Skip to main content
Top
Gepubliceerd in: Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders 2/2016

09-09-2015 | Original Paper

Low Fidelity Imitation of Atypical Biological Kinematics in Autism Spectrum Disorders Is Modulated by Self-Generated Selective Attention

Auteurs: Spencer J. Hayes, Matthew Andrew, Digby Elliott, Emma Gowen, Simon J. Bennett

Gepubliceerd in: Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders | Uitgave 2/2016

Log in om toegang te krijgen
share
DELEN

Deel dit onderdeel of sectie (kopieer de link)

  • Optie A:
    Klik op de rechtermuisknop op de link en selecteer de optie “linkadres kopiëren”
  • Optie B:
    Deel de link per e-mail

Abstract

We examined whether adults with autism had difficulty imitating atypical biological kinematics. To reduce the impact that higher-order processes have on imitation we used a non-human agent model to control social attention, and removed end-state target goals in half of the trials to minimise goal-directed attention. Findings showed that only neurotypical adults imitated atypical biological kinematics. Adults with autism did, however, become significantly more accurate at imitating movement time. This confirmed they engaged in the task, and that sensorimotor adaptation was self-regulated. The attentional bias to movement time suggests the attenuation in imitating kinematics might be a compensatory strategy due to deficits in lower-level visuomotor processes associated with self-other mapping, or selective attention modulated the processes that represent biological kinematics.
Literatuur
go back to reference Bandura, A. (1977). Social learning theory. Prentice-Hall, NJ: Englewood Cliffs. Bandura, A. (1977). Social learning theory. Prentice-Hall, NJ: Englewood Cliffs.
go back to reference Bekkering, H., Wohlschlaeger, A., & Gattis, M. (2000). Imitation of gestures in children is goal-directed. The Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology, 53(1), 153–164. doi:10.1080/713755872.CrossRefPubMed Bekkering, H., Wohlschlaeger, A., & Gattis, M. (2000). Imitation of gestures in children is goal-directed. The Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology, 53(1), 153–164. doi:10.​1080/​713755872.CrossRefPubMed
go back to reference Bird, G., & Heyes, C. (2005). Effector-dependent learning by observation of a finger movement sequence. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 31(2), 262–275.PubMed Bird, G., & Heyes, C. (2005). Effector-dependent learning by observation of a finger movement sequence. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 31(2), 262–275.PubMed
go back to reference Brass, M., Bekkering, H., & Prinz, W. (2001). Movement observation affects movement execution in a simple response task. Acta Psychologica, 106(1–2), 3–22.CrossRefPubMed Brass, M., Bekkering, H., & Prinz, W. (2001). Movement observation affects movement execution in a simple response task. Acta Psychologica, 106(1–2), 3–22.CrossRefPubMed
go back to reference Byrne, R. W., & Russon, A. E. (1998). Learning by imitation: A hierarchical approach. Behavioral and Brain Sciences, 21(5), 667–684.PubMed Byrne, R. W., & Russon, A. E. (1998). Learning by imitation: A hierarchical approach. Behavioral and Brain Sciences, 21(5), 667–684.PubMed
go back to reference Carroll, W. R., & Bandura, A. (1982). The role of visual monitoring in observational learning of action patterns: Making the unobservable observable. Journal of Motor Behavior, 14(2), 153–167.CrossRefPubMed Carroll, W. R., & Bandura, A. (1982). The role of visual monitoring in observational learning of action patterns: Making the unobservable observable. Journal of Motor Behavior, 14(2), 153–167.CrossRefPubMed
go back to reference Chartrand, T. L., & Bargh, J. A. (1999). The chameleon effect: The perception-behavior link and social interaction. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 76(6), 893–910.CrossRefPubMed Chartrand, T. L., & Bargh, J. A. (1999). The chameleon effect: The perception-behavior link and social interaction. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 76(6), 893–910.CrossRefPubMed
