Skip to main content
Top
Gepubliceerd in: Psychological Research 5/2016

02-08-2015 | Original Article

The time-course of distractor processing in auditory spatial negative priming

Auteurs: Malte Möller, Susanne Mayr, Axel Buchner

Gepubliceerd in: Psychological Research | Uitgave 5/2016

Log in om toegang te krijgen
share
DELEN

Deel dit onderdeel of sectie (kopieer de link)

  • Optie A:
    Klik op de rechtermuisknop op de link en selecteer de optie “linkadres kopiëren”
  • Optie B:
    Deel de link per e-mail

Abstract

The spatial negative priming effect denotes slowed-down and sometimes more error-prone responding to a location that previously contained a distractor as compared with a previously unoccupied location. In vision, this effect has been attributed to the inhibition of irrelevant locations, and recently, of their task-assigned responses. Interestingly, auditory versions of the task did not yield evidence for inhibitory processing of task-irrelevant events which might suggest modality-specific distractor processing in vision and audition. Alternatively, the inhibitory processes may differ in how they develop over time. If this were the case, the absence of inhibitory after-effects might be due to an inappropriate timing of successive presentations in previous auditory spatial negative priming tasks. Specifically, the distractor may not yet have been inhibited or inhibition may already have dissipated at the time performance is assessed. The present study was conducted to test these alternatives. Participants indicated the location of a target sound in the presence of a concurrent distractor sound. Performance was assessed between two successive prime-probe presentations. The time between the prime response and the probe sounds (response-stimulus interval, RSI) was systematically varied between three groups (600, 1250, 1900 ms). For all RSI groups, the results showed no evidence for inhibitory distractor processing but conformed to the predictions of the feature mismatching hypothesis. The results support the assumption that auditory distractor processing does not recruit an inhibitory mechanism but involves the integration of spatial and sound identity features into common representations.
Voetnoten
1
This conclusion is further supported by studies employing so-called “free choice” probe trials. While prime trials require a standard forced choice response to the prime target location, participants can choose to answer with one of two valid response options in probe trials. In the critical trials, prime distractor and probe target are presented at different locations and participants can decide whether to perform the former prime distractor response or a new response to the probe target location. The finding that former distractor-assigned responses are less likely to be executed in these trials has been taken as further evidence for distractor-related response inhibition (e.g., Buckolz et al., 2015; Fitzgeorge et al., 2011).
 
2
Buckolz, Fitzgeorge, and Knowles (2012) argued that a location-based “orienting inhibition” mechanism only operates in visuospatial negative priming when parafoveal stimulus locations are employed in the task at hand.
 
3
Note that this effect was found in most, but not in all auditory spatial negative priming tasks which might indicate that object file retrieval is more effectively triggered by location repetitions (as compared with sound identity repetitions) between prime distractor and probe target. Moreover, evidence for feature mismatching effects has also been reported in visuospatial negative priming (Park & Kanwisher, 1994). However, several studies found reliable spatial negative priming effects in the absence of feature mismatches (Milliken et al., 2000; Tipper et al., 1995), so that this mechanism seems to be only of minor importance in the visual modality.
 
4
Ignored repetition and control trials in Experiment 2 did not require a prime response.
 
5
Given that after-effects attributed to inhibition in vision require at least 75 ms to emerge but are present, at the latest, after 750 ms following the prime presentation (Buckolz et al., 2008), it might also be possible that reliable performance costs in ignored repetition trials (as compared with control trials) can also be obtained with interval durations in between these values. However, this assumption remains speculative. Therefore, the presumed time-course of distractor response inhibition in the visual modality can only be tentatively applied to the 600-ms RSI in the present study.
 
