Skip to main content
Top
Gepubliceerd in: Psychological Research 1/2017

19-11-2015 | Original Article

Observation and physical practice: different practice contexts lead to similar outcomes for the acquisition of kinematic information

Auteurs: John J. Buchanan, Inchon Park

Gepubliceerd in: Psychological Research | Uitgave 1/2017

Log in om toegang te krijgen
share
DELEN

Deel dit onderdeel of sectie (kopieer de link)

  • Optie A:
    Klik op de rechtermuisknop op de link en selecteer de optie “linkadres kopiëren”
  • Optie B:
    Deel de link per e-mail

Abstract

This study differentiated the contributions of physical and observational practice to the learning of a single-limb multi-joint coordination pattern. Three groups (physical-practice, observation-practice, observation-physical) practiced for 2 days and were given two performance tests 24 h after the second practice session. The performance tests revealed that physical and observational practice contributed similarly to identifying and using kinematic information related to the relative motion direction between joints (lead/lag relationship) and to the to-be-learned relative phase pattern (ϕ = 90°). Physical practice resulted in more stable coordination during performance tests and in the ability to produce different joint amplitudes with less variability. A serendipitous finding was that maximum elbow flexion (point of movement reversal) emerged as a kinematic event around which elbow and wrist coordination were organized. Movement reversals often serve to anchor the movement dynamics, and this anchoring effect was evident following both physical and observational practice, yet physical practice resulted in an advantage with regard to this anchor point on several kinematic measures. The results are discussed within the context of contemporary behavioral theories (coordination dynamics, visual perception perspective) of observational learning.
Voetnoten
1
The physical-practice participant that performed poorly during both performance tests showed a significant improvement in performance with practice. No explanation for poor performance during the performance tests is readily available. However, both the physical-practice participant and observer-practice participant that performed poorly based on the relative phase measures did perform very similar to the other participants in their groups based on joint amplitudes and harmonicity measures.
 