go back to reference Cook, J. L., & Bird, G. (2012). Atypical social modulation of imitation in autism spectrum conditions. Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders, 42(6), 1045–1051.PubMedCentralCrossRefPubMed Cook, J. L., & Bird, G. (2012). Atypical social modulation of imitation in autism spectrum conditions. Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders, 42(6), 1045–1051.PubMedCentralCrossRefPubMed
go back to reference Dapretto, M., Davies, M. S., Pfeifer, J. H., Scott, A. A., Sigman, M., Bookheimer, S. Y., et al. (2006). Understanding emotions in others: Mirror neuron dysfunction in children with autism spectrum disorders. Nature Neuroscience, 9(1), 28–30. doi:10.1038/nn1611 Dapretto, M., Davies, M. S., Pfeifer, J. H., Scott, A. A., Sigman, M., Bookheimer, S. Y., et al. (2006). Understanding emotions in others: Mirror neuron dysfunction in children with autism spectrum disorders. Nature Neuroscience, 9(1), 28–30. doi:10.​1038/​nn1611
go back to reference Elliott, D., Hansen, S., Grierson, L. E. M., Lyons, J., Bennett, S. J., & Hayes, S. J. (2010). Goal-directed aiming: Two components but multiple processes. Psychological Bulletin, 136(6), 1023–1044. doi:10.1037/a0020958.CrossRefPubMed Elliott, D., Hansen, S., Grierson, L. E. M., Lyons, J., Bennett, S. J., & Hayes, S. J. (2010). Goal-directed aiming: Two components but multiple processes. Psychological Bulletin, 136(6), 1023–1044. doi:10.​1037/​a0020958.CrossRefPubMed
go back to reference Flash, T., & Hogan, N. (1985). The coordination of arm movements: an experimentally confirmed mathematical model. The Journal of Neuroscience, 5(7), 1688–1703.PubMed Flash, T., & Hogan, N. (1985). The coordination of arm movements: an experimentally confirmed mathematical model. The Journal of Neuroscience, 5(7), 1688–1703.PubMed
go back to reference Gangitano, M., Mottaghy, F. M., & Pascual-Leone, A. (2001). Phase-specific modulation of cortical motor output during movement observation. NeuroReport, 12(7), 1489–1492.CrossRefPubMed Gangitano, M., Mottaghy, F. M., & Pascual-Leone, A. (2001). Phase-specific modulation of cortical motor output during movement observation. NeuroReport, 12(7), 1489–1492.CrossRefPubMed
go back to reference Grossman, E. D., Donnelly, M., Price, R., Pickens, D., Morgan, V., Neighbor, G., & Blake, R. (2000). Brain areas involved in perception of biological motion. Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience, 12(5), 711–720.CrossRefPubMed Grossman, E. D., Donnelly, M., Price, R., Pickens, D., Morgan, V., Neighbor, G., & Blake, R. (2000). Brain areas involved in perception of biological motion. Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience, 12(5), 711–720.CrossRefPubMed
go back to reference Hamilton, A. F. D. C. (2013). Reflecting on the mirror neuron system in autism: A systematic review of current theories. Developmental Cognitive Neuroscience, 3, 91–105.CrossRefPubMed Hamilton, A. F. D. C. (2013). Reflecting on the mirror neuron system in autism: A systematic review of current theories. Developmental Cognitive Neuroscience, 3, 91–105.CrossRefPubMed
go back to reference Hamilton, A. F. D. C., Brindley, R. M., & Frith, U. (2007). Imitation and action understanding in autistic spectrum disorders: How valid is the hypothesis of a deficit in the mirror neuron system? Neuropsychologia, 45(8), 1859–1868.CrossRefPubMed Hamilton, A. F. D. C., Brindley, R. M., & Frith, U. (2007). Imitation and action understanding in autistic spectrum disorders: How valid is the hypothesis of a deficit in the mirror neuron system? Neuropsychologia, 45(8), 1859–1868.CrossRefPubMed
