Elsevier

Psychiatry Research

Volume 166, Issue 1, 31 March 2009, Pages 46-53
Psychiatry Research

Theory of mind and verbal working memory deficits in parents of autistic children

https://doi.org/10.1016/j.psychres.2007.11.016Get rights and content

Abstract

The objective of this study was to investigate the potential values of executive function and social cognition deficits as endophenotypes of autism. While theory of mind (ToM) is generally accepted as a unitary concept, some have suggested that ToM may be separated into two components (mental state reasoning and decoding). In this study, both aspects of ToM and verbal working memory abilities were investigated with relatively demanding tasks. The authors used a neurocognitive battery to compare the executive function and social cognition skills of 76 parents of autistic probands with 41 parents of healthy children. Both groups were matched for IQ, age and gender. Index parents had verbal working memory deficits. They had also low performance on a mental state reasoning task. Index parents had difficulties in reasoning about others' emotions. In contrast to findings in the control group, low performance of mental state reasoning ability was not associated with working memory deficit in index parents. Social cognition and working memory impairments may represent potential endophenotypes, related to an underlying vulnerability for autistic spectrum disorders.

Introduction

Autistic disorder, Asperger syndrome (AS) and pervasive developmental disorder not otherwise specified are the most common disorders believed to lie along a severity continuum in the spectrum of autistic conditions. These disorders seem to share common genetic susceptibility factors (Pickles et al., 2000). Indeed, these genetic factors can also be manifested in the characteristics of “unaffected” family members of probands with autism. Previous studies have shown that relatives of individuals with autism exhibit higher than normal rates of subtle autism-related personality, social and language features, which are referred to as the “broader autism phenotype” (Bolton et al., 1994, Piven, 2001, Dawson et al., 2002, Constantino et al., 2006).

Despite these findings, no specific genetic factor for autistic disorder has definitely been identified. Heterogeneity and complex phenotypes of psychiatric disorders are among the major challenging issues in genetic studies. A different strategy for overcoming these difficulties is the investigation of pre-behavioural phenotypes that should also be found in unaffected relatives of patients with psychiatric disorders. These phenotypes are commonly called endophenotypes and include electrophysiological, cognitive and neuroanatomical processes. Since endophenotypes are thought to reflect more basic components of complex mental disorders, they may be more closely related to underlying etiological processes than to clinical symptoms. This approach may help to fill the gap in understanding the causal link between complex behavioural manifestation (as in autistic disorder) and genes, and may be essential to discover the genetic susceptibility factors in this disorder (Gottesman and Gould, 2003). Although the endophenotype research for developmental psychopathologies is in its early stages, it holds a promise for clarifying the gene–brain–behaviour pathways for autistic spectrum disorders (Viding and Blakemore, 2007).

Since there is considerable evidence for executive (Ozonoff et al., 1991, Bennetto et al., 1996, Hill, 2004) and social-cognitive dysfunctions (Baron-Cohen et al., 1985) in autistic patients, these cognitive abilities are among the potential endophenotypes of autism. The executive function theory of autism assumes that a form of frontal pathology leading to deficits in working memory (WM) or other abilities such as the inability to shift attention and cognitive inflexibility may be essential in explaining symptoms of the disorder. A limited number of studies have reported deficits in some aspects of executive functions (EF), including cognitive flexibility and spatial working memory in relatives of autistic patients (Ozonoff et al., 1993, Hughes et al., 1997, Piven and Palmer, 1997, Koczat et al., 2002). EF deficits seen in relatives of autistic patients have been most consistently reported with the Tower of Hanoi test (Ozonoff et al., 1993, Hughes et al., 1997, Piven and Palmer, 1997) and to a lesser extent with some cognitive flexibility tasks including the Extradimensional/Intradimensional Shifting Test (Hughes et al., 1997, Wong et al., 2006). Spatial WM deficits seen in relatives of patients with autistic spectrum disorders have also been reported (Koczat et al., 2002). As far as we know, evidence for verbal WM dysfunction in relatives of autistic patients has not been reported yet. Since WM is also considered to be a key component of the other EF abilities, WM deficit may have the potential to explain the relationship between other EF abilities and autism. However, some negative findings in patients with autistic spectrum diagnoses cast doubt on the importance of WM dysfunction as a core feature of the disease (Williams et al., 2005). We believe that these inconsistencies are due to task difficulty; WM tasks that place greater demands on EF may be more suitable to demonstrate WM dysfunction in autism-related conditions.

One well-known area of research in autism involves the theory of mind (ToM) deficit hypothesis (Baron-Cohen et al., 1985). Investigators have proposed that deficits in a component of social cognition may explain many symptoms of autism. Although ToM deficits have been extensively studied in autistic patients, ToM abilities have not been adequately studied in relatives of autistic patients. Furthermore, ToM ability has been investigated as a unitary skill in most of the previous studies. It has been suggested, however, that the ToM concept is composed of two components: mental state decoding (social-perceptual) and mental state reasoning (social-cognitive) (Tager-Flusberg and Sullivan, 2000, Sabbagh, 2004). The mental state decoding component involves the ability to perceive mental states of others based on observable information such as facial expressions or gestures. The mental state reasoning component involves the ability to integrate contextual and historical information about a person (attitudes, knowledge, experiences) to understand behaviour. Previous findings suggest that different aspects of ToM depend on different social brain networks (Sabbagh, 2004). While orbitofrontal cortex and temporal cortex activation may be related to social-perceptual abilities, the medial frontal cortex seems to be critical for the ability to reason about others' mental states.