Literatuur
go back to reference Broadbent, D. E. (1970). Stimulus and response set: two kinds of selective attention. In I. Mostotsky (Ed.), Attention: Comtemporary theories and analysis (pp. 51–60). New York: Appleton-Century-Crofts. Broadbent, D. E. (1970). Stimulus and response set: two kinds of selective attention. In I. Mostotsky (Ed.), Attention: Comtemporary theories and analysis (pp. 51–60). New York: Appleton-Century-Crofts.
go back to reference Buckolz, E., Edgar, C., Kajaste, B., Lok, M., & Khan, M. (2012a). Inhibited prime-trial distractor responses solely produce the visual spatial negative priming effect. Attention, Perception, & Psychophysics, 74(8), 1632–1643. doi:10.3758/s13414-012-0366-0.CrossRef Buckolz, E., Edgar, C., Kajaste, B., Lok, M., & Khan, M. (2012a). Inhibited prime-trial distractor responses solely produce the visual spatial negative priming effect. Attention, Perception, & Psychophysics, 74(8), 1632–1643. doi:10.​3758/​s13414-012-0366-0.CrossRef
go back to reference Buckolz, E., Goldfarb, A., & Khan, M. (2004). The use of a distractor-assigned response slows later responding in a location negative priming task. Perception and Psychophysics, 66(5), 837–845. doi:10.3758/BF03194977.CrossRefPubMed Buckolz, E., Goldfarb, A., & Khan, M. (2004). The use of a distractor-assigned response slows later responding in a location negative priming task. Perception and Psychophysics, 66(5), 837–845. doi:10.​3758/​BF03194977.CrossRefPubMed
go back to reference Buckolz, E., Lok, M., Kajaste, B., Edgar, C., & Khan, M. (2015). The preservation of response inhibition aftereffects in a location-based spatial negative priming task: younger versus older adults. Psychological Research, 79(1), 120–133. doi:10.1007/s00426-014-0541-0.CrossRefPubMed Buckolz, E., Lok, M., Kajaste, B., Edgar, C., & Khan, M. (2015). The preservation of response inhibition aftereffects in a location-based spatial negative priming task: younger versus older adults. Psychological Research, 79(1), 120–133. doi:10.​1007/​s00426-014-0541-0.CrossRefPubMed
go back to reference Buckolz, E., Stoddart, A., Edgar, C., & Khan, M. (2014). The error protection impact of inhibitory after-effects in a location-based task and its preservation with practice. Attention, Perception, & Psychophysics, 76(6), 1721–1728. doi:10.3758/s13414-014-0701-8.CrossRef Buckolz, E., Stoddart, A., Edgar, C., & Khan, M. (2014). The error protection impact of inhibitory after-effects in a location-based task and its preservation with practice. Attention, Perception, & Psychophysics, 76(6), 1721–1728. doi:10.​3758/​s13414-014-0701-8.CrossRef
go back to reference Chao, H.-F. (2009). Revisiting the role of probe distractors in negative priming: location negative priming is observed when probe distractors are consistently absent. Attention, Perception, & Psychophysics, 71(5), 1072–1082. doi:10.3758/APP.71.5.1072.CrossRef Chao, H.-F. (2009). Revisiting the role of probe distractors in negative priming: location negative priming is observed when probe distractors are consistently absent. Attention, Perception, & Psychophysics, 71(5), 1072–1082. doi:10.​3758/​APP.​71.​5.​1072.CrossRef
go back to reference Cohen, J. (1988). Statistical power analysis for the behavioral sciences (2nd ed.). Hillsdale: Erlbaum. Cohen, J. (1988). Statistical power analysis for the behavioral sciences (2nd ed.). Hillsdale: Erlbaum.
go back to reference Faul, F., Erdfelder, E., Lang, A.-G., & Buchner, A. (2007). G*Power 3: a flexible statistical power analysis program for the social, behavioral, and biomedical sciences. Behavior Research Methods, 39(2), 175–191. doi:10.3758/BF03193146.CrossRefPubMed Faul, F., Erdfelder, E., Lang, A.-G., & Buchner, A. (2007). G*Power 3: a flexible statistical power analysis program for the social, behavioral, and biomedical sciences. Behavior Research Methods, 39(2), 175–191. doi:10.​3758/​BF03193146.CrossRefPubMed
go back to reference Fitzgeorge, L., & Buckolz, E. (2008). Spatial negative priming modulation: the influence of probe-trial target cueing, distractor presence, and an intervening response. European Journal of Cognitive Psychology, 20(6), 994–1026. doi:10.1080/09541440701686250.CrossRef Fitzgeorge, L., & Buckolz, E. (2008). Spatial negative priming modulation: the influence of probe-trial target cueing, distractor presence, and an intervening response. European Journal of Cognitive Psychology, 20(6), 994–1026. doi:10.​1080/​0954144070168625​0.CrossRef
