Skip to main content
Top
Gepubliceerd in: Psychological Research 1/2017

12-01-2016 | Original Article

Surface and structural effects of pitch and time on global melodic expectancies

Auteurs: Jon B. Prince, Leong-Min Loo

Gepubliceerd in: Psychological Research | Uitgave 1/2017

Log in om toegang te krijgen
share
DELEN

Deel dit onderdeel of sectie (kopieer de link)

  • Optie A:
    Klik op de rechtermuisknop op de link en selecteer de optie “linkadres kopiëren”
  • Optie B:
    Deel de link per e-mail

Abstract

We investigated how the surface and structural information of pitch and time in melodies contribute to the perceived expectancy of melodic segments. The contour (pitch surface), tonality (pitch structure), rhythm (time surface) and metre (time structure) were preserved or altered in factorial fashion, either for the full length of a melody (Full condition) or only its last phrase (Last condition). Participants (N = 24) with a range of musical training received instructions to rate how expected the second portion of a melody was, having heard its first part. Additionally, instructions varied across blocks to attend selectively to pitch, time, or both. Expectancy ratings for the Last condition were lower than for the Full condition, indicating that ratings truly reflected expectancy (rather than overall goodness, which would predict the opposite). Interestingly, tonality and rhythm contributed to global expectancy ratings, but not contour or metre. Furthermore, listeners were unable to ignore entirely either dimension, but successfully attenuated their influence in accordance with instructions. These findings offer a unique insight into music perception by testing expectancies of melody segments (beyond single-note continuations), factorially varying both the surface and structure of pitch and time, and using a selective attention manipulation.
Voetnoten
1
We use these operationalisations to express clearly the within-dimension differences (e.g., contour vs. tonality) and across-dimension similarities (e.g., tonality vs. metre). Thus they are relative classifications and we do not claim they are definitive or prescriptive.
 
2
The pitch manipulations are presented in a different order than Prince (2014a)—in that article, the second level was labelled “atonal original contour”, and the third was “contour-violated” (but tonal). The same label reversal applies to the time manipulations.
 
3
All eta-squared values are full eta-squared, not partial.
 
Literatuur
go back to reference Barnes, R., & Jones, M. R. (2000). Expectancy, attention, and time. Cognitive Psychology, 41, 254–311.CrossRefPubMed Barnes, R., & Jones, M. R. (2000). Expectancy, attention, and time. Cognitive Psychology, 41, 254–311.CrossRefPubMed
go back to reference Bartlett, J. C., & Dowling, W. J. (1988). Scale structure and similarity of melodies. Music Perception, 5, 285–314.CrossRef Bartlett, J. C., & Dowling, W. J. (1988). Scale structure and similarity of melodies. Music Perception, 5, 285–314.CrossRef
go back to reference Bharucha, J. J., & Stoeckig, K. (1986). Reaction time and musical expectancy: priming of chords. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 12, 403–410.PubMed Bharucha, J. J., & Stoeckig, K. (1986). Reaction time and musical expectancy: priming of chords. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 12, 403–410.PubMed
go back to reference Bigand, E. (1997). Perceiving musical stability: the effect of tonal structure, rhythm, and musical expertise. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 23, 808–822.PubMed Bigand, E. (1997). Perceiving musical stability: the effect of tonal structure, rhythm, and musical expertise. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 23, 808–822.PubMed
go back to reference Bigand, E., Madurell, F., Tillmann, B., & Pineau, M. (1999). Effect of global structure and temporal organization on chord processing. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 25, 184–197. Bigand, E., Madurell, F., Tillmann, B., & Pineau, M. (1999). Effect of global structure and temporal organization on chord processing. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 25, 184–197.
