Introduction
Autism spectrum disorder (ASD) is a group of highly heritable and severely impairing neurodevelopmental disorders, characterized by impairments in social interaction and communication, and restricted, stereotyped and repetitive behaviour (American Psychological Association
2013). ASD is the most heritable of all complex neuropsychiatric conditions, with heritability estimates ranging up to 90 % (Lichtenstein et al.
2010). ASD is marked by substantial heterogeneity in symptom presentation, developmental course and etiologic mechanisms (Jones and Klin
2009). The genetics of ASD is complex with involvement of both rare and common genetic variants. Rare genetic variants predisposing to ASD are currently thought to account for 10–20 % of all ASD cases (Betancur
2011). They include rare mutations in genes which lead to monogenic disorders that are frequently associated with ASD, such as fragile X syndrome and tuberous sclerosis, as well as mutations and copy number variations (CNVs, these constitute deletions or duplications of larger fragments of DNA often involving several genes) that may contribute to (mono- and) oligogenic forms of ASD (Betancur
2011 Persico and Napolioni
2013). Common variants, e.g. single nucleotide polymorphism (SNPs), implicated in the etiology of ASD, on the other hand, are assumed to each contribute a (very) small increase in disease risk (Wang et al.
2009). As ASD strongly reduces reproductive fitness, it has been argued that part of the genetic contribution to ASD is due to
de novo mutations (Neale et al.
2012; D’Onofrio et al.
2014). In addition to the strong genetic background, environmental influences, gene × environment interaction, epigenetic factors, and pre-perinatal complications also play an important role in susceptibility to ASD (Gardener et al.
2009,
2011; Dietert et al.
2011; Kinney et al.
2010; Wong et al.
2014). Multiple causal pathways may thus underlie the same clinical profiles, and, at the same time, the complex etiology may result in highly heterogeneous clinical profiles.
The heterogeneous character of ASD strongly hinders research into etiology and effective treatment. An approach to parse etiologic heterogeneity is to form more homogeneous subgroups of patients based on the familial occurrence of the disorder. Several studies have reported on the genetic differences between families with only one individual with ASD (the so-called single-incidence or simplex [SPX] families) compared to families with two or more affected individuals (multiple-incidence or multiplex [MPX] families). These studies reported a more than threefold rate of de novo mutations in SPX families (~7–10 %), compared to MPX families (~2–3 %) or control families (~ 1 %) (Sebat et al.
2007; Marshall et al.
2008). In MPX families, shared genetic predispositions based on a multifactorial etiology of common genes appear to play a more important role (Freitag
2007), with members of MPX families more often exhibiting ASD traits compared to members of SPX families (Virkud et al.
2009; Gerdts et al.
2013). We recently replicated the latter finding in the sample described in the current study (Oerlemans et al.
2015). These findings suggest that individuals from SPX families are more likely than individuals from MPX families to develop ASD as a result of sporadic genetic and/or non-genetic causes strictly personal to the patient.
Assuming that SPX-MPX stratification identifies forms of ASD with a different genetic architecture, we aimed to study whether cognitive deficits differ between SPX and MPX families, in probands and/or in unaffected siblings. Previous studies in individuals with ASD have found deficits in intelligence (typically strengths in performance IQ (PIQ) over verbal IQ (VIQ)), social cognition (SC), executive functions (EF) and central coherence (CC) (Joseph et al.
2002; Black et al.
2009; Happe and Ronald
2008; Pellicano
2012). Direct comparisons of cognitive deficits between individuals with SPX and MPX ASD are mostly lacking thus far, and the vast majority of cognitive studies have failed to clearly specify or adjust for simplex or multiplex ascertainment process. So far, studies in SPX ASD-only samples report a higher frequency of performance > verbal IQ discrepancy in cases compared to controls (Ankenman et al.
2014), and an altered cortical shape in brain regions that have been implicated in communication, higher order social processes (e.g. empathy and theory of mind), spatial attention, visual processing and face recognition (Dierker et al.
2013). Studies in MPX ASD-only samples report deficits in EF components such as planning and set-shifting, theory of mind, and fluid and crystallized intelligence (Nydén et al.
2011). To our knowledge, only one study has examined the association of SPX versus MPX status with cognitive functioning. Verbal and non-verbal IQ and head circumference [HC; associated with impaired brain connectivity and higher order abilities (Courchesne and Pierce
2005)] were compared between children and adolescents with autism from SPX and MPX families. The authors reported that enlarged HC was related to social deficits in SPX, but not MPX individuals, and that individuals with the lowest nonverbal IQ scores were mostly classified SPX, whereas individuals with a higher than average nonverbal IQ were mostly MPX (Davis et al.
2013). These findings suggest that both SPX and MPX forms of ASD are associated with a wide range of similar disabilities in higher order cognitive processes, but that some cognitive factors may be uniquely related to either SPX or MPX ASD (e.g. lower IQ scores were reported for SPX ASD), and more research is needed to clarify this issue.
Studies reporting on the presence of ASD-related cognitive deficits in first-degree relatives are sparse and report inconsistent findings (Oerlemans et al.
2014; Wong et al.
