Skip to main content
Top
Gepubliceerd in:

19-11-2015 | Original Article

Action induction due to visual perception of linear motion in depth

Auteurs: Claudia Classen, Armin Kibele

Gepubliceerd in: Psychological Research | Uitgave 1/2017

Log in om toegang te krijgen
share
DELEN

Deel dit onderdeel of sectie (kopieer de link)

  • Optie A:
    Klik op de rechtermuisknop op de link en selecteer de optie “linkadres kopiëren”
  • Optie B:
    Deel de link per e-mail

Abstract

Visually perceived motion can affect observers’ motor control in such a way that an intended action can be activated automatically when it contains similar spatial features. So far, effects have been mostly demonstrated with simple displays where objects were moving in a two-dimensional plane. However, almost all actions we perform and visually perceive in everyday life are much more complex and take place in three-dimensional space. The purpose of this study was to examine action inductions due to visual perception of motion in depth. Therefore, we conducted two Simon experiments where subjects were presented with video displays of a sphere (simple displays, experiment 1) and a real person (complex displays, experiment 2) moving in depth. In both experiments, motion direction towards and away from the observer served as task irrelevant information whereas a color change in the video served as relevant information to choose the correct response (close or far positioned response key). The results show that subjects reacted faster when motion direction of the dynamic stimulus was corresponding to the spatial position of the demanded response. In conclusion, this direction-based Simon effect is modulated by spatial position information, higher sensitivity of our visual system for looming objects, and a high salience of objects being on a collision course.
Voetnoten
1
Colors were chosen for the same reasons as described in Classen and Kibele (2015).
 
2
With this experimental condition we wanted to examine a small question alongside that is not of further interest for the main hypotheses of our study. For this reason we will outline the results of this condition but will subsequently not address it any further.
 
3
Note: all participants used their right index finger to press the proximal key and their left index finger to press the distal key. To rule out impacts of handedness in this experiment, we replicated experiment 1 with changed index finger to response key mapping (n = 5). The results were similar and allowed us to assume that handedness is no factor of influence here.
 
4
One might argue that the stimulus is becoming smaller from the close to the far position and that a size effect is responsible for the different magnitudes of the Simon effects. It is well known that stimulus size affects simple reaction time in such a way that reaction times are shorter to larger as to smaller stimuli. Nevertheless, this only holds for stimuli smaller than 0.5° (e.g. Froeberg, 1907; O’Donell, Barraza, & Colombo, 2010). The stimulus size at the far position corresponds to a visual angle of 1.2° (with a viewing distance of 70 cm). Therefore, reaction times should not be affected by stimulus size here.
 
5
In an experiment of our own conducted in our lab, we tested choice reaction times with different colors on different backgrounds and found that there was no difference between the color magenta (RGB 255, 0, 255) and green (RGB 0, 255, 0) on a gray background (RGB 128, 128, 128) (Hofmann, 2012).
 