go back to reference Bigand, E., & Parncutt, R. (1999). Perceiving musical tension in long chord sequences. Psychological Research/Psychologische Forschung, 62, 237–254.CrossRefPubMed Bigand, E., & Parncutt, R. (1999). Perceiving musical tension in long chord sequences. Psychological Research/Psychologische Forschung, 62, 237–254.CrossRefPubMed
go back to reference Bigand, E., & Pineau, M. (1997). Global context effects on musical expectancy. Perception & Psychophysics, 59, 1098–1107.CrossRef Bigand, E., & Pineau, M. (1997). Global context effects on musical expectancy. Perception & Psychophysics, 59, 1098–1107.CrossRef
go back to reference Bigand, E., Poulin, B., Tillmann, B., Madurell, F., & D’Adamo, D. A. (2003). Sensory versus cognitive components in harmonic priming. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 29, 159–171.PubMed Bigand, E., Poulin, B., Tillmann, B., Madurell, F., & D’Adamo, D. A. (2003). Sensory versus cognitive components in harmonic priming. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 29, 159–171.PubMed
go back to reference Bigand, E., & Poulin-Charronnat, B. (2006). Are we “experienced listeners”? A review of the musical capacities that do not depend on formal musical training. Cognition, 100, 100–130.CrossRefPubMed Bigand, E., & Poulin-Charronnat, B. (2006). Are we “experienced listeners”? A review of the musical capacities that do not depend on formal musical training. Cognition, 100, 100–130.CrossRefPubMed
go back to reference Bolger, D., Coull, J. T., & Schön, D. (2014). Metrical rhythm implicitly orients attention in time as indexed by improved target detection and left inferior parietal activation. Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience, 26, 593–605. doi:10.1162/jocn_a_00511 CrossRefPubMed Bolger, D., Coull, J. T., & Schön, D. (2014). Metrical rhythm implicitly orients attention in time as indexed by improved target detection and left inferior parietal activation. Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience, 26, 593–605. doi:10.​1162/​jocn_​a_​00511 CrossRefPubMed
go back to reference Boltz, M. G. (1989a). Perceiving the end: effects of tonal relationships on melodic completion. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 15, 749–761.PubMed Boltz, M. G. (1989a). Perceiving the end: effects of tonal relationships on melodic completion. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 15, 749–761.PubMed
go back to reference Boltz, M. G. (1989b). Rhythm and good endings - effects of temporal structure on tonality judgments. Perception & Psychophysics, 46, 9–17.CrossRef Boltz, M. G. (1989b). Rhythm and good endings - effects of temporal structure on tonality judgments. Perception & Psychophysics, 46, 9–17.CrossRef
go back to reference Boltz, M. G. (1989c). Time judgments of musical endings - effects of expectancies on the filled interval effect. Perception & Psychophysics, 46, 409–418.CrossRef Boltz, M. G. (1989c). Time judgments of musical endings - effects of expectancies on the filled interval effect. Perception & Psychophysics, 46, 409–418.CrossRef
go back to reference Boltz, M. G. (1993). The generation of temporal and melodic expectancies during musical listening. Perception & Psychophysics, 53, 585–600.CrossRef Boltz, M. G. (1993). The generation of temporal and melodic expectancies during musical listening. Perception & Psychophysics, 53, 585–600.CrossRef
go back to reference Cambouropoulos, E. (2010). The musical surface: Challenging basic assumptions. Musicae Scientiae, 131-147. Cambouropoulos, E. (2010). The musical surface: Challenging basic assumptions. Musicae Scientiae, 131-147.