go back to reference Fitzgeorge, L., Buckolz, E., & Khan, M. (2011). Recently inhibited responses are avoided for both masked and nonmasked primes in a spatial negative priming task. Attention, Perception, & Psychophysics, 73(5), 1435–1452. doi:10.3758/s13414-011-0125-7.CrossRef Fitzgeorge, L., Buckolz, E., & Khan, M. (2011). Recently inhibited responses are avoided for both masked and nonmasked primes in a spatial negative priming task. Attention, Perception, & Psychophysics, 73(5), 1435–1452. doi:10.​3758/​s13414-011-0125-7.CrossRef
go back to reference Fletcher, B. C., & Rabbitt, P. M. (1978). The changing pattern of perceptual analytic strategies and response selection with practice in a two-choice reaction time task. The Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology, 30(3), 417–427. doi:10.1080/00335557843000025.CrossRefPubMed Fletcher, B. C., & Rabbitt, P. M. (1978). The changing pattern of perceptual analytic strategies and response selection with practice in a two-choice reaction time task. The Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology, 30(3), 417–427. doi:10.​1080/​0033555784300002​5.CrossRefPubMed
go back to reference Guy, S., Buckolz, E., & Khan, M. (2006). The locus of location repetition latency effects. Canadian Journal of Experimental Psychology/Revue canadienne de psychologie experimentale, 60(4), 307–318. doi:10.1037/cjep2006028.CrossRefPubMed Guy, S., Buckolz, E., & Khan, M. (2006). The locus of location repetition latency effects. Canadian Journal of Experimental Psychology/Revue canadienne de psychologie experimentale, 60(4), 307–318. doi:10.​1037/​cjep2006028.CrossRefPubMed
go back to reference Hall, M. D., Pastore, R. E., Acker, B. E., & Huang, W. (2000). Evidence for auditory feature integration with spatially distributed items. Perception and Psychophysics, 62(6), 1243–1257. doi:10.3758/BF03212126.CrossRefPubMed Hall, M. D., Pastore, R. E., Acker, B. E., & Huang, W. (2000). Evidence for auditory feature integration with spatially distributed items. Perception and Psychophysics, 62(6), 1243–1257. doi:10.​3758/​BF03212126.CrossRefPubMed
go back to reference Haworth, P., Buckolz, E., & Kajaste, B. (2014). The role of probe trial distractors in the production/removal of the spatial negative priming effect. Journal of Cognitive Psychology, 26(4). doi:10.1080/20445911.2014.896368. Haworth, P., Buckolz, E., & Kajaste, B. (2014). The role of probe trial distractors in the production/removal of the spatial negative priming effect. Journal of Cognitive Psychology, 26(4). doi:10.​1080/​20445911.​2014.​896368.
go back to reference Holm, S. (1979). A simple sequentially rejective multiple test procedure. Scandinavian Journal of Statistics, 6(2), 65–70. Holm, S. (1979). A simple sequentially rejective multiple test procedure. Scandinavian Journal of Statistics, 6(2), 65–70.
go back to reference Houghton, G., & Tipper, S. P. (1994). A model of inhibitory mechanisms in selective attention. In D. Dagenbach & T. Carr (Eds.), Inhibitory processes in attention, memory, and language (pp. 53–112). San Diego: Academic Press. Houghton, G., & Tipper, S. P. (1994). A model of inhibitory mechanisms in selective attention. In D. Dagenbach & T. Carr (Eds.), Inhibitory processes in attention, memory, and language (pp. 53–112). San Diego: Academic Press.
go back to reference Krueger, L. E., & Shapiro, R. G. (1981). Intertrial effects of same-different judgements. The Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology A: Human Experimental Psychology, 33A(3), 241–265. doi:10.1080/14640748108400791.CrossRef Krueger, L. E., & Shapiro, R. G. (1981). Intertrial effects of same-different judgements. The Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology A: Human Experimental Psychology, 33A(3), 241–265. doi:10.​1080/​1464074810840079​1.CrossRef
go back to reference Maybery, M. T., Clissa, P. J., Parmentier, F. B. R., Leung, D., Harsa, G., Fox, A. M., & Jones, D. M. (2009). Binding of verbal and spatial features in auditory working memory. Journal of Memory and Language, 61(1), 112–133. doi:10.1016/j.jml.2009.03.001.CrossRef Maybery, M. T., Clissa, P. J., Parmentier, F. B. R., Leung, D., Harsa, G., Fox, A. M., & Jones, D. M. (2009). Binding of verbal and spatial features in auditory working memory. Journal of Memory and Language, 61(1), 112–133. doi:10.​1016/​j.​jml.​2009.​03.​001.CrossRef