Since the two aspects of ToM rely on different neural networks, potential endophenotypes of these two aspects should be investigated separately. Several studies have shown that relatives of patients with autistic spectrum disorders seem to fail in the mental state decoding aspect of ToM. While relatives of autistic patients have been reported not to be different from control subjects in basic emotion recognition tasks, Dorris et al. (2004) and Baron-Cohen and Hammer (1997) reported a deficit in one task of social cognition targeting automatic recognition of complex states of mind, called the Eyes task, in parents and siblings of individuals with autistic spectrum disorders. Although the second aspect of ToM, mental state reasoning ability, has been shown to be severely impaired in autistic patients (failure to pass false belief paradigms), this aspect of social cognition has not been adequately studied in relatives of autistic patients. Ozonoff et al. (1993) reported that relatives of autistic patients do not have a deficit in false belief tasks, but this task may be extremely easy for high functioning populations and even for many high functioning autistic/autistic spectrum patients. Several advanced ToM tests have been developed (e.g., the Strange Stories Test; Rajendran and Mitchell, 2007), and these include tasks that may be suitable for research on endophenotypes.

While domain-specific theories suggest that ToM abilities are independent from executive functions, domain-general theories expect a relationship between these two cognitive skills. In accordance with the domain-general view, Gordon and Olson (1998) reported an association between development of ToM abilities and the WM capacity of children. It is likely that the capacity to hold critical information regarding social cues in mind and to process this information may be essential for intact ToM skills, especially for reasoning abilities.

The primary aim of this present study was to test the potential values of verbal working memory and different aspects of social cognition as endophenotypes for autism. We used more demanding tasks for verbal working memory and mental state reasoning abilities. We hypothesized that relatives of probands with classical autism will have deficits in social cognition and verbal working memory. We also investigated the relationship between ToM and WM abilities in relatives of autistic patients. We predicted that WM deficits seen in relatives of autistic patients would contribute to ToM dysfunction.

Section snippets

Participants

All of the autistic children were recruited from the pool of outpatients with a diagnosis of autistic disorder established in the Child and Adolescent Psychiatry Department of Ege University, Izmir, Turkey. We selected probands in the age range of 4–18 with classical autism and mental retardation in order to increase the homogeneity of the group. We limited the sample to subjects with a core diagnosis of autism because of concerns about distinguishing between conditions within the autism

Demographic and clinical variables

The two groups were not significantly different in terms of demographic variables. Although depressive symptoms were not prominent in either of the groups, the BPRS depression item score was still significantly higher in index parents (Table 1).

Neurocognitive and ToM measures

The total IQ scores of both groups were not significantly different. Verbal working memory performance was better in the control parents than in the index parents. Index parents also had deficits on the UOT. Index parents did not perform significantly

Discussion

This study compared the executive functions and social-cognitive functions of parents of autistic children with parents of healthy children. The index group had deficits in verbal working memory and in the reasoning component of social cognition compared with the control group. Although parents of autistic patients had no history of psychiatric treatment, they had mild depressive symptoms compared with the control group, which is consistent with previous findings (Piven and Palmer, 2001).

Our

References (43)

  • AdolphsR. et al.

    Impaired recognition of social emotions following amygdala damage

    Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience

    (2002)
  • Baron-CohenS. et al.

    Parents of children with Asperger syndrome: what is cognitive phenotype?

    Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience

    (1997)
  • Baron-CohenS. et al.

    The “Reading the Mind in the Eyes” Test revised version: a study with normal adults, and adults with Asperger syndrome or high-functioning autism

    Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry

    (2001)
  • BennettoL. et al.

    Intact and impaired memory functions in autism

    Child Development

    (1996)
  • BolteS. et al.

    The recognition of facial affect in autistic and schizophrenic individuals and their first-degree relatives

    Psychological Medicine

    (2003)
  • BoltonP. et al.

    A case control family history study of autism

    Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry

    (1994)
  • BoraE. et al.

    Evidence for theory of mind deficits in bipolar disorder

    Acta Psychiatrica Scandinavica

    (2005)
  • ConstantinoJ.N. et al.

    Autistic social impairment in the siblings of children with pervasive developmental disorder

    American Journal of Psychiatry

    (2006)
  • CrawfordJ.R. et al.

    Short-forms of the UK WAIS-R: regression equations and their predictive validity in a general population sample

    British Journal of Clinical Psychology

    (1992)
  • DawsonG. et al.

    Defining the broader phenotype of autism: genetic, brain and behavioral perspectives

    Development and Psychopathology

    (2002)
  • DorrisL. et al.

    Mind-reading difficulties in the siblings of people with Asperger's syndrome: evidence for a genetic influence in the abnormal development of a specific cognitive domain

    Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry

    (2004)
  • Cited by (59)

    • Intact verbal fluency abilities in the Broad Autism Phenotype

      2018, Psychiatry Research
      Citation Excerpt :

      This result is not likely to be related to low power, as there were an adequate number of participants for the number of variables entered (Cohen et al., 2003). As research suggests a relation between global executive function and social competence, this finding suggests that other components of executive function aside from strategy generation may be targets for research, such as working memory or inhibition (Amado et al., 2016; Cox et al., 2016; Engelhardt et al., 2010; Gökçen et al., 2009). Methodological improvements that impacted sampling may be related to the failure to replicate prior findings related to group differences.

    View all citing articles on Scopus
    View full text