go back to reference Collins, T., Tillmann, B., Barrett, F. S., Delbé, C., & Janata, P. (2014). A combined model of sensory and cognitive representations underlying tonal expectations in music: from audio signals to behavior. Psychological Review, 121, 33–65. doi:10.1037/a0034695 CrossRefPubMed Collins, T., Tillmann, B., Barrett, F. S., Delbé, C., & Janata, P. (2014). A combined model of sensory and cognitive representations underlying tonal expectations in music: from audio signals to behavior. Psychological Review, 121, 33–65. doi:10.​1037/​a0034695 CrossRefPubMed
go back to reference Cuddy, L. L., & Cohen, A. J. (1976). Recognition of transposed melodic sequences. Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology, 28, 255–270.CrossRef Cuddy, L. L., & Cohen, A. J. (1976). Recognition of transposed melodic sequences. Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology, 28, 255–270.CrossRef
go back to reference Cuddy, L. L., Cohen, A. J., & Miller, J. (1979). Melody recognition: the experimental application of musical rules. Canadian Journal of Psychology-Revue Canadienne De Psychologie, 33, 148–157.CrossRef Cuddy, L. L., Cohen, A. J., & Miller, J. (1979). Melody recognition: the experimental application of musical rules. Canadian Journal of Psychology-Revue Canadienne De Psychologie, 33, 148–157.CrossRef
go back to reference Cuddy, L. L., & Lunney, C. A. (1995). Expectancies generated by melodic intervals - perceptual judgments of melodic continuity. Perception & Psychophysics, 57, 451–462. doi:10.3758/bf03213071 CrossRef Cuddy, L. L., & Lunney, C. A. (1995). Expectancies generated by melodic intervals - perceptual judgments of melodic continuity. Perception & Psychophysics, 57, 451–462. doi:10.​3758/​bf03213071 CrossRef
go back to reference Deutsch, D. (Ed.). (2013). The psychology of music (3rd ed.): Academic Press. Deutsch, D. (Ed.). (2013). The psychology of music (3rd ed.): Academic Press.
go back to reference Dewitt, L. A., & Samuel, A. G. (1990). The role of knowledge-based expectations in music perception - evidence from musical restoration. Journal of Experimental Psychology: General, 119, 123–144.CrossRef Dewitt, L. A., & Samuel, A. G. (1990). The role of knowledge-based expectations in music perception - evidence from musical restoration. Journal of Experimental Psychology: General, 119, 123–144.CrossRef
go back to reference Dowling, W. J. (1973). Rhythmic groups and subjective chunks in memory for melodies. Perception & Psychophysics, 14, 37–40.CrossRef Dowling, W. J. (1973). Rhythmic groups and subjective chunks in memory for melodies. Perception & Psychophysics, 14, 37–40.CrossRef
go back to reference Dowling, W. J. (1978). Scale and contour: two components of a theory of memory for melodies. Psychological Review, 85, 341–354.CrossRef Dowling, W. J. (1978). Scale and contour: two components of a theory of memory for melodies. Psychological Review, 85, 341–354.CrossRef
go back to reference Dowling, W. J., & Fujitani, D. S. (1971). Contour, interval, and pitch recognition in memory for melodies. Journal of the Acoustical Society of America, 49, 524–531.CrossRef Dowling, W. J., & Fujitani, D. S. (1971). Contour, interval, and pitch recognition in memory for melodies. Journal of the Acoustical Society of America, 49, 524–531.CrossRef
go back to reference Escoffier, N., Sheng, D. Y. J., & Schirmer, A. (2010). Unattended musical beats enhance visual processing. Acta Psychologica, 135, 12–16.CrossRefPubMed Escoffier, N., Sheng, D. Y. J., & Schirmer, A. (2010). Unattended musical beats enhance visual processing. Acta Psychologica, 135, 12–16.CrossRefPubMed
go back to reference Farbood, M. M. (2012). A parametric, temporal model of musical tension. Music Perception, 29, 387–428.CrossRef Farbood, M. M. (2012). A parametric, temporal model of musical tension. Music Perception, 29, 387–428.CrossRef
go back to reference Handel, S. (1998). The interplay between metric and figural rhythmic organization. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 24, 1546–1561. Handel, S. (1998). The interplay between metric and figural rhythmic organization. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 24, 1546–1561.
go back to reference Hébert, S., & Cuddy, L. L. (2002). Detection of metric structure in auditory figural patterns. Perception & Psychophysics, 64, 909–918.CrossRef Hébert, S., & Cuddy, L. L. (2002). Detection of metric structure in auditory figural patterns. Perception & Psychophysics, 64, 909–918.CrossRef
go back to reference Huron, D. (2006). Sweet anticipation: Music and the psychology of expectation. Cambridge: MIT Press. Huron, D. (2006). Sweet anticipation: Music and the psychology of expectation. Cambridge: MIT Press.