go back to reference Hamilton, A. F. D. C., & Grafton, S. T. (2007). The motor hierarchy: From kinematics to goals and intentions. In P. Haggard, Y. Rosetti, & M. Kawato (Eds.), Sensorimotor foundations of higher cognition: Attention and performance XXII (pp. 381–408). Oxford, UK: Oxford University Press. Hamilton, A. F. D. C., & Grafton, S. T. (2007). The motor hierarchy: From kinematics to goals and intentions. In P. Haggard, Y. Rosetti, & M. Kawato (Eds.), Sensorimotor foundations of higher cognition: Attention and performance XXII (pp. 381–408). Oxford, UK: Oxford University Press.
go back to reference Hayes, S. J., Andrew, M., Elliott, D., Roberts, J. W., & Bennett, S. J. (2012). Dissociable contributions of motor-execution and action-observation to intermanual transfer. Neuroscience Letters, 506(2), 346–350.CrossRefPubMed Hayes, S. J., Andrew, M., Elliott, D., Roberts, J. W., & Bennett, S. J. (2012). Dissociable contributions of motor-execution and action-observation to intermanual transfer. Neuroscience Letters, 506(2), 346–350.CrossRefPubMed
go back to reference Hayes, S. J., Ashford, D., & Bennett, S. J. (2008). Goal-directed imitation: The means to an end. Acta Psychologica, 127(2), 407–415.CrossRefPubMed Hayes, S. J., Ashford, D., & Bennett, S. J. (2008). Goal-directed imitation: The means to an end. Acta Psychologica, 127(2), 407–415.CrossRefPubMed
go back to reference Hayes, S. J., Elliott, D., & Bennett, S. J. (2010). General motor representations are developed during action-observation. Experimental Brain Research, 204, 1–8.CrossRef Hayes, S. J., Elliott, D., & Bennett, S. J. (2010). General motor representations are developed during action-observation. Experimental Brain Research, 204, 1–8.CrossRef
go back to reference Hayes, S. J., Hodges, N. J., Huys, R., & Williams, A. M. (2007). End-point focus manipulations to determine what information is used during observational learning. Acta Psychologica, 126(2), 120–137.CrossRefPubMed Hayes, S. J., Hodges, N. J., Huys, R., & Williams, A. M. (2007). End-point focus manipulations to determine what information is used during observational learning. Acta Psychologica, 126(2), 120–137.CrossRefPubMed
go back to reference Hayes, S. J., Roberts, J. W., Elliott, D., & Bennett, S. J. (2014). Top-down attentional processes modulate the coding of atypical biological motion kinematics in the absence of motor signals. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 40(4), 1641–1653.PubMed Hayes, S. J., Roberts, J. W., Elliott, D., & Bennett, S. J. (2014). Top-down attentional processes modulate the coding of atypical biological motion kinematics in the absence of motor signals. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 40(4), 1641–1653.PubMed
go back to reference Hayes, S. J., Timmis, M. A., & Bennett, S. J. (2009). Eye movements are not a prerequisite for learning movement sequence timing through observation. Acta Psychologica, 131(3), 202–208.CrossRefPubMed Hayes, S. J., Timmis, M. A., & Bennett, S. J. (2009). Eye movements are not a prerequisite for learning movement sequence timing through observation. Acta Psychologica, 131(3), 202–208.CrossRefPubMed
go back to reference Heyes, C. (2001). Causes and consequences of imitation. Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 5(6), 253–261.CrossRefPubMed Heyes, C. (2001). Causes and consequences of imitation. Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 5(6), 253–261.CrossRefPubMed
go back to reference Heyes, C., & Bird, G. (2007). Mirroring, association, and the correspondence problem. In P. Haggard, Y. Rossetti, & M. Kawato (Eds.), Sensorimotor foundations of higher cognition: Attention and performance XX (pp. 461–479). Oxford, England: Oxford University Press. Heyes, C., & Bird, G. (2007). Mirroring, association, and the correspondence problem. In P. Haggard, Y. Rossetti, & M. Kawato (Eds.), Sensorimotor foundations of higher cognition: Attention and performance XX (pp. 461–479). Oxford, England: Oxford University Press.