Literatuur
go back to reference Al-Abood, S. A., Davids, K., & Bennett, S. J. (2001a). Specificity of task constraints and effects of visual demonstrations and verbal instructions in directing learners’ search during skill acquisition. Journal of Motor Behavior, 33(3), 295–305.CrossRefPubMed Al-Abood, S. A., Davids, K., & Bennett, S. J. (2001a). Specificity of task constraints and effects of visual demonstrations and verbal instructions in directing learners’ search during skill acquisition. Journal of Motor Behavior, 33(3), 295–305.CrossRefPubMed
go back to reference Al-Abood, S. A., Davids, K., Bennett, S. J., Ashford, D., & Marin, M. M. (2001b). Effects of manipulating relative and absolute motion information during observational learning of an aiming task. Journal of Sports Sciences, 19, 507–520.CrossRefPubMed Al-Abood, S. A., Davids, K., Bennett, S. J., Ashford, D., & Marin, M. M. (2001b). Effects of manipulating relative and absolute motion information during observational learning of an aiming task. Journal of Sports Sciences, 19, 507–520.CrossRefPubMed
go back to reference Badets, A., & Blandin, Y. (2010). Feedback schedules for motor-skill learning: The similarities and differences between physical and observational practice. Journal of Motor Behavior, 42(4), 257–268.CrossRefPubMed Badets, A., & Blandin, Y. (2010). Feedback schedules for motor-skill learning: The similarities and differences between physical and observational practice. Journal of Motor Behavior, 42(4), 257–268.CrossRefPubMed
go back to reference Bingham, G. P., Schmidt, R. C., & Zaal, F. T. J. M. (1999). Visual perception of the relative phasing of human limb movements. Perception and Psychophysics, 61(2), 246–258.CrossRefPubMed Bingham, G. P., Schmidt, R. C., & Zaal, F. T. J. M. (1999). Visual perception of the relative phasing of human limb movements. Perception and Psychophysics, 61(2), 246–258.CrossRefPubMed
go back to reference Bingham, G. P., Zaal, F., Shull, J. A., & Collins, D. R. (2001). The effect of frequency on the visual perception of relative phase and phase variability of two oscillating objects. Experimental Brain Research, 136(4), 543–552. doi:10.1007/s002210000610.CrossRefPubMed Bingham, G. P., Zaal, F., Shull, J. A., & Collins, D. R. (2001). The effect of frequency on the visual perception of relative phase and phase variability of two oscillating objects. Experimental Brain Research, 136(4), 543–552. doi:10.​1007/​s002210000610.CrossRefPubMed
go back to reference Breslin, G., Hodges, N. J., Williams, A. M., Curran, W., & Kremer, J. (2005). Modelling relative motion to facilitate intra-limb coordination. Human Movement Science, 24, 446–463.CrossRefPubMed Breslin, G., Hodges, N. J., Williams, A. M., Curran, W., & Kremer, J. (2005). Modelling relative motion to facilitate intra-limb coordination. Human Movement Science, 24, 446–463.CrossRefPubMed
go back to reference Breslin, G., Hodges, N. J., Williams, A. M., Kremer, J., & Curran, W. (2006). A comparison of intra- and inter-limb relative motion information in modeling a novel motor skill. Human Movement Science, 25, 753–766.CrossRefPubMed Breslin, G., Hodges, N. J., Williams, A. M., Kremer, J., & Curran, W. (2006). A comparison of intra- and inter-limb relative motion information in modeling a novel motor skill. Human Movement Science, 25, 753–766.CrossRefPubMed
go back to reference Buchanan, J. J., & Dean, N. (2010). Specificity in practice benefits learning in novice models and variability in demonstration benefits observational practice. Psychological Research, 74, 313–320.CrossRefPubMed Buchanan, J. J., & Dean, N. (2010). Specificity in practice benefits learning in novice models and variability in demonstration benefits observational practice. Psychological Research, 74, 313–320.CrossRefPubMed
go back to reference Buchanan, J. J., & Dean, N. (2014). Consistently modeling the same movement strategy is more important than model skill level in observational learning contexts. Acta Psychologica, 146, 19–27.CrossRefPubMed Buchanan, J. J., & Dean, N. (2014). Consistently modeling the same movement strategy is more important than model skill level in observational learning contexts. Acta Psychologica, 146, 19–27.CrossRefPubMed