go back to reference Huron, D., & Margulis, E. H. (2010). Musical expectancy and thrills Handbook of music and emotion: Theory, research, applications. (pp. 575–604): Oxford University Press, New York, NY. Huron, D., & Margulis, E. H. (2010). Musical expectancy and thrills Handbook of music and emotion: Theory, research, applications. (pp. 575–604): Oxford University Press, New York, NY.
go back to reference Jones, M. R. (1976). Time, our lost dimension - toward a new theory of perception, attention, and memory. Psychological Review, 83, 323–355.CrossRefPubMed Jones, M. R. (1976). Time, our lost dimension - toward a new theory of perception, attention, and memory. Psychological Review, 83, 323–355.CrossRefPubMed
go back to reference Jones, M. R. (1987). Dynamic pattern structure in music - recent theory and research. Perception & Psychophysics, 41, 621–634.CrossRef Jones, M. R. (1987). Dynamic pattern structure in music - recent theory and research. Perception & Psychophysics, 41, 621–634.CrossRef
go back to reference Jones, M. R., & Boltz, M. G. (1989). Dynamic attending and responses to time. Psychological Review, 96, 459–491.CrossRefPubMed Jones, M. R., & Boltz, M. G. (1989). Dynamic attending and responses to time. Psychological Review, 96, 459–491.CrossRefPubMed
go back to reference Jones, M. R., Boltz, M. G., & Klein, J. M. (1993). Expected endings and judged duration. Memory & Cognition, 21, 646–665.CrossRef Jones, M. R., Boltz, M. G., & Klein, J. M. (1993). Expected endings and judged duration. Memory & Cognition, 21, 646–665.CrossRef
go back to reference Jones, M. R., Moynihan, H., MacKenzie, N., & Puente, J. (2002). Temporal aspects of stimulus-driven attending in dynamic arrays. Psychological Science, 13, 313–319.CrossRefPubMed Jones, M. R., Moynihan, H., MacKenzie, N., & Puente, J. (2002). Temporal aspects of stimulus-driven attending in dynamic arrays. Psychological Science, 13, 313–319.CrossRefPubMed
go back to reference Jones, M. R., & Ralston, J. T. (1991). Some influences of accent structure on melody recognition. Memory & Cognition, 19, 8–20.CrossRef Jones, M. R., & Ralston, J. T. (1991). Some influences of accent structure on melody recognition. Memory & Cognition, 19, 8–20.CrossRef
go back to reference Jones, M. R., Summerell, L., & Marshburn, E. (1987). Recognizing melodies - a dynamic interpretation. Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology. A, Human Experimental Psychology, 39, 89–121. Jones, M. R., Summerell, L., & Marshburn, E. (1987). Recognizing melodies - a dynamic interpretation. Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology. A, Human Experimental Psychology, 39, 89–121.
go back to reference Kidd, G., Boltz, M. G., & Jones, M. R. (1984). Some effects of rhythmic context on melody recognition. American Journal of Psychology, 97, 153–173.CrossRefPubMed Kidd, G., Boltz, M. G., & Jones, M. R. (1984). Some effects of rhythmic context on melody recognition. American Journal of Psychology, 97, 153–173.CrossRefPubMed
go back to reference Krumhansl, C. L. (1990). Cognitive foundations of musical pitch. New York, NY: Oxford University Press. Krumhansl, C. L. (1990). Cognitive foundations of musical pitch. New York, NY: Oxford University Press.