go back to reference Iacoboni, M., Koski, L. M., Brass, M., Bekkering, H., Woods, R. P., Dubeau, M. C., & Rizzolatti, G. (2001). Reafferent copies of imitated actions in the right superior temporal cortex. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America, 98(24), 13995–13999. doi:10.1073/pnas.241474598.PubMedCentralCrossRefPubMed Iacoboni, M., Koski, L. M., Brass, M., Bekkering, H., Woods, R. P., Dubeau, M. C., & Rizzolatti, G. (2001). Reafferent copies of imitated actions in the right superior temporal cortex. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America, 98(24), 13995–13999. doi:10.​1073/​pnas.​241474598.PubMedCentralCrossRefPubMed
go back to reference Kozlowski, L. T., & Cutting, J. E. (1977). Recognizing the sex of a walker from a dynamic point-light display. Perception and Psychophysics, 21(6), 575–580. doi:10.3758/BF03198740.CrossRef Kozlowski, L. T., & Cutting, J. E. (1977). Recognizing the sex of a walker from a dynamic point-light display. Perception and Psychophysics, 21(6), 575–580. doi:10.​3758/​BF03198740.CrossRef
go back to reference Liepelt, R., & Brass, M. (2010). Automatic imitation of physically impossible movements. Social Cognition, 28(1), 59–73.CrossRef Liepelt, R., & Brass, M. (2010). Automatic imitation of physically impossible movements. Social Cognition, 28(1), 59–73.CrossRef
go back to reference Longo, M. R., Kosobud, A., & Bertenthal, B. I. (2008). Automatic imitation of biomechanically possible and impossible actions: Effects of priming movements versus goals. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 34(2), 489–501. doi:10.1037/0096-1523.34.2.489.PubMed Longo, M. R., Kosobud, A., & Bertenthal, B. I. (2008). Automatic imitation of biomechanically possible and impossible actions: Effects of priming movements versus goals. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 34(2), 489–501. doi:10.​1037/​0096-1523.​34.​2.​489.PubMed
go back to reference Lord, C., Risi, S., Lambrecht, L., Cook, E., Jr, Leventhal, B., DiLavore, P., & Rutter, M. (2000). The autism diagnostic observation schedule—Generic: A standard measure of social and communication deficits associated with the spectrum of autism. Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders, 30(3), 205–223. doi:10.1023/A:1005592401947.CrossRefPubMed Lord, C., Risi, S., Lambrecht, L., Cook, E., Jr, Leventhal, B., DiLavore, P., & Rutter, M. (2000). The autism diagnostic observation schedule—Generic: A standard measure of social and communication deficits associated with the spectrum of autism. Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders, 30(3), 205–223. doi:10.​1023/​A:​1005592401947.CrossRefPubMed
go back to reference Oberman, L. M., Hubbard, E. M., McCleery, J. P., Altschuler, E. L., Ramachandran, V. S., & Pineda, J. A. (2005). EEG evidence for mirror neuron dysfunction in autism spectrum disorders. Cognitive Brain Research, 24(2), 190–198.CrossRefPubMed Oberman, L. M., Hubbard, E. M., McCleery, J. P., Altschuler, E. L., Ramachandran, V. S., & Pineda, J. A. (2005). EEG evidence for mirror neuron dysfunction in autism spectrum disorders. Cognitive Brain Research, 24(2), 190–198.CrossRefPubMed
go back to reference Perra, O., Williams, J. H. G., Whiten, A., Fraser, L., Benzie, H., & Perrett, D. I. (2008). Imitation and ‘theory of mind’ competencies in discrimination of autism from other neurodevelopmental disorders. Research in Autism Spectrum Disorders, 2(3), 456–468. doi:10.1016/j.rasd.2007.09.007.CrossRef Perra, O., Williams, J. H. G., Whiten, A., Fraser, L., Benzie, H., & Perrett, D. I. (2008). Imitation and ‘theory of mind’ competencies in discrimination of autism from other neurodevelopmental disorders. Research in Autism Spectrum Disorders, 2(3), 456–468. doi:10.​1016/​j.​rasd.​2007.​09.​007.CrossRef
go back to reference Prinz, W. (1997). Perception and action planning. European Journal of Cognitive Psychology, 9(2), 129–154.CrossRef Prinz, W. (1997). Perception and action planning. European Journal of Cognitive Psychology, 9(2), 129–154.CrossRef