2006; Gokcen et al.
2009). A possible explanation for these discrepant findings might be that these studies did not differentiate between etiologically different (inherited versus non-inherited) forms of ASD and thus might have investigated relatives with and without familial loading as a mixed group. A recent study using SPX-MPX stratification to examine executive function of the parents of patients with familial versus non-familial (sporadic) schizophrenia confirmed this idea and reported that executive functions were only impaired in parents with a family history of schizophrenia (Erol et al.
2012). Of interest to us is whether similar patterns can also be found in familial (MPX) versus sporadic (SPX) ASD.
To test whether the cognitive architecture underlying SPX and MPX autism families is different and useful for parsing the etiological heterogeneity of ASD, the cognitive performance of ASD probands and unaffected siblings from SPX and MPX families was compared with each other and with healthy controls. We selected cognitive tasks that assess various cognitive domains previously implicated in ASD (Gokcen et al.
2009; Eapen et al.
2013), or have been described as promising cognitive endophenotypes for ASD in previous literature (Oerlemans et al.
2013; Oerlemans et al.
2014; Rommelse et al.
2011). We hypothesized that potentially different forms of ASD might result in dissimilar cognitive profiles in SPX and MPX ASD probands, a finding with implications for treatment. Further, we hypothesized that the within family contrast between probands and unaffected siblings regarding cognitive aspects of the disorder was larger in SPX compared to MPX families as indicated by (mild) cognitive deficits (similar to their affected brother/sister) compared to controls in unaffected siblings from MPX, but not SPX families, a finding highly relevant to the identification of cognitive endophenotypes for genetic research.
Discussion
The main goal of the current study was to examine whether the cognitive architecture underlying SPX and MPX autism families is different and useful for parsing etiological heterogeneity of ASD. This model of different etiologies in SPX and MPX families is based on evidence from behaviorally-based and genetic research (Sebat et al.
2007; Marshall et al.
2008; Freitag
2007; Virkud et al.
2009; Gerdts et al.
2013). We hypothesized that (a) the different forms of ASD might result in dissimilar cognitive profiles in SPX and MPX ASD probands, and (b) unaffected siblings from MPX but not SPX would display (mild) cognitive deficits compared to controls. Our results showed that directly comparing SPX and MPX ASD cases, no significant differences were detected and both were associated with impairments in VIQ, PIQ, face recognition, affective prosody recognition, and verbal working memory compared to healthy controls. However, when compared to their unaffected siblings, impairments in identification of facial emotions, VIQ, PIQ, and verbal working memory were more pronounced in SPX cases than MPX cases. Unaffected siblings from MPX families had a significantly lower VIQ (similar to their affected brother/sister) compared to controls, whereas SPX unaffected siblings were unimpaired in this domain. Both MPX and SPX unaffected siblings differed significantly from controls on affective prosody and were unimpaired on the other cognitive domains. ASD probands and unaffected siblings from MPX families resembled each other more closely in cognitive functioning than affected-unaffected siblings from SPX families.
Results support the hypothesis that a partly different cognitive architecture may underlie SPX and MPX forms of ASD, which only becomes evident when contrasting cognitive performances within families. That is, the direct comparison between autistic children from SPX and MPX families revealed very similar cognitive problems, but when using unaffected siblings as an ideal reference group (viewed as indexing the ‘full potential’ of children with ASD had they not developed the disorder and correcting for shared environmental factors), SPX probands seem to be more impaired in intelligence, verbal working memory and emotion recognition than MPX probands, which is not explained by a more severe ASD phenotype in SPX probands (i.e., in our sample, SPX and MPX ASD probands demonstrate equally severe ASD traits, see sample characteristics and Oerlemans et al.
2015). This could indicate that partly different developmental pathways may result in a similar phenotype and similar cognitive deficits, a phenomenon that has been referred to in developmental psychopathology as
equifinality (Cicchetti and Rogosch
1996). ASD has often been associated with lower full scale IQ or intellectual disability (ID) (Charman et al.
2011). One model that has been proposed for the overlap between ID and ASD suggests that rare, highly penetrant mutations set the stage for abnormal developmental trajectories including ASD, developmental delay and mental retardation (Eapen
2011). Assuming that SPX ASD is more likely than MPX ASD to develop as a result of such rare (sporadic) genetic causes, our finding that SPX probands seem to be more impaired in intelligence than MPX probands and the finding of Davis et al. (
2013) that ASD children with low(ered) intelligence levels more often had SPX than MPX forms of ASD corroborate this theory.
SPX unaffected siblings were largely unimpaired on cognitive measures compared to controls, except for affective prosody, whereas MPX unaffected siblings were impaired on both affective prosody and VIQ. Several implications may result from this finding. First of all, it suggests that affective prosody is the most sensitive cognitive marker for detecting familial risk for ASD. This finding is in line with previous analyses using the same cognitive task in a younger subsample of this cohort (Oerlemans et al.