go back to reference Krumhansl, C. L. (1997). Effects of perceptual organization and musical form on melodic expectancies. In M. Leman (Ed.), Music, Gestalt, and computing (Vol. 1317, pp. 294-320): Springer Berlin Heidelberg. doi:10.1007/BFb0034122 Krumhansl, C. L. (1997). Effects of perceptual organization and musical form on melodic expectancies. In M. Leman (Ed.), Music, Gestalt, and computing (Vol. 1317, pp. 294-320): Springer Berlin Heidelberg. doi:10.​1007/​BFb0034122
go back to reference Krumhansl, C. L., & Cuddy, L. L. (2010). A theory of tonal hierarchies in music. In M. R. Jones, R. R. Fay & A. N. Popper (Eds.), Music perception. Springer handbook of auditory research (pp. 51-87). New York, NY: Springer Science. doi:10.1007/978-1-4419-6114-3_1 Krumhansl, C. L., & Cuddy, L. L. (2010). A theory of tonal hierarchies in music. In M. R. Jones, R. R. Fay & A. N. Popper (Eds.), Music perception. Springer handbook of auditory research (pp. 51-87). New York, NY: Springer Science. doi:10.​1007/​978-1-4419-6114-3_​1
go back to reference Krumhansl, C. L., Louhivuori, J., Toiviainen, P., Jarvinen, T., & Eerola, T. (1999). Melodic expectation in finnish spiritual folk hymns: convergence of statistical, behavioral, and computational approaches. Music Perception, 17, 151–195.CrossRef Krumhansl, C. L., Louhivuori, J., Toiviainen, P., Jarvinen, T., & Eerola, T. (1999). Melodic expectation in finnish spiritual folk hymns: convergence of statistical, behavioral, and computational approaches. Music Perception, 17, 151–195.CrossRef
go back to reference Krumhansl, C. L., & Schmuckler, M. A. (1986, July). Key-finding in music: An algorithm based on pattern matching to tonal hierarchies. Paper presented at the 19th Annual Meeting of the Society of Mathematical Psychology, Cambridge, MA. Krumhansl, C. L., & Schmuckler, M. A. (1986, July). Key-finding in music: An algorithm based on pattern matching to tonal hierarchies. Paper presented at the 19th Annual Meeting of the Society of Mathematical Psychology, Cambridge, MA.
go back to reference Lamont, A., & Dibben, N. (2001). Motivic structure and the perception of similarity. Music Perception, 18, 245–274.CrossRef Lamont, A., & Dibben, N. (2001). Motivic structure and the perception of similarity. Music Perception, 18, 245–274.CrossRef
go back to reference Large, E. W., & Jones, M. R. (1999). The dynamics of attending: how people track time-varying events. Psychological Review, 106, 119–159.CrossRef Large, E. W., & Jones, M. R. (1999). The dynamics of attending: how people track time-varying events. Psychological Review, 106, 119–159.CrossRef
go back to reference Large, E. W., & Palmer, C. (2002). Perceiving temporal regularity in music. Cognitive Science, 26, 1–37.CrossRef Large, E. W., & Palmer, C. (2002). Perceiving temporal regularity in music. Cognitive Science, 26, 1–37.CrossRef
go back to reference Lerdahl, F., & Jackendoff, R. (1983). A generative theory of tonal music. Cambridge, Massachusetts: MIT Press. Lerdahl, F., & Jackendoff, R. (1983). A generative theory of tonal music. Cambridge, Massachusetts: MIT Press.
go back to reference London, J. (2004). Hearing in time: Psychological aspects of musical meter. Oxford: Oxford University Press.CrossRef London, J. (2004). Hearing in time: Psychological aspects of musical meter. Oxford: Oxford University Press.CrossRef
go back to reference Marmel, F., Tillmann, B., & Delbe, C. (2010). Priming in melody perception: tracking down the strength of cognitive expectations. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 36, 1016–1028. doi:10.1037/a0018735 PubMed Marmel, F., Tillmann, B., & Delbe, C. (2010). Priming in melody perception: tracking down the strength of cognitive expectations. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 36, 1016–1028. doi:10.​1037/​a0018735 PubMed
go back to reference Mathworks, (2004). Matlab. Natick, MA: The Mathworks Inc. Mathworks, (2004). Matlab. Natick, MA: The Mathworks Inc.
go back to reference Matsunaga, R., & Abe, J. I. (2005). Cues for key perception of a melody: pitch set alone? Music Perception, 23, 153–164.CrossRef Matsunaga, R., & Abe, J. I. (2005). Cues for key perception of a melody: pitch set alone? Music Perception, 23, 153–164.CrossRef
go back to reference Meyer, L. B. (1956). Emotion and meaning in music. Chicago: University of Chicago Press. Meyer, L. B. (1956). Emotion and meaning in music. Chicago: University of Chicago Press.