go back to reference Buchanan, J. J., & Kelso, J. A. S. (1993). Posturally induced transitions in rhythmic multijoint limb movements. Experimental Brain Research, 94, 131–142.CrossRefPubMed Buchanan, J. J., & Kelso, J. A. S. (1993). Posturally induced transitions in rhythmic multijoint limb movements. Experimental Brain Research, 94, 131–142.CrossRefPubMed
go back to reference Buchanan, J. J., Park, J. H., Ryu, Y. U., & Shea, C. H. (2003). Discrete and cyclical units of action in a mixed target pair aiming task. Experimental Brain Research, 150, 473–489.CrossRefPubMed Buchanan, J. J., Park, J. H., Ryu, Y. U., & Shea, C. H. (2003). Discrete and cyclical units of action in a mixed target pair aiming task. Experimental Brain Research, 150, 473–489.CrossRefPubMed
go back to reference Buchanan, J. J., Park, J. H., & Shea, C. H. (2006). Target width scaling in a repetitive aiming task: Switching between cyclical and discrete units of action. Experimental Brain Research, 175, 710–725.CrossRefPubMed Buchanan, J. J., Park, J. H., & Shea, C. H. (2006). Target width scaling in a repetitive aiming task: Switching between cyclical and discrete units of action. Experimental Brain Research, 175, 710–725.CrossRefPubMed
go back to reference Buchanan, J. J., Ramos, J., & Robson, N. (2015). The perception–action dynamics of action competency are altered by both physical and observational training. Experimental Brain Research, 233, 1289–1305. doi:10.1007/s00221-015-4207-y.CrossRefPubMed Buchanan, J. J., Ramos, J., & Robson, N. (2015). The perception–action dynamics of action competency are altered by both physical and observational training. Experimental Brain Research, 233, 1289–1305. doi:10.​1007/​s00221-015-4207-y.CrossRefPubMed
go back to reference Buchanan, J. J., Ryu, Y. U., Zihlman, K., & Wright, D. A. (2008). Observational practice of a relative phase pattern but not an amplitude ratio in a multijoint task. Experimental Brain Research, 191, 157–169.CrossRefPubMed Buchanan, J. J., Ryu, Y. U., Zihlman, K., & Wright, D. A. (2008). Observational practice of a relative phase pattern but not an amplitude ratio in a multijoint task. Experimental Brain Research, 191, 157–169.CrossRefPubMed
go back to reference Buchanan, J. J., & Wright, D. L. (2011). Generalization of action knowledge following observational learning. Acta Psychologica, 136, 167–178.CrossRefPubMed Buchanan, J. J., & Wright, D. L. (2011). Generalization of action knowledge following observational learning. Acta Psychologica, 136, 167–178.CrossRefPubMed
go back to reference Byblow, W. D., Carson, R. G., & Goodman, D. (1994). Expressions of asymmetries and anchoring in bimanual coordination. Human Movement Science, 13, 3–28.CrossRef Byblow, W. D., Carson, R. G., & Goodman, D. (1994). Expressions of asymmetries and anchoring in bimanual coordination. Human Movement Science, 13, 3–28.CrossRef
go back to reference Cross, E. S., Kraemer, D. J. M., Hamilton, A. F. D., Kelley, W. M., & Grafton, S. T. (2009). Sensitivity of the action observation network to physical and observational learning. Cerebral Cortex, 19, 315–326.CrossRefPubMed Cross, E. S., Kraemer, D. J. M., Hamilton, A. F. D., Kelley, W. M., & Grafton, S. T. (2009). Sensitivity of the action observation network to physical and observational learning. Cerebral Cortex, 19, 315–326.CrossRefPubMed
go back to reference Cutting, J. E., & Proffitt, D. R. (1982). The minimum principle and the perception of absolute, common, and relative motions. Cognitive Psychology, 14, 211–246.CrossRefPubMed Cutting, J. E., & Proffitt, D. R. (1982). The minimum principle and the perception of absolute, common, and relative motions. Cognitive Psychology, 14, 211–246.CrossRefPubMed
go back to reference Dounskaia, N. V. (2005). The internal model and the leading joint hypothesis: Implications for control of multi-joint movements. Experimental Brain Research, 166, 1–16.CrossRefPubMed Dounskaia, N. V. (2005). The internal model and the leading joint hypothesis: Implications for control of multi-joint movements. Experimental Brain Research, 166, 1–16.CrossRefPubMed
