Skip to main content
Log in

Is there an internal association of numbers to hands? The task set influences the nature of the SNARC effect

  • Published:
Memory & Cognition Aims and scope Submit manuscript

Abstract

The SNARCeffect refers to the association of smaller numbers with the left and of larger numbers with the right side of extracorporal space (Dehaene, Bossini, & Giraux, 1993). We tested the assumption that, in addition to these associations, numbers are also related to participants’ hands. We report two experiments with vertically arranged buttons in which the nature of the SNARC effect depended on whether the task set was button or hand related: In the first case, a vertical location-related SNARC effect occurred, whereas in the second a handrelated SNARC effect was found. Our third experiment confirmed that space-related number representations dominate the SNARC effect when the buttons are arranged horizontally. We concluded that both effector- and space-related number representations can influence and modify the SNARC effect.

This is a preview of subscription content, log in via an institution to check access.

Access this article

Price excludes VAT (USA)
Tax calculation will be finalised during checkout.

Instant access to the full article PDF.

Similar content being viewed by others

References

  • Bächtold, D., Baumüller, M., &Brugger, P. (1998). Stimulus-response compatibility in representational space.Neuropsychologia,36, 731–735.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Bauer, D. W., &Miller, J. (1982). Stimulus-response compatibility and the motor system.Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology,34A, 367–380.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Berch, D. B., Foley, E. J., Hill, R. J., &Ryan, P. M. (1999). Extracting parity and magnitude from Arabic numerals: Developmental changes in number processing and mental representation.Journal of Experimental Child Psychology,74, 286–308.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Berlucchi, G., Crea, F., di Stefano, M., &Tassinari, G. (1977). Influence of spatial stimulus-response compatibility on reaction time of ipsilateral and contralateral hand to lateralized light stimuli.Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception & Performance,3, 505–517.

    Google Scholar 

  • Bishop, Y. M. M., Fienberg, S. E., &Holland, P. W. (1975).Discrete multivariate analysis: Theory and practice. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Boroditsky, L. (2001). Does language shape thought? Mandarin and English speakers’ conceptions of time.Cognitive Psychology,43, 1–22.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Brannon, E. M. (2002). The development of ordinal numerical knowledge in infancy.Cognition,83, 223–240.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Brannon, E. M., &Terrace, H. S. (1998). Ordering of the numerosities 1 to 9 by monkeys.Science,282, 746–749.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Brannon, E. M., Wusthoff, C. J., Gallistel, C. R., &Gibbon, J. (2001). Numerical subtraction in the pigeon: Evidence for a linear subjective number scale.Psychological Science,12, 238–243.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Butterworth, B. (1999).The mathematical brain. London: Macmillan.

    Google Scholar 

  • Dehaene, S. (1997).The number sense: How the mind creates mathematics. New York: Oxford University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Dehaene, S., Bossini, S., &Giraux, P. (1993). The mental representation of parity and number magnitude.Journal of Experimental Psychology: General,122, 371–396.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Ehrenstein, W. H., Schroeder-Heister, P., &Heister, G. (1989). Spatial S-R compatibility with orthogonal stimulus-response relationship.Perception & Psychophysics,45, 215–220.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Fias, W. (2001). Two routes for the processing of verbal numbers: Evidence from the SNARC effect.Psychological Research,65, 250–259.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Fias, W., Brysbaert, M., Geypens, F., &d’Ydewalle, G. (1996). The importance of magnitude information in numerical processing: Evidence from the SNARC effect.Mathematical Cognition,2, 95–110.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Fias, W., Lauwereyns, J., &Lammertyn, J. (2001). Irrelevant digits affect feature-based attention depending on the overlap of neural circuits.Cognitive Brain Research,12, 415–423.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Fischer, M. H. (2001). Number processing induces spatial performance biases.Neurology,57, 822–826.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Fischer, M. H. (2003). Spatial representations in number processing-Evidence from a pointing task.Visual Cognition,10, 493–508.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Fischer, M. H., Dewulf, N., &Hill, R. L. (2005). Designing bar graphs: Orientation matters.Applied Cognitive Psychology,19, 953–962.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Fischer, M. H., Warlop, N., Hill, R. L., &Fias, W. (2004). Oculomotor bias induced by number perception.Experimental Psychology,51, 91–97.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Fitts, P. M., &Seeger, C. M. (1953). S-R compatibility: Spatial characteristics of stimulus and response codes.Journal of Experimental Psychology,46, 199–210.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Gallistel, C. R., &Gelman, R. (1992). Preverbal and verbal counting and computation.Cognition,44, 43–74.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Gallistel, C. R., &Gelman, R. (2000). Non-verbal numerical cognition: From reals to integers.Trends in Cognitive Sciences,4, 59–65.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Gevers, W., &Lammertyn, J. (2005). The hunt for SNARC.Psychology Science,47, 10–21. [See also the entire special issue: H.-C. Nuerk,K. Willmes, & W. Fias (Eds.) (2005). Brain and number.Psychology Science,47(1). Free access at www.pabst-publishers.de/psychologyscience/ 1-2005/index.html.]