go back to reference Narmour, E. (1990). The analysis and cognition of basic melodic structures: The implication-realization model. Chicago, IL, US: University of Chicago Press. Narmour, E. (1990). The analysis and cognition of basic melodic structures: The implication-realization model. Chicago, IL, US: University of Chicago Press.
go back to reference Narmour, E. (1992). The analysis and cognition of melodic complexity: The implication-realization model: University of Chicago Press. Narmour, E. (1992). The analysis and cognition of melodic complexity: The implication-realization model: University of Chicago Press.
go back to reference Palmer, C., & Krumhansl, C. L. (1987a). Independent temporal and pitch structures in determination of musical phrases. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 13, 116–126.PubMed Palmer, C., & Krumhansl, C. L. (1987a). Independent temporal and pitch structures in determination of musical phrases. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 13, 116–126.PubMed
go back to reference Palmer, C., & Krumhansl, C. L. (1987b). Pitch and temporal contributions to musical phrase perception - effects of harmony, performance timing, and familiarity. Perception & Psychophysics, 41, 505–518.CrossRef Palmer, C., & Krumhansl, C. L. (1987b). Pitch and temporal contributions to musical phrase perception - effects of harmony, performance timing, and familiarity. Perception & Psychophysics, 41, 505–518.CrossRef
go back to reference Pearce, M. T., & Wiggins, G. A. (2006). Expectation in melody: the influence of context and learning. Music Perception, 23, 377–405.CrossRef Pearce, M. T., & Wiggins, G. A. (2006). Expectation in melody: the influence of context and learning. Music Perception, 23, 377–405.CrossRef
go back to reference Pfordresher, P. Q. (2008). Auditory feedback in music performance: the role of transition-based similarity. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 34, 708–725.PubMed Pfordresher, P. Q. (2008). Auditory feedback in music performance: the role of transition-based similarity. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 34, 708–725.PubMed
go back to reference Povel, D. J. (1981). Internal representation of simple temporal patterns. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 7, 3–18.PubMed Povel, D. J. (1981). Internal representation of simple temporal patterns. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 7, 3–18.PubMed
go back to reference Povel, D. J., & Essens, P. (1985). Perception of temporal patterns. Music Perception, 2, 411–440.CrossRef Povel, D. J., & Essens, P. (1985). Perception of temporal patterns. Music Perception, 2, 411–440.CrossRef
go back to reference Prince, J. B. (2014a). Contributions of pitch contour, tonality, rhythm, and meter to melodic similarity. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 40, 2319–2337. doi:10.1037/a0038010 PubMed Prince, J. B. (2014a). Contributions of pitch contour, tonality, rhythm, and meter to melodic similarity. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 40, 2319–2337. doi:10.​1037/​a0038010 PubMed
go back to reference Prince, J. B. (2014b). Pitch structure, but not selective attention, affects accent weightings in metrical grouping. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 40, 2073–2090. doi:10.1037/a0037730 PubMed Prince, J. B. (2014b). Pitch structure, but not selective attention, affects accent weightings in metrical grouping. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 40, 2073–2090. doi:10.​1037/​a0037730 PubMed
go back to reference Repp, B. H. (1992). Probing the cognitive representation of musical time - structural constraints on the perception of timing perturbations. Cognition, 44, 241–281.CrossRefPubMed Repp, B. H. (1992). Probing the cognitive representation of musical time - structural constraints on the perception of timing perturbations. Cognition, 44, 241–281.CrossRefPubMed
go back to reference Repp, B. H. (1995). Detectability of duration and intensity increments in melody tones - a partial connection between music perception and performance. Perception & Psychophysics, 57, 1217–1232.CrossRef Repp, B. H. (1995). Detectability of duration and intensity increments in melody tones - a partial connection between music perception and performance. Perception & Psychophysics, 57, 1217–1232.CrossRef