go back to reference Mayr, S., Buchner, A., Möller, M., & Hauke, R. (2011). Spatial and identity negative priming in audition: evidence of feature binding in auditory spatial memory. Attention, Perception, & Psychophysics, 73(6), 1710–1732. doi:10.3758/s13414-011-0138-2.CrossRef Mayr, S., Buchner, A., Möller, M., & Hauke, R. (2011). Spatial and identity negative priming in audition: evidence of feature binding in auditory spatial memory. Attention, Perception, & Psychophysics, 73(6), 1710–1732. doi:10.​3758/​s13414-011-0138-2.CrossRef
go back to reference Milliken, B., Tipper, S. P., Houghton, G., & Lupiáñez, J. (2000). Attending, ignoring, and repetition: on the relation between negative priming and inhibition of return. Perception and Psychophysics, 62(6), 1280–1296. doi:10.3758/BF03212130.CrossRefPubMed Milliken, B., Tipper, S. P., Houghton, G., & Lupiáñez, J. (2000). Attending, ignoring, and repetition: on the relation between negative priming and inhibition of return. Perception and Psychophysics, 62(6), 1280–1296. doi:10.​3758/​BF03212130.CrossRefPubMed
go back to reference Milliken, B., Tipper, S. P., & Weaver, B. (1994). Negative priming in a spatial localization task: feature mismatching and distractor inhibition. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 20(3), 624–646. doi:10.1037/0096-1523.20.3.624. Milliken, B., Tipper, S. P., & Weaver, B. (1994). Negative priming in a spatial localization task: feature mismatching and distractor inhibition. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 20(3), 624–646. doi:10.​1037/​0096-1523.​20.​3.​624.
go back to reference Möller, M., Mayr, S., & Buchner, A. (2013). Target localization among concurrent sound sources: no evidence for the inhibition of previous distractor responses. Attention, Perception, & Psychophysics, 75(1), 132–144. doi:10.3758/s13414-012-0380-2.CrossRef Möller, M., Mayr, S., & Buchner, A. (2013). Target localization among concurrent sound sources: no evidence for the inhibition of previous distractor responses. Attention, Perception, & Psychophysics, 75(1), 132–144. doi:10.​3758/​s13414-012-0380-2.CrossRef
go back to reference Mondor, T. A., Zatorre, R. J., & Terrio, N. A. (1998). Constraints on the selection of auditory information. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 24(1), 66–79. doi:10.1037/0096-1523.24.1.66. Mondor, T. A., Zatorre, R. J., & Terrio, N. A. (1998). Constraints on the selection of auditory information. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 24(1), 66–79. doi:10.​1037/​0096-1523.​24.​1.​66.
go back to reference Neill, W. T., & Valdes, L. A. (1996). Facilitatory and inhibitory aspects of attention Converging operations in the study of visual selective attention (pp. 77–106). Washington, DC: American Psychological Association; US.CrossRef Neill, W. T., & Valdes, L. A. (1996). Facilitatory and inhibitory aspects of attention Converging operations in the study of visual selective attention (pp. 77–106). Washington, DC: American Psychological Association; US.CrossRef
go back to reference Neill, W. T., Valdes, L. A., & Terry, K. M. (1995). Selective attention and the inhibitory control of cognition. Interference and inhibition in cognition (pp. 207–261). San Diego: Academic Press.CrossRef Neill, W. T., Valdes, L. A., & Terry, K. M. (1995). Selective attention and the inhibitory control of cognition. Interference and inhibition in cognition (pp. 207–261). San Diego: Academic Press.CrossRef
go back to reference Neill, W. T., Valdes, L. A., Terry, K. M., & Gorfein, D. S. (1992). Persistence of negative priming: II. Evidence for episodic trace retrieval. Journal of Experimental Psychology. Learning, Memory, and Cognition, 18(5), 993–1000. doi:10.1037/0278-7393.18.5.993.CrossRefPubMed Neill, W. T., Valdes, L. A., Terry, K. M., & Gorfein, D. S. (1992). Persistence of negative priming: II. Evidence for episodic trace retrieval. Journal of Experimental Psychology. Learning, Memory, and Cognition, 18(5), 993–1000. doi:10.​1037/​0278-7393.​18.​5.​993.CrossRefPubMed
go back to reference Park, J., & Kanwisher, N. (1994). Negative priming for spatial locations: identity mismatching, not distractor inhibition. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 20(3), 613–623. doi:10.1037/0096-1523.20.3.613.PubMed Park, J., & Kanwisher, N. (1994). Negative priming for spatial locations: identity mismatching, not distractor inhibition. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 20(3), 613–623. doi:10.​1037/​0096-1523.​20.​3.​613.PubMed
