Skip to main content
Top
Gepubliceerd in: Psychological Research 2/2007

01-03-2007 | Original Article

The locus and modulation of the location negative priming effect

Auteurs: Sarah Guy, Eric Buckolz

Gepubliceerd in: Psychological Research | Uitgave 2/2007

Log in om toegang te krijgen
share
DELEN

Deel dit onderdeel of sectie (kopieer de link)

  • Optie A:
    Klik op de rechtermuisknop op de link en selecteer de optie “linkadres kopiëren”
  • Optie B:
    Deel de link per e-mail

Abstract

Responding to the location of a target is slower when it appears at a recent distractor location [ignored-repetition (IR) trial] than when it arises at a new position [control (CO) trial], defining the location negative priming (NP) effect. On IR trials, both the distractor location and response are from the prior trial, and the locus question asks whether the delayed responding that arises is caused by the reused distractor position (i.e., a location locus) or the need to execute a distractor output (i.e., a response locus). A location NP procedure was used, incorporating a many:1 location-to-response mapping design, along with a response cue on some trials. A response locus for the location NP effect was indicated. Distractor-turned-target responses took longer to initiate than new outputs (many:1 paradigm), and valid response cues reduced distractor response interference and the location NP effect. Importantly, a possible S-R compatibility problem within the many:1 S-R paradigm was not supported.
Voetnoten
1
While Tipper et al. (1990) have shown that the prime-trial distractor object itself would be inhibited, it is clear that object inhibition cannot be the locus or cause of the location NP effect, simply because the prime distractor object does not participate in ignored-repetition trials. Rather, on these occasions, the prime distractor object is replaced by the target stimulus object. Thus, any slowing seen on ignored-repetition trials cannot be the result of the re-appearance of the prime distractor object from one trial to the next.
 
2
While the predictable removal of the probe distractor eliminates the location NP effect when the prime trial contains both a target and a distractor (Buckolz et al., 2002a), it does not do so when distractor-only primes are used (not tested for identity tasks) [Guy et al., 2004; Milliken et al., 2000]. Hence, we were able to study the influence of valid response cues when the probe trial knowingly lacked a distractor event. We want to emphasize that probe distractor manipulations do not always influence location and identity NP effects in the same way (e.g., Buckolz et al., 2004a; Guy et al., 2004; Moore, 1994; Neill et al., 1994; Tipper et al., 1990) and so the findings here in this regard are intended to be applied only to the location NP process.
 
3
An anonymous reviewer raised the possibility that the need to process cue information between prime and probe trial deliveries, irrespective of its information content (i.e., uninformative vs. informative cues), might have had some unexpected impact on the normal operation of the NP process. Accordingly, we ran a location NP task where the cue event was removed but where the time delay between prime onset and probe offset approximated that used in the cue conditions (i.e., 2,000 ms). A significant location NP effect of 30 ms was obtained [RT(IR)= 504 ms (SD= 98) vs. RT(CO)= 474 ms (SD=85)], t(15)= 3.06, SDD= 39.00, P< 0.01, which compared favourably in size to the NP effect produced with the uninformative cue condition (36 ms, Table 2). These findings indicate that requiring cue processing that intervenes between prime and probe trials does not appear to influence the normal functioning of the location NP process, and also show that the NP process continues to function for at least 2000 ms. These findings are predictable from results reported by Tipper, Weaver, Cameron, Brehaut, and Bastedo (1991). They showed that processing that intervened between prime and probe trial presentations, which is unrelated to the NP task and which might include an overt response, does not influence the location NP process, and that this process continued to operate for about 6 seconds.
 
Literatuur
go back to reference Baylis, G. C., Tipper. S. P., & Houghton, G. (1997). Externally cued and internallygenerated selection: Differences in distractor analysis and inhibition. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 23, 1617–1630.PubMedCrossRef Baylis, G. C., Tipper. S. P