go back to reference Scharf, B., Quigley, S., Aoki, C., Peachey, N., & Reeves, A. (1987). Focused auditory attention and frequency-selectivity. Perception & Psychophysics, 42, 215–223. doi:10.3758/bf03203073 CrossRef Scharf, B., Quigley, S., Aoki, C., Peachey, N., & Reeves, A. (1987). Focused auditory attention and frequency-selectivity. Perception & Psychophysics, 42, 215–223. doi:10.​3758/​bf03203073 CrossRef
go back to reference Schellenberg, E. G. (1996). Expectancy in melody: tests of the implication realization model. Cognition, 58, 75–125.CrossRefPubMed Schellenberg, E. G. (1996). Expectancy in melody: tests of the implication realization model. Cognition, 58, 75–125.CrossRefPubMed
go back to reference Schellenberg, E. G. (1997). Simplifying the implication-realization model of melodic expectancy. Music Perception, 14, 295–318.CrossRef Schellenberg, E. G. (1997). Simplifying the implication-realization model of melodic expectancy. Music Perception, 14, 295–318.CrossRef
go back to reference Schellenberg, E. G. (2001). Asymmetries in the discrimination of musical intervals: going out-of-tune is more noticeable than going in-tune. Music Perception, 19, 223–248.CrossRef Schellenberg, E. G. (2001). Asymmetries in the discrimination of musical intervals: going out-of-tune is more noticeable than going in-tune. Music Perception, 19, 223–248.CrossRef
go back to reference Schmuckler, M. A. (1989). Expectation in music - investigation of melodic and harmonic processes. Music Perception, 7, 109–150.CrossRef Schmuckler, M. A. (1989). Expectation in music - investigation of melodic and harmonic processes. Music Perception, 7, 109–150.CrossRef
go back to reference Schmuckler, M. A. (1990). The performance of global expectations. Psychomusicology Special Edition: Music expectancy, 9, 122–147.CrossRef Schmuckler, M. A. (1990). The performance of global expectations. Psychomusicology Special Edition: Music expectancy, 9, 122–147.CrossRef
go back to reference Schmuckler, M. A. (1997). Expectancy effects in memory for melodies. Canadian Journal of Experimental Psychology, 51, 292–305.CrossRefPubMed Schmuckler, M. A. (1997). Expectancy effects in memory for melodies. Canadian Journal of Experimental Psychology, 51, 292–305.CrossRefPubMed
go back to reference Schmuckler, M. A. (2009). Components of melodic processing. In S. Hallam, I. Cross, & M. Thaut (Eds.), Oxford handbook of music psychology (pp. 93–106). Oxford, UK: Oxford University Press. Schmuckler, M. A. (2009). Components of melodic processing. In S. Hallam, I. Cross, & M. Thaut (Eds.), Oxford handbook of music psychology (pp. 93–106). Oxford, UK: Oxford University Press.
go back to reference Schmuckler, M. A., & Boltz, M. G. (1994). Harmonic and rhythmic influences on musical expectancy. Perception & Psychophysics, 56, 313–325.CrossRef Schmuckler, M. A., & Boltz, M. G. (1994). Harmonic and rhythmic influences on musical expectancy. Perception & Psychophysics, 56, 313–325.CrossRef
go back to reference Schubert, E., & Stevens, C. (2006). The effect of implied harmony, contour and musical expertise on judgments of similarity of familiar melodies. Journal of New Music Research, 35, 161–174. doi:10.1080/09298210600835000 CrossRef Schubert, E., & Stevens, C. (2006). The effect of implied harmony, contour and musical expertise on judgments of similarity of familiar melodies. Journal of New Music Research, 35, 161–174. doi:10.​1080/​0929821060083500​0 CrossRef
go back to reference Schulkind, M. D. (1999). Long-term memory for temporal structure: evidence from the identification of well-known and novel songs. Memory & Cognition, 27, 896–906.CrossRef Schulkind, M. D. (1999). Long-term memory for temporal structure: evidence from the identification of well-known and novel songs. Memory & Cognition, 27, 896–906.CrossRef