go back to reference Parmentier, F. B. R., Maybery, M. T., & Elsley, J. (2010). The involuntary capture of attention by novel feature pairings: a study of voice-location integration in auditory sensory memory. Attention, Perception, & Psychophysics, 72(2), 279–284. doi:10.3758/APP.72.2.279.CrossRef Parmentier, F. B. R., Maybery, M. T., & Elsley, J. (2010). The involuntary capture of attention by novel feature pairings: a study of voice-location integration in auditory sensory memory. Attention, Perception, & Psychophysics, 72(2), 279–284. doi:10.​3758/​APP.​72.​2.​279.CrossRef
go back to reference Ridderinkhof, K. R. (2002). Activation and suppression in conflict tasks: Empirical clarification through distributional analyses. In W. Prinz & B. Hommel (Eds.), Common mechanisms in perception and action (pp. 494–519). Oxford: Oxford University Press. Ridderinkhof, K. R. (2002). Activation and suppression in conflict tasks: Empirical clarification through distributional analyses. In W. Prinz & B. Hommel (Eds.), Common mechanisms in perception and action (pp. 494–519). Oxford: Oxford University Press.
go back to reference Tipper, S. P. (2001). Does negative priming reflect inhibitory mechanisms? A review and integration of conflicting views. The Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology A: Human Experimental Psychology, 54A(2), 321–343. doi:10.1080/713755969.CrossRef Tipper, S. P. (2001). Does negative priming reflect inhibitory mechanisms? A review and integration of conflicting views. The Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology A: Human Experimental Psychology, 54A(2), 321–343. doi:10.​1080/​713755969.CrossRef
go back to reference Tipper, S. P., Brehaut, J. C., & Driver, J. (1990). Selection of moving and static objects for the control of spatially directed action. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 16(3), 492–504. doi:10.1037/0096-1523.16.3.492.PubMed Tipper, S. P., Brehaut, J. C., & Driver, J. (1990). Selection of moving and static objects for the control of spatially directed action. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 16(3), 492–504. doi:10.​1037/​0096-1523.​16.​3.​492.PubMed
go back to reference Tipper, S. P., Weaver, B., Cameron, S., Brehaut, J. C., & Bastedo, J. (1991). Inhibitory mechanisms of attention in identification and localization tasks: time course and disruption. Journal of Experimental Psychology. Learning, Memory, and Cognition, 17(4), 681–692. doi:10.1037/0278-7393.17.4.681.CrossRefPubMed Tipper, S. P., Weaver, B., Cameron, S., Brehaut, J. C., & Bastedo, J. (1991). Inhibitory mechanisms of attention in identification and localization tasks: time course and disruption. Journal of Experimental Psychology. Learning, Memory, and Cognition, 17(4), 681–692. doi:10.​1037/​0278-7393.​17.​4.​681.CrossRefPubMed
go back to reference Tipper, S. P., Weaver, B., & Milliken, B. (1995). Spatial negative priming without mismatching: comment on Park and Kanwisher (1994). Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 21(5), 1220–1229. doi:10.1037/0096-1523.21.5.1220. Tipper, S. P., Weaver, B., & Milliken, B. (1995). Spatial negative priming without mismatching: comment on Park and Kanwisher (1994). Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 21(5), 1220–1229. doi:10.​1037/​0096-1523.​21.​5.​1220.
go back to reference Treisman, A. (1993). The perception of features and objects. In A. Baddeley & L. Weiskrantz (Eds.), Attention: selection, awareness, and control: a tribute to Donald Broadbent (pp. 5–35). New York: Clarendon Press/Oxford University Press. Treisman, A. (1993). The perception of features and objects. In A. Baddeley & L. Weiskrantz (Eds.), Attention: selection, awareness, and control: a tribute to Donald Broadbent (pp. 5–35). New York: Clarendon Press/Oxford University Press.
go back to reference Van der Heijden, A. H. C. (1981). Short term visual forgetting. London: Routledge and Keegan Paul. Van der Heijden, A. H. C. (1981). Short term visual forgetting. London: Routledge and Keegan Paul.
Metagegevens
Titel
The time-course of distractor processing in auditory spatial negative priming
Auteurs
Malte Möller
Susanne Mayr
Axel Buchner
Publicatiedatum
02-08-2015
Uitgeverij
Springer Berlin Heidelberg
Gepubliceerd in
Psychological Research / Uitgave 5/2016
Print ISSN: 0340-0727
Elektronisch ISSN: 1430-2772
DOI
https://doi.org/10.1007/s00426-015-0685-6

Andere artikelen Uitgave 5/2016

Psychological Research 5/2016 Naar de uitgave