V. U., Adachi, I., & Matsuzawa, T. (2011). Visuoauditory mappings between high luminance and high pitch are shared by chimpanzees (Pan troglodytes) and humans. PNAS, 108(51), 20661–20665.CrossRefPubMedPubMedCentral Ludwig, V. U., Adachi, I., & Matsuzawa, T. (2011). Visuoauditory mappings between high luminance and high pitch are shared by chimpanzees (Pan troglodytes) and humans. PNAS, 108(51), 20661–20665.CrossRefPubMedPubMedCentral
go back to reference McAdams, S. (2012). Musical timbre perception. In D. Deutsch (Ed.), The psychology of music (pp. 35–67). Oxford: Elsevier Academic Press. McAdams, S. (2012). Musical timbre perception. In D. Deutsch (Ed.), The psychology of music (pp. 35–67). Oxford: Elsevier Academic Press.
go back to reference McAdams, S., Winsberg, S., Donnadieu, S., De Soete, G., & Krimphoff, J. (1995). Perceptual scaling of synthesized musical timbres: common dimensions, specificities, and latent subject classes. Psychological Research, 58(3), 177–192.CrossRefPubMed McAdams, S., Winsberg, S., Donnadieu, S., De Soete, G., & Krimphoff, J. (1995). Perceptual scaling of synthesized musical timbres: common dimensions, specificities, and latent subject classes. Psychological Research, 58(3), 177–192.CrossRefPubMed
go back to reference Melara, R. D., & Marks, L. E. (1990). Interaction among auditory dimensions: timbre, pitch, and loudness. Perception and Psychophysics, 48(2), 169–178.CrossRefPubMed Melara, R. D., & Marks, L. E. (1990). Interaction among auditory dimensions: timbre, pitch, and loudness. Perception and Psychophysics, 48(2), 169–178.CrossRefPubMed
go back to reference Müller, D., & Schwarz, W. (2007). Is there an internal association of numbers to hands? The task set influences the nature of the SNARC effect. Memory and Cognition, 35(5), 1151–1161.CrossRefPubMed Müller, D., & Schwarz, W. (2007). Is there an internal association of numbers to hands? The task set influences the nature of the SNARC effect. Memory and Cognition, 35(5), 1151–1161.CrossRefPubMed
go back to reference Nishimura, A., & Yokosawa, K. (2009). Effects of laterality and pitch height of an auditory accessory stimulus on horizontal response selection: the Simon effect and the SMARC effect. Psychonomic Bulletin and Review, 16(4), 666–670.CrossRefPubMed Nishimura, A., & Yokosawa, K. (2009). Effects of laterality and pitch height of an auditory accessory stimulus on horizontal response selection: the Simon effect and the SMARC effect. Psychonomic Bulletin and Review, 16(4), 666–670.CrossRefPubMed
go back to reference Nygaard, L. C., Herold, D. S., & Namy, L. L. (2009). The semantics of prosody: acoustic and perceptual evidence of prosodic correlates to word meaning. Cognitive Science, 33(1), 127–146.CrossRefPubMed Nygaard, L. C., Herold, D. S., & Namy, L. L. (2009). The semantics of prosody: acoustic and perceptual evidence of prosodic correlates to word meaning. Cognitive Science, 33(1), 127–146.CrossRefPubMed
go back to reference Parkinson, C., Kohler, P. J., Sievers, B., & Wheatley, T. (2012). Associations between auditory pitch and visual elevation do not depend on language: evidence from a remote population. Perception, 41(7), 854–861.CrossRefPubMed Parkinson, C., Kohler, P. J., Sievers, B., & Wheatley, T. (2012). Associations between auditory pitch and visual elevation do not depend on language: evidence from a remote population. Perception, 41(7), 854–861.CrossRefPubMed
go back to reference Pratt, C. C. (1930). The spatial character of high and low tones. Journal of Experimental Psychology, 13, 278–285.CrossRef Pratt, C. C. (1930). The spatial character of high and low tones. Journal of Experimental Psychology, 13, 278–285.CrossRef
go back to reference Rugani, R., Vallortigara, G., Priftis, K., & Regolin, L. (2015). Animal cognition. Number-space mapping in the newborn chick resembles humans’ mental number line. Science, 347(6221), 534–536.CrossRefPubMed Rugani, R., Vallortigara, G., Priftis, K., & Regolin, L. (2015). Animal cognition. Number-space mapping in the newborn chick resembles humans’ mental number line. Science, 347(6221), 534–536.CrossRefPubMed
go back to reference Rusconi, E., Kwan, B., Giordano, B. L., Umiltà, C., & Butterworth, B. (2006). Spatial representation of pitch height: the SMARC effect. Cognition, 99(2), 113–129.CrossRefPubMed Rusconi, E., Kwan, B., Giordano, B. L., Umiltà, C., & Butterworth, B. (2006). Spatial representation of pitch height: the SMARC effect. Cognition, 99(2), 113–129.CrossRefPubMed
go back to reference Russo, F. A., & Thompson, W. F. (2005). The subjective size of melodic intervals over a two-octave range. Psychonomic Bulletin and Review, 12(6), 1068–1075.CrossRefPubMed Russo, F. A., & Thompson, W. F. (2005). The subjective size of melodic intervals over a two-octave range. Psychonomic Bulletin and Review, 12(6), 1068–1075.CrossRefPubMed