go back to reference Rizzolatti, G., & Craighero, L. (2004). The mirror-neuron system. Annual Review of Neuroscience, 27, 169–192.CrossRefPubMed Rizzolatti, G., & Craighero, L. (2004). The mirror-neuron system. Annual Review of Neuroscience, 27, 169–192.CrossRefPubMed
go back to reference Rogers, S. J. (1999). An examination of the imitation deficit in autism. In J. A. B. Nadel (Ed.), Imitation in infancy (pp. 255–283). Cambridge: Cambridge University Press. Rogers, S. J. (1999). An examination of the imitation deficit in autism. In J. A. B. Nadel (Ed.), Imitation in infancy (pp. 255–283). Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.
go back to reference Rogers, S. J., Hepburn, S. L., Stackhouse, T., & Wehner, E. (2003). Imitation performance in toddlers with autism and those with other developmental disorders. Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry, 44(5), 763–781. doi:10.1111/1469-7610.00162.CrossRefPubMed Rogers, S. J., Hepburn, S. L., Stackhouse, T., & Wehner, E. (2003). Imitation performance in toddlers with autism and those with other developmental disorders. Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry, 44(5), 763–781. doi:10.​1111/​1469-7610.​00162.CrossRefPubMed
go back to reference Rumiati, R. I., Weiss, P. H., Tessari, A., Assmus, A., Zilles, K., Herzog, H., & Fink, G. R. (2005). Common and differential neural mechanisms supporting imitation of meaningful and meaningless actions. Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience, 17(9), 1420–1431.CrossRefPubMed Rumiati, R. I., Weiss, P. H., Tessari, A., Assmus, A., Zilles, K., Herzog, H., & Fink, G. R. (2005). Common and differential neural mechanisms supporting imitation of meaningful and meaningless actions. Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience, 17(9), 1420–1431.CrossRefPubMed
go back to reference Salowitz, N. G., Eccarius, P., Karst, J., Carson, A., Schohl, K., Stevens, S., & Scheidt, R. (2013). Brief report: Visuo-spatial guidance of movement during gesture imitation and mirror drawing in children with autism spectrum disorders. Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders, 43(4), 985–995. doi:10.1007/s10803-012-1631-8.CrossRefPubMed Salowitz, N. G., Eccarius, P., Karst, J., Carson, A., Schohl, K., Stevens, S., & Scheidt, R. (2013). Brief report: Visuo-spatial guidance of movement during gesture imitation and mirror drawing in children with autism spectrum disorders. Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders, 43(4), 985–995. doi:10.​1007/​s10803-012-1631-8.CrossRefPubMed
go back to reference Smith, I. M., & Bryson, S. E. (1994). Imitation and action in autism: A critical review. Psychological Bulletin, 116(2), 259–273.CrossRefPubMed Smith, I. M., & Bryson, S. E. (1994). Imitation and action in autism: A critical review. Psychological Bulletin, 116(2), 259–273.CrossRefPubMed
go back to reference Stewart, H. J., McIntosh, R. D., & Williams, J. H. G. (2013). A specific deficit of imitation in autism spectrum disorder. Autism Research,. doi:10.1002/aur.1312. Stewart, H. J., McIntosh, R. D., & Williams, J. H. G. (2013). A specific deficit of imitation in autism spectrum disorder. Autism Research,. doi:10.​1002/​aur.​1312.
go back to reference Swettenham, J., Baron-Cohen, S., Charman, T., Cox, A., Baird, G., Drew, A., & Wheelwright, S. (1998). The frequency and distribution of spontaneous attention shifts between social and nonsocial stimuli in autistic, typically developing, and nonautistic developmentally delayed infants. Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry and Allied Disciplines, 39(05), 747–753.CrossRef Swettenham, J., Baron-Cohen, S., Charman, T., Cox, A., Baird, G., Drew, A., & Wheelwright, S. (1998). The frequency and distribution of spontaneous attention shifts between social and nonsocial stimuli in autistic, typically developing, and nonautistic developmentally delayed infants. Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry and Allied Disciplines, 39(05), 747–753.CrossRef