    Google Scholar 

  • Gevers, W., Reynvoet, B., &Fias, W. (2003). The mental representation of ordinal sequences is spatially organized.Cognition,87, B87-B95.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Heister, G., Schroeder-Heister, P., &Ehrenstein, W. H. (1990). Spatial coding and spatio-anatomical mapping: Evidence for a hierarchical model of spatial stimulus-response compatibility. In R. W. Proctor & T. G. Reeve (Eds.),Stimulus-response compatibility: An integrated perspective (pp. 117–143). Amsterdam: North-Holland.

    Google Scholar 

  • Hines, T. M. (1990). An odd effect: Lengthened reaction times for judgments about odd digits. Memory & Cognition,18, 40–46.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Hubbard, E. M., Piazza, M., Pinel, P., &Dehaene, S. (2005). Interactions between number and space in parietal cortex.Nature Reviews Neuroscience,6, 435–448.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Ito, Y., &Hatta, T. (2004). Spatial structure of quantitative representation of numbers: Evidence from the SNARC effect.Memory & Cognition,32, 662–673.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Keus, I. M., &Schwarz, W. (2005). Searching for the functional locus of the SNARC effect: Evidence for a response-related origin.Memory & Cognition,33, 681–695.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Lorch, R. F., Jr., &Myers, J. L. (1990). Regression analyses of repeated measures data in cognitive research.Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, & Cognition,16, 149–157.

    Google Scholar 

  • Maass, A., &Russo, A. (2003). Directional bias in the mental representation of spatial events: Nature or culture?Psychological Science,14, 296–301.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Moyer, R. S., &Landauer, T. K. (1967). Time required for judgements of numerical inequality.Nature,215, 1519–1520.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Nicoletti, R., Umiltà, C., &Làdavas, E. (1984). Compatibility due to the coding of the relative position of the effectors.Acta Psychologica,57, 133–143.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Nuerk, H.-C., Iversen, W., &Willmes, K. (2004). Notational modulation of the SNARC and the MARC (linguistic markedness of response codes) effect.Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology,57A, 835–863.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Piazza, M., &Dehaene, S. (2004). From number neurons to mental arithmetic: The cognitive neuroscience of number sense. In M. S. Gazzaniga (Ed.),The cognitive neurosciences (3rd ed., pp. 865–875). Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Proctor, R. W., &Reeve, T. G. (1990).Stimulus-response compatibility: An integrated perspective. Amsterdam: North-Holland.

    Google Scholar 

  • Restle, F. (1970). Speed of adding and comparing numbers.Journal of Experimental Psychology,83, 274–278.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Riggio, L., Gawryszewski, L. G., &Umiltà, C. (1986). What is crossed in crossed-hand effects?Acta Psychologica,62, 89–100.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Rubinsten, O., Henik, A., Berger, A., &Shahar-Shalev, S. (2002). The development of internal representations of magnitude and their association with Arabic numerals.Journal of Experimental Child Psychology,81, 74–92.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Schwarz, W., &Ischebeck, A. (2003). On the relative speed account of number-size interference in comparative judgments of numerals.Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception & Performance,29, 507–522.

    Google Scholar 

  • Schwarz, W., &Keus, I. M. (2004). Moving the eyes along the mental number line: Comparing SNARC effects with saccadic and manual responses.Perception & Psychophysics,66, 651–664.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Schwarz, W., &Müller, D. (2006). Spatial associations in numberrelated tasks: A comparison of manual and pedal responses.Experimental Psychology,53, 4–15.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Shepard, R. N., Kilpatric, D. W., &Cunningham, J. P. (1975). The internal representation of numbers.Cognitive Psychology,7, 82–138.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Temple, E., &Posner, M. I. (1998). Brain mechanisms of quantity are similar in 5-year-old children and adults.Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences,95, 7836–7841.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Turconi, E., Campbell, J. I. D., &Seron, X. (2006). Numerical order and quantity processing in number comparison.Cognition,98, 273–285.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Tversky, B., Kugelmass, S., &Winter, A. (1991). Cross-cultural and developmental trends in graphic productions.Cognitive Psychology,23, 515–557.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Umiltà, C., &Nicoletti, R. (1990). Spatial stimulus-response compatibility. In R. W. Proctor & T. G. Reeve (Eds.),Stimulus-response compatibility: An integrated perspective (pp. 89–116). Amsterdam: North-Holland.

    Google Scholar 

  • Vu, K.-P. L., Proctor, R. W., &Pick, D. F. (2000). Vertical versus horizontal spatial incompatibility: Right-left prevalence with bimanual responses.Psychological Research,64, 25–40.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Vuilleumier, P., Ortigue, S., &Brugger, P. (2004). The number space and neglect.Cortex,40, 399–410.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Zebian, S. (2005). Linkages between number concepts, spatial thinking, and directionality of writing: The SNARC effect and the REVERSE SNARC effect in English and Arabic monoliterates, biliterates, and illiterate Arabic speakers.Journal of Cognition & Culture,5, 165–190.

    Article  Google Scholar 

Download references

Author information

Authors and Affiliations

Authors

Corresponding author

Correspondence to Wolf Schwarz.

Rights and permissions

Reprints and permissions

About this article

Cite this article

Müller, D., Schwarz, W. Is there an internal association of numbers to hands? The task set influences the nature of the SNARC effect. Memory & Cognition 35, 1151–1161 (2007). https://doi.org/10.3758/BF03193485

Download citation

  • Received:

  • Accepted:

  • Published:

  • Issue Date:

  • DOI: https://doi.org/10.3758/BF03193485

Keywords

Navigation