go back to reference Ellenbuerger, T., Boutin, A., Blandin, Y., Shea, C. H., & Panzer, S. (2012). Scheduling observational and physical practice: Influence on the coding of simple motor sequences. Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology, 65(7), 1260–1273. doi:10.1080/17470218.2011.654126.CrossRef Ellenbuerger, T., Boutin, A., Blandin, Y., Shea, C. H., & Panzer, S. (2012). Scheduling observational and physical practice: Influence on the coding of simple motor sequences. Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology, 65(7), 1260–1273. doi:10.​1080/​17470218.​2011.​654126.CrossRef
go back to reference Fink, P. W., Foo, P., Jirsa, V. K., & Kelso, J. A. S. (2000). Local and global stabilization of coordination by sensory information. Experimental Brain Research, 134, 12.CrossRef Fink, P. W., Foo, P., Jirsa, V. K., & Kelso, J. A. S. (2000). Local and global stabilization of coordination by sensory information. Experimental Brain Research, 134, 12.CrossRef
go back to reference Franceschini, M., Ceravolo, M. G., Agosti, M., Cavallini, P., Bonassi, S., Dall’Armi, V., & Sale, P. (2012). Clinical relevance of action observation in upper-limb stroke rehabilitation: A possible role in recovery of functional dexterity. A randomized clinical trial. Neurorehabilitation and Neural Repair, 26(5), 456–462. doi:10.1177/1545968311427406.CrossRefPubMed Franceschini, M., Ceravolo, M. G., Agosti, M., Cavallini, P., Bonassi, S., Dall’Armi, V., & Sale, P. (2012). Clinical relevance of action observation in upper-limb stroke rehabilitation: A possible role in recovery of functional dexterity. A randomized clinical trial. Neurorehabilitation and Neural Repair, 26(5), 456–462. doi:10.​1177/​1545968311427406​.CrossRefPubMed
go back to reference Gibson, J. J. (1979). The ecological approach to visual perception. Boston, MA: Houghton Mifflin. Gibson, J. J. (1979). The ecological approach to visual perception. Boston, MA: Houghton Mifflin.
go back to reference Guiard, Y. (1993). On Fitts’s and Hooke’s laws: Simple harmonic movement in upper-limb cyclical aiming. Acta Psychologica, 82, 139–159.CrossRefPubMed Guiard, Y. (1993). On Fitts’s and Hooke’s laws: Simple harmonic movement in upper-limb cyclical aiming. Acta Psychologica, 82, 139–159.CrossRefPubMed
go back to reference Guiard, Y. (1997). Fitts’ law in the discrete vs. cyclical paradigm. Human Movement Science, 16(1), 97–131.CrossRef Guiard, Y. (1997). Fitts’ law in the discrete vs. cyclical paradigm. Human Movement Science, 16(1), 97–131.CrossRef
go back to reference Hamilton, A., & Grafton, S. T. (2007). The motor hierarchy: From kinematics to goals and intentions. In P. Haggard, Y. Rossetti, & M. Kawato (Eds.), Sensorimotor foundations of higher cognition: Attention and performance XXll (pp. 381–408). Oxford: Oxford University Press. Hamilton, A., & Grafton, S. T. (2007). The motor hierarchy: From kinematics to goals and intentions. In P. Haggard, Y. Rossetti, & M. Kawato (Eds.), Sensorimotor foundations of higher cognition: Attention and performance XXll (pp. 381–408). Oxford: Oxford University Press.
go back to reference Hayes, S. J., Hodges, N. J., Huys, R., & Williams, A. M. (2007). End-point focus manipulations to determine what information is used during observational learning. Acta Psychologica, 126, 120–137.CrossRefPubMed Hayes, S. J., Hodges, N. J., Huys, R., & Williams, A. M. (2007). End-point focus manipulations to determine what information is used during observational learning. Acta Psychologica, 126, 120–137.CrossRefPubMed
go back to reference Hodges, N. J., Williams, A. M., Hayes, S. J., & Breslin, G. (2007). What is modelled during observational learning? Journal of Sports Sciences, 25(5), 531–545.CrossRefPubMed Hodges, N. J., Williams, A. M., Hayes, S. J., & Breslin, G. (2007). What is modelled during observational learning? Journal of Sports Sciences, 25(5), 531–545.CrossRefPubMed