go back to reference Serafine, M. L., Glassman, N., & Overbeeke, C. (1989). The cognitive reality of hierarchic structure in music. Music Perception, 6, 397–430.CrossRef Serafine, M. L., Glassman, N., & Overbeeke, C. (1989). The cognitive reality of hierarchic structure in music. Music Perception, 6, 397–430.CrossRef
go back to reference Stalinski, S. M., & Schellenberg, E. G. (2010). Shifting perceptions: developmental changes in judgments of melodic similarity. Developmental Psychology, 46, 1799–1803.CrossRefPubMed Stalinski, S. M., & Schellenberg, E. G. (2010). Shifting perceptions: developmental changes in judgments of melodic similarity. Developmental Psychology, 46, 1799–1803.CrossRefPubMed
go back to reference Steinbeis, N., Koelsch, S., & Sloboda, J. A. (2006). The role of harmonic expectancy violations in musical emotions: evidence from subjective, physiological, and neural responses. Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience, 18, 1380–1393.CrossRefPubMed Steinbeis, N., Koelsch, S., & Sloboda, J. A. (2006). The role of harmonic expectancy violations in musical emotions: evidence from subjective, physiological, and neural responses. Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience, 18, 1380–1393.CrossRefPubMed
go back to reference Temperley, D., & Marvin, E. W. (2008). Pitch-class distribution and the identification of key. Music Perception, 25, 193–212.CrossRef Temperley, D., & Marvin, E. W. (2008). Pitch-class distribution and the identification of key. Music Perception, 25, 193–212.CrossRef
go back to reference Thompson, W. F., Cuddy, L. L., & Plaus, C. (1997). Expectancies generated by melodic intervals: evaluation of principles of melodic implication in a melody-completion task. Perception & Psychophysics, 59, 1069–1076. doi:10.3758/bf03205521 CrossRef Thompson, W. F., Cuddy, L. L., & Plaus, C. (1997). Expectancies generated by melodic intervals: evaluation of principles of melodic implication in a melody-completion task. Perception & Psychophysics, 59, 1069–1076. doi:10.​3758/​bf03205521 CrossRef
go back to reference Tillmann, B., & Lebrun-Guillaud, G. (2006). Influence of tonal and temporal expectations on chord processing and on completion judgments of chord sequences. Psychological Research, 70, 345–358.CrossRefPubMed Tillmann, B., & Lebrun-Guillaud, G. (2006). Influence of tonal and temporal expectations on chord processing and on completion judgments of chord sequences. Psychological Research, 70, 345–358.CrossRefPubMed
go back to reference Tillmann, B., & Marmel, F. (2013). Musical expectations within chord sequences: facilitation due to tonal stability without closure effects. Psychomusicology: Music, Mind, and Brain, 23, 1–5.CrossRef Tillmann, B., & Marmel, F. (2013). Musical expectations within chord sequences: facilitation due to tonal stability without closure effects. Psychomusicology: Music, Mind, and Brain, 23, 1–5.CrossRef
go back to reference Tillmann, B., Poulin-Charronnat, B., & Bigand, E. (2014). The role of expectation in music: from the score to emotions and the brain. Wiley Interdisciplinary Reviews: Cognitive Science, 5, 105–113.PubMed Tillmann, B., Poulin-Charronnat, B., & Bigand, E. (2014). The role of expectation in music: from the score to emotions and the brain. Wiley Interdisciplinary Reviews: Cognitive Science, 5, 105–113.PubMed
go back to reference Zurbriggen, E. L., Fontenot, D. L., & Meyer, D. E. (2006). Representation and execution of vocal motor programs for expert singing of tonal melodies. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 32, 944–963. doi:10.1037/0096-1523.32.4.944 PubMed Zurbriggen, E. L., Fontenot, D. L., & Meyer, D. E. (2006). Representation and execution of vocal motor programs for expert singing of tonal melodies. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 32, 944–963. doi:10.​1037/​0096-1523.​32.​4.​944 PubMed
Metagegevens
Titel
Surface and structural effects of pitch and time on global melodic expectancies
Auteurs
Jon B. Prince
Leong-Min Loo
Publicatiedatum
12-01-2016
Uitgeverij
Springer Berlin Heidelberg
Gepubliceerd in
Psychological Research / Uitgave 1/2017
Print ISSN: 0340-0727
Elektronisch ISSN: 1430-2772
DOI
https://doi.org/10.1007/s00426-015-0737-y

Andere artikelen Uitgave 1/2017

Psychological Research 1/2017 Naar de uitgave