2014). The perception of emotional expressions via affective prosody is highly relevant for the development of Theory of Mind (ToM), which refers to the ability to understand other people’s thoughts, beliefs, and other internal states (Korkmaz
2011). Many believe that social cognition deficits are central to explaining the difficulties experienced by people with ASD (Korkmaz
2011; Baron-Cohen
1995). Our finding that unaffected siblings (regardless SPX/MPX status) were impaired on affective prosody, but not on other cognitive domains, might suggest that impaired social cognition is the primary cognitive deficit in ASD, resulting from shared (genetic and/or environmental) risk factors that disrupt the ability to process emotional cues in individuals with autism and (to some extent) their unaffected first-degree relatives. Second, it suggests that the unaffected siblings from SPX families are not completely free from cognitive deficits. The finding is consistent with findings that although
de novo genetic variations most likely play a role in the development of simplex ASD, they do not fully explain genetic etiology (Krumm et al.
2013). In other words, also in SPX ASD families some risks may be shared between family members (Klei et al.
2012), and the distinction between MPX and SPX ASD may reflect variation in the magnitude of effects rather than qualitative differences between groups. Third, only a few comparisons between MPX unaffected siblings and controls reached significance. This finding clearly contrasts with studies in ADHD that firmly demonstrate significant impairments on cognitive functions and brain morphology in first-degree unaffected relatives who are at risk of the disorder (Allen et al.
2009; Rommelse et al.
2011). This does not seem to be due to a simple lack of power: visual inspection of the data indicate no or only very minor cognitive impairments on several domains that are impaired in the MPX probands (face recognition, PIQ, verbal working memory). This suggests that—in contrast to ADHD—cognitive factors in ASD may have a stronger determining effect on the development of the final phenotype. That is, cognitive problems may be part of the defining features of ASD, rather than being an endophenotypic trait that can be seen in unaffected relatives. An important exception is affective prosody, suggesting this domain may be sensitive towards familial risk factors for ASD.
Some limitations to this study need to be acknowledged when interpreting the results. First, sample sizes were moderate; it follows that our study needs replication in larger samples to fully uncover effects. Second, reproductive stoppage is a factor in ASD. Stoppage is the phenomenon in which parents who already have a child affected with ASD may decide to not have more children after symptoms appear and/or an affected child receives the diagnosis (Hoffmann et al.
2014). It is difficult to tell if SPX families would be MPX families if not for stoppage, especially when the proband is severely affected. The issue of stoppage also has implications for recurrence risk estimates and birth order studies in ASD (Wood et al.
2014). Ideally, one would account for stoppage, for example by only examining families with unaffected siblings born before the affected probands or by only including information from the first ASD case in each family. Due to our limited sample size, we were unable to do so, but we believe that insofar this has affected our results, it would likely lead to an underestimation of potential differences between SPX and MPX ASD cases. Of note, the family size of the SPX and MPX families did not differ from each other and were slightly larger than control families and in about half of the SPX families an unaffected sibling (29 male, 7 female) was born before the affected child. Third, boys were overrepresented in both proband groups and in SPX unaffected siblings, but were underrepresented in MPX unaffected siblings and controls. This was likely due to the fact that a) ASD is more frequently diagnosed in males and b) because the presence of male unaffected siblings was only required for SPX, but not MPX families. However, we do not believe that this has affected the results, since the effect of sex was controlled for in this study. Fourth, although effort was made to include several tasks tapping the domains of SC and EF, we were not able to assess all aspects of these cognitive domains. For example, fluency, planning and theory of mind were not assessed here. We cannot rule out the possibility that the cognitive functions not studied here are sensitive to familial effects. Fifth, only Dutch participants of European Caucasian ethnicity were included in our study. This may limit the generalization of our findings to other ethnic groups. Also, by focusing on average and high-functioning ASD, the generalizability of our findings to the broad range of ASD is limited. Future research should consider extending these findings to lower-functioning individuals with ASD—a group that is greatly under-represented in research studies—which may reveal different SPX-MPX ratios and more pronounced impairments on the various cognitive domains. Last, the difficulty with matching non-ASD IQ levels with ASD IQ levels should be discussed. Often, studies match cases and controls on mental age or IQ but given that IQ is inherently confounded with ASD, it cannot be fully separated from the effects of the condition (Jarrold and Brock
2004; Dennis et al.
2009). Matching IQ to controls in children with ASD may create unrepresentative groups, with either the ASD group having higher IQs than the population with the disorder, or the control group having IQs below normal expectations (Dennis et al.
2009). The authors suggest that instead, controlling for sociodemographic characteristics may be desirable. Given that estimated verbal and performance IQ (separately) were outcome measures in our study, we did not match on total IQ, except from the inclusion criterion of IQ ≥ 70 for all participants. Sample characteristics reveal that IQ distributions were highly similar for affected and unaffected siblings and controls (albeit higher mean IQ for controls than affected children) and parental educational levels were largely similar across groups, indicating that the controls may be an adequate comparison group. Differences in age and sex across groups were controlled for in the analyses.
All in all, results suggest that some differences between SPX and MPX forms of ASD exist, which becomes evident when contrasting cognitive performances within families. These findings may help parse etiological heterogeneity of ASD by stratifying ASD families into families with stronger versus weaker familial aggregation of ASD-related cognitive deficits.