go back to reference Johansson, G. (1973). Visual perception of biological motion and a model for its analysis. Perception and Psychophysics, 14, 201–211.CrossRef Johansson, G. (1973). Visual perception of biological motion and a model for its analysis. Perception and Psychophysics, 14, 201–211.CrossRef
go back to reference Kelso, J. A. S. (1994). The informational character of self-organized coordination dynamics. Human Movement Science, 13, 393–413.CrossRef Kelso, J. A. S. (1994). The informational character of self-organized coordination dynamics. Human Movement Science, 13, 393–413.CrossRef
go back to reference Kelso, J. A. S., Buchanan, J. J., & Wallace, S. A. (1991). Order parameters for the neural organization of single limb, multijoint movement patterns. Experimental Brain Research, 85, 432–445.CrossRefPubMed Kelso, J. A. S., Buchanan, J. J., & Wallace, S. A. (1991). Order parameters for the neural organization of single limb, multijoint movement patterns. Experimental Brain Research, 85, 432–445.CrossRefPubMed
go back to reference Kelso, J. A. S., & Pandya, A. S. (1991). Dynamic pattern generation and recognition. In N. I. Badler, B. A. Barsky, & D. Zeltzer (Eds.), Making them move: Mechanics, control, and animation of articulated figures (pp. 171–190). San Mateo, CA: Morgan Kaufmann. Kelso, J. A. S., & Pandya, A. S. (1991). Dynamic pattern generation and recognition. In N. I. Badler, B. A. Barsky, & D. Zeltzer (Eds.), Making them move: Mechanics, control, and animation of articulated figures (pp. 171–190). San Mateo, CA: Morgan Kaufmann.
go back to reference Kovacs, A. J., Buchanan, J. J., & Shea, C. H. (2008). Perceptual influences on Fitts’ law. Experimental Brain Research, 190, 99–103.CrossRefPubMed Kovacs, A. J., Buchanan, J. J., & Shea, C. H. (2008). Perceptual influences on Fitts’ law. Experimental Brain Research, 190, 99–103.CrossRefPubMed
go back to reference Kovacs, A. J., Buchanan, J. J., & Shea, C. H. (2009). Bimanual 1:1 with 90° degrees continuous relative phase: Difficult or easy! Experimental Brain Research, 193(1), 129–136. doi:10.1007/s00221-008-1676-2. Kovacs, A. J., Buchanan, J. J., & Shea, C. H. (2009). Bimanual 1:1 with 90° degrees continuous relative phase: Difficult or easy! Experimental Brain Research, 193(1), 129–136. doi:10.​1007/​s00221-008-1676-2.
go back to reference Kovacs, A. J., Buchanan, J. J., & Shea, C. H. (2010). Perceptual and attentional influences on continuous 2:1 and 3:2 multi-frequency bimanual coordination. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 36(4), 936–954.PubMed Kovacs, A. J., Buchanan, J. J., & Shea, C. H. (2010). Perceptual and attentional influences on continuous 2:1 and 3:2 multi-frequency bimanual coordination. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 36(4), 936–954.PubMed
go back to reference Martens, R., Burwitz, L., & Zuckerman, J. (1976). Modeling effects on motor performance. The Research Quarterly, 47(2), 277–291.PubMed Martens, R., Burwitz, L., & Zuckerman, J. (1976). Modeling effects on motor performance. The Research Quarterly, 47(2), 277–291.PubMed
go back to reference Osman, M., Bird, G., & Heyes, C. (2005). Action observation supports effector-dependent learning of finger movement sequences. Experimental Brain Research, 165, 19–27.CrossRefPubMed Osman, M., Bird, G., & Heyes, C. (2005). Action observation supports effector-dependent learning of finger movement sequences. Experimental Brain Research, 165, 19–27.CrossRefPubMed
go back to reference Pinto, J., & Shiffrar, M. (1999). Subconfigurations of the human form in the perception of biological motion displays. Acta Psychologica, 102, 293–318.CrossRefPubMed Pinto, J., & Shiffrar, M. (1999). Subconfigurations of the human form in the perception of biological motion displays. Acta Psychologica, 102, 293–318.CrossRefPubMed
go back to reference Scholz, J. P., & Kelso, J. A. S. (1989). A quantitative approach to understanding the formation and change of coordinated movement patterns. Journal of Motor Behavior, 21(2), 122–144.CrossRefPubMed Scholz, J. P., & Kelso, J. A. S. (1989). A quantitative approach to understanding the formation and change of coordinated movement patterns. Journal of Motor Behavior, 21(2), 122–144.CrossRefPubMed