Literatuur
go back to reference Bortz, J. (1999). Statistik: Für Sozialwissenschaftler [Statistics: For Social Scientists] (5th ed.). Berlin: Springer. Bortz, J. (1999). Statistik: Für Sozialwissenschaftler [Statistics: For Social Scientists] (5th ed.). Berlin: Springer.
go back to reference Bosbach, S., Prinz, W., & Kerzel, D. (2005). Is direction position? Position- and direction-based correspondence effects in tasks with moving stimuli. The Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology Section A, 58(3), 467–506. doi:10.1080/02724980443000016.CrossRef Bosbach, S., Prinz, W., & Kerzel, D. (2005). Is direction position? Position- and direction-based correspondence effects in tasks with moving stimuli. The Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology Section A, 58(3), 467–506. doi:10.​1080/​0272498044300001​6.CrossRef
go back to reference Box, G. E. P. (1954). Some theorems on quadratic forms applied in the study of analysis of variance problems, II. Effects of inequality of variance and of correlation between errors in the two-way classification. The Annals of Mathematical Statistics, 25(3), 484–498. doi:10.1214/aoms/1177728717.CrossRef Box, G. E. P. (1954). Some theorems on quadratic forms applied in the study of analysis of variance problems, II. Effects of inequality of variance and of correlation between errors in the two-way classification. The Annals of Mathematical Statistics, 25(3), 484–498. doi:10.​1214/​aoms/​1177728717.CrossRef
go back to reference De Jong, R., Liang, C.-C., & Lauber, E. (1994). Conditional and unconditional automaticity: a dual-process model of effects of spatial stimulus-response correspondence. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 20(4), 731–750. doi:10.1037/0096-1523.20.4.731.PubMed De Jong, R., Liang, C.-C., & Lauber, E. (1994). Conditional and unconditional automaticity: a dual-process model of effects of spatial stimulus-response correspondence. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 20(4), 731–750. doi:10.​1037/​0096-1523.​20.​4.​731.PubMed
go back to reference Froeberg, S. (1907). The relation between the magnitude of stimulus and the time of reaction. Archives of Psychology, 16(8), 1–38. Froeberg, S. (1907). The relation between the magnitude of stimulus and the time of reaction. Archives of Psychology, 16(8), 1–38.
go back to reference Geisser, S., & Greenhouse, S. W. (1958). An extension of Box’s results on the use of the F-distribution in multivariate analysis. The Annals of Mathematical Statistics, 29(3), 885–891. doi:10.1214/aoms/1177706545.CrossRef Geisser, S., & Greenhouse, S. W. (1958). An extension of Box’s results on the use of the F-distribution in multivariate analysis. The Annals of Mathematical Statistics, 29(3), 885–891. doi:10.​1214/​aoms/​1177706545.CrossRef
go back to reference Hays, W. L. (1994). Statistics (5th ed.). Fort Worth: Harcourt Brace College Publishers. Hays, W. L. (1994). Statistics (5th ed.). Fort Worth: Harcourt Brace College Publishers.
go back to reference Hofmann, J. (2012). Der Einfluss von Farbe auf die Reaktionszeit im Wettkampfsport: eine empirische Grundlagenuntersuchung zur Frage danach, ob der Vorteil bestimmter Kleiderfarben im Wettkampfsport auf farbabhängige Reaktionszeiten zurückgeführt werden kann [Impact of color on reaction time in competitive sports: A basic examination about the question whether the advantage of special colors of sportswear is due to color dependent reaction times]. Unpublished master‘s thesis. University of Kassel. Hofmann, J. (2012). Der Einfluss von Farbe auf die Reaktionszeit im Wettkampfsport: eine empirische Grundlagenuntersuchung zur Frage danach, ob der Vorteil bestimmter Kleiderfarben im Wettkampfsport auf farbabhängige Reaktionszeiten zurückgeführt werden kann [Impact of color on reaction time in competitive sports: A basic examination about the question whether the advantage of special colors of sportswear is due to color dependent reaction times]. Unpublished master‘s thesis. University of Kassel.
go back to reference Michaels, C. F. (1988). S-R compatibility between response position and destination of apparent motion: evidence of the detection of affordances. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 14(2), 231–240. doi:10.1037/0096-1523.14.2.231.PubMed Michaels, C. F. (1988). S-R compatibility between response position and destination of apparent motion: evidence of the detection of affordances. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 14(2), 231–240. doi:10.​1037/​0096-1523.​14.​2.​231.PubMed
go back to reference Prinz, W. (1990). A Common Coding Approach to Perception and Action. In W. Prinz, P. Bieri, & O. Newmann (Eds.), Relationships Between Perception and Action. Current Approaches. Berlin: Springer. Prinz, W. (1990). A Common Coding Approach to Perception and Action. In W. Prinz, P. Bieri, & O. Newmann (Eds.), Relationships Between Perception and Action. Current Approaches. Berlin: Springer.
go back to reference Proctor, R. W., Van Zandt, T., Lu, C.-H., & Weeks, D. J. (1993). Stimulus-response compatibility for moving stimuli: perception of affordances or directional coding? Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 19(1), 81–91. doi:10.1037/0096-1523.19.1.81.PubMed Proctor, R. W., Van Zandt, T., Lu, C.-H., & Weeks, D. J. (1993). Stimulus-response compatibility for moving stimuli: perception of affordances or directional coding? Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 19(1), 81–91. doi:10.​1037/​0096-1523.​19.​1.​81.PubMed
go back to reference Skarratt, P., Cole, G., & Gellatly, A. H. (2009). Prioritization of looming and receding objects: equal slopes, different intercepts. Attention, Perception, & Psychophysics, 71(4), 964–970. doi:10.3758/APP.71.4.964.CrossRef Skarratt, P., Cole, G., & Gellatly, A. H. (2009). Prioritization of looming and receding objects: equal slopes, different intercepts. Attention, Perception, & Psychophysics, 71(4), 964–970. doi:10.​3758/​APP.​71.​4.​964.CrossRef
go back to reference von Mühlenen, A., & Lleras, A. (2007). No-onset looming motion guides spatial attention. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 33(6), 1297–1310. doi:10.1037/0096-1523.33.6.1297. von Mühlenen, A., & Lleras, A. (2007). No-onset looming motion guides spatial attention. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 33(6), 1297–1310. doi:10.​1037/​0096-1523.​33.​6.​1297.
Metagegevens
Titel
Action induction due to visual perception of linear motion in depth
Auteurs
Claudia Classen
Armin Kibele
Publicatiedatum
19-11-2015
Uitgeverij
Springer Berlin Heidelberg
Gepubliceerd in
Psychological Research / Uitgave 1/2017
Print ISSN: 0340-0727
Elektronisch ISSN: 1430-2772
DOI
https://doi.org/10.1007/s00426-015-0724-3