go back to reference Shaki, S., & Fischer, M. H. (2012). Multiple spatial mappings in numerical cognition. Journal of Experimental Psychology Human Perception and Performance, 38(3), 804–809.CrossRefPubMed Shaki, S., & Fischer, M. H. (2012). Multiple spatial mappings in numerical cognition. Journal of Experimental Psychology Human Perception and Performance, 38(3), 804–809.CrossRefPubMed
go back to reference Shaki, S., Fischer, M. H., & Petrusic, W. M. (2009). Reading habits for both words and numbers contribute to the SNARC effect. Psychonomic Bulletin and Review, 16(2), 328–331.CrossRefPubMed Shaki, S., Fischer, M. H., & Petrusic, W. M. (2009). Reading habits for both words and numbers contribute to the SNARC effect. Psychonomic Bulletin and Review, 16(2), 328–331.CrossRefPubMed
go back to reference Shimizu, H. (2002). Measuring keyboard response delays by comparing keyboard and joystick inputs. Behavior Research Methods, Instruments, and Computers, 34(2), 250–256.CrossRefPubMed Shimizu, H. (2002). Measuring keyboard response delays by comparing keyboard and joystick inputs. Behavior Research Methods, Instruments, and Computers, 34(2), 250–256.CrossRefPubMed
go back to reference Stern, D. N., Spieker, S., & MacKain, K. (1982). Intonation contours as signals in maternal speech to prelinguistic infants. Developmental Psychology, 18(5), 727–735.CrossRef Stern, D. N., Spieker, S., & MacKain, K. (1982). Intonation contours as signals in maternal speech to prelinguistic infants. Developmental Psychology, 18(5), 727–735.CrossRef
go back to reference Vallesi, A., Binns, M. A., & Shallice, T. (2008). An effect of spatial–temporal association of response codes: understanding the cognitive representations of time. Cognition, 107(2), 501–527.CrossRefPubMed Vallesi, A., Binns, M. A., & Shallice, T. (2008). An effect of spatial–temporal association of response codes: understanding the cognitive representations of time. Cognition, 107(2), 501–527.CrossRefPubMed
go back to reference Van Selst, M., & Jolicoeur, P. (1994). A solution to the effect of sample size on outlier elimination. The Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology Section A, 47(3), 631–650.CrossRef Van Selst, M., & Jolicoeur, P. (1994). A solution to the effect of sample size on outlier elimination. The Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology Section A, 47(3), 631–650.CrossRef
go back to reference Vu, K. P., Proctor, R. W., & Pick, D. F. (2000). Vertical versus horizontal spatial compatibility: right–left prevalence with bimanual responses. Psychological Research, 64(1), 25–40.CrossRefPubMed Vu, K. P., Proctor, R. W., & Pick, D. F. (2000). Vertical versus horizontal spatial compatibility: right–left prevalence with bimanual responses. Psychological Research, 64(1), 25–40.CrossRefPubMed
go back to reference Walker, P., Bremner, J. G., Mason, U., Spring, J., Mattock, K., Slater, A., & Johnson, S. P. (2010). Preverbal infants’ sensitivity to synaesthetic cross-modality correspondences. Psychological Science, 21, 21–25.CrossRefPubMed Walker, P., Bremner, J. G., Mason, U., Spring, J., Mattock, K., Slater, A., & Johnson, S. P. (2010). Preverbal infants’ sensitivity to synaesthetic cross-modality correspondences. Psychological Science, 21, 21–25.CrossRefPubMed
go back to reference Wood, G., Nuerk, H.-C., & Willmes, K. (2006). Crossed hands and the SNARC effect: a failure to replicate Dehaene, Bossini, and Giraux (1993). Cortex, 42(8), 1069–1079.CrossRefPubMed Wood, G., Nuerk, H.-C., & Willmes, K. (2006). Crossed hands and the SNARC effect: a failure to replicate Dehaene, Bossini, and Giraux (1993). Cortex, 42(8), 1069–1079.CrossRefPubMed
go back to reference Zebian, S. (2005). Linkages between number concepts, spatial thinking, and directionality of writing: the SNARC effect and the REVERSE SNARC effect in English and Arabic monoliterates, biliterates, and illiterate Arabic speakers. Journal of Cognition and Culture, 5(1–2), 165–190.CrossRef Zebian, S. (2005). Linkages between number concepts, spatial thinking, and directionality of writing: the SNARC effect and the REVERSE SNARC effect in English and Arabic monoliterates, biliterates, and illiterate Arabic speakers. Journal of Cognition and Culture, 5(1–2), 165–190.CrossRef
Metagegevens
Titel
Naturally together: pitch-height and brightness as coupled factors for eliciting the SMARC effect in non-musicians
Auteurs
Marco Pitteri
Mauro Marchetti
Konstantinos Priftis
Massimo Grassi
Publicatiedatum
30-09-2015
Uitgeverij
Springer Berlin Heidelberg
Gepubliceerd in
Psychological Research / Uitgave 1/2017
Print ISSN: 0340-0727
Elektronisch ISSN: 1430-2772
DOI
https://doi.org/10.1007/s00426-015-0713-6

Andere artikelen Uitgave 1/2017

Psychological Research 1/2017 Naar de uitgave