go back to reference Scully, D. M. (1986). Visual perception of technical execution and aesthetic quality in biological motion. Human Movement Science, 5, 185–206.CrossRef Scully, D. M. (1986). Visual perception of technical execution and aesthetic quality in biological motion. Human Movement Science, 5, 185–206.CrossRef
go back to reference Scully, D. M., & Carnegie, E. (1998). Observational learning in motor skill acquisition: A look at demonstrations. The Irish Journal of Psychology, 19(4), 472–485.CrossRef Scully, D. M., & Carnegie, E. (1998). Observational learning in motor skill acquisition: A look at demonstrations. The Irish Journal of Psychology, 19(4), 472–485.CrossRef
go back to reference Scully, D. M., & Newell, K. M. (1985). Observational learning and the acquisition of motor skills: Toward a visual perception perspective. Journal of Human Movement Studies, 11, 169–186. Scully, D. M., & Newell, K. M. (1985). Observational learning and the acquisition of motor skills: Toward a visual perception perspective. Journal of Human Movement Studies, 11, 169–186.
go back to reference Shea, C. H., Wright, D. L., Wulf, G., & Whitacre, C. (2000). Physical and observational practice afford unique learning opportunities. Journal of Motor Behavior, 32(1), 10.CrossRef Shea, C. H., Wright, D. L., Wulf, G., & Whitacre, C. (2000). Physical and observational practice afford unique learning opportunities. Journal of Motor Behavior, 32(1), 10.CrossRef
go back to reference Shiffrar, M., Lichtey, L., & Chatterjee, S. H. (1997). The perception of biological motion across apertures. Perception and Psychophysics, 59(1), 51–59.CrossRefPubMed Shiffrar, M., Lichtey, L., & Chatterjee, S. H. (1997). The perception of biological motion across apertures. Perception and Psychophysics, 59(1), 51–59.CrossRefPubMed
go back to reference Wilson, A. D., Collins, D. R., & Bingham, G. P. (2005). Human movement coordination implicates relative direction as the information for relative phase. Experimental Brain Research, 165(3), 351–361.CrossRefPubMed Wilson, A. D., Collins, D. R., & Bingham, G. P. (2005). Human movement coordination implicates relative direction as the information for relative phase. Experimental Brain Research, 165(3), 351–361.CrossRefPubMed
go back to reference Zaal, F. T. J. M., Bingham, G. P., & Schmidt, R. C. (2000). Visual perception of mean relative phase and phase variability. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 26(3), 1209–1220.PubMed Zaal, F. T. J. M., Bingham, G. P., & Schmidt, R. C. (2000). Visual perception of mean relative phase and phase variability. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 26(3), 1209–1220.PubMed
go back to reference Zanone, P. G., & Kelso, J. A. S. (1992). The evolution of behavioral attractors with learning: Nonequilibrium phase transitions. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 18, 403–421.PubMed Zanone, P. G., & Kelso, J. A. S. (1992). The evolution of behavioral attractors with learning: Nonequilibrium phase transitions. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 18, 403–421.PubMed
go back to reference Zanone, P. G., & Kelso, J. A. S. (1997). Coordination dynamics of learning and transfer: Collective and component levels. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 23(5), 1454–1480.PubMed Zanone, P. G., & Kelso, J. A. S. (1997). Coordination dynamics of learning and transfer: Collective and component levels. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 23(5), 1454–1480.PubMed
Metagegevens
Titel
Observation and physical practice: different practice contexts lead to similar outcomes for the acquisition of kinematic information
Auteurs
John J. Buchanan
Inchon Park
Publicatiedatum
19-11-2015
Uitgeverij
Springer Berlin Heidelberg
Gepubliceerd in
Psychological Research / Uitgave 1/2017
Print ISSN: 0340-0727
Elektronisch ISSN: 1430-2772
DOI
https://doi.org/10.1007/s00426-015-0723-4

Andere artikelen Uitgave 1/2017

Psychological Research 1/2017 Naar de uitgave