go back to reference Théoret, H., Halligan, E., Kobayashi, M., Fregni, F., Tager-Flusberg, H., & Pascual-Leone, A. (2005). Impaired motor facilitation during action observation in individuals with autism spectrum disorder. Current Biology, 15(3), R84–R85. doi:10.1016/j.cub.2005.01.022.CrossRefPubMed Théoret, H., Halligan, E., Kobayashi, M., Fregni, F., Tager-Flusberg, H., & Pascual-Leone, A. (2005). Impaired motor facilitation during action observation in individuals with autism spectrum disorder. Current Biology, 15(3), R84–R85. doi:10.​1016/​j.​cub.​2005.​01.​022.CrossRefPubMed
go back to reference Vivanti, G., & Hamilton, A. F. D. C. (2014). Imitation in autism spectrum disorders handbook of autism and pervasive developmental disorders (4th Edn.). New Jersey: Wiley. Vivanti, G., & Hamilton, A. F. D. C. (2014). Imitation in autism spectrum disorders handbook of autism and pervasive developmental disorders (4th Edn.). New Jersey: Wiley.
go back to reference Wang, Y., & Hamilton, A. F. D. C. (2012). Social top-down response modulation (STORM): A model of the control of mimicry in social interaction. Frontiers in Human Neuroscience, 6, 1. doi:10.3389/fnhum.2012.00153. Wang, Y., & Hamilton, A. F. D. C. (2012). Social top-down response modulation (STORM): A model of the control of mimicry in social interaction. Frontiers in Human Neuroscience, 6, 1. doi:10.​3389/​fnhum.​2012.​00153.
go back to reference Wechsler, D. (1999). Wechsler abbreviated scales of intelligence (WASI). San Antonio, TX: Psychological Corporation. Wechsler, D. (1999). Wechsler abbreviated scales of intelligence (WASI). San Antonio, TX: Psychological Corporation.
go back to reference Wild, K. S., Poliakoff, E., Jerrison, A., & Gowen, E. (2012). Goal-directed and goal-less imitation in autism spectrum disorder. Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders. doi:10.1007/s10803-011-1417-4.PubMed Wild, K. S., Poliakoff, E., Jerrison, A., & Gowen, E. (2012). Goal-directed and goal-less imitation in autism spectrum disorder. Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders. doi:10.​1007/​s10803-011-1417-4.PubMed
go back to reference Williams, J. H. G., Casey, J. M., Braadbaart, L., Culmer, P. R., & Mon-Williams, M. (2014). Kinematic measures of imitation fidelity in primary school children. Journal of Cognition and Development, 15(2), 345–362. doi:10.1080/15248372.2013.771265.CrossRef Williams, J. H. G., Casey, J. M., Braadbaart, L., Culmer, P. R., & Mon-Williams, M. (2014). Kinematic measures of imitation fidelity in primary school children. Journal of Cognition and Development, 15(2), 345–362. doi:10.​1080/​15248372.​2013.​771265.CrossRef
go back to reference Wohlschlager, A., Gattis, M., & Bekkering, H. (2003). Action generation and action perception in imitation: An instance of the ideomotor principle. Philosophical Transactions of the Royal Society B: Biological Sciences, 358(1431), 501–515. doi:10.1098/rstb.2002.1257.CrossRef Wohlschlager, A., Gattis, M., & Bekkering, H. (2003). Action generation and action perception in imitation: An instance of the ideomotor principle. Philosophical Transactions of the Royal Society B: Biological Sciences, 358(1431), 501–515. doi:10.​1098/​rstb.​2002.​1257.CrossRef
Metagegevens
Titel
Low Fidelity Imitation of Atypical Biological Kinematics in Autism Spectrum Disorders Is Modulated by Self-Generated Selective Attention
Auteurs
Spencer J. Hayes
Matthew Andrew
Digby Elliott
Emma Gowen
Simon J. Bennett
Publicatiedatum
09-09-2015
Uitgeverij
Springer US
Gepubliceerd in
Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders / Uitgave 2/2016
Print ISSN: 0162-3257
Elektronisch ISSN: 1573-3432
DOI
https://doi.org/10.1007/s10803-015-2588-1

Andere artikelen Uitgave 2/2016

Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders 2/2016 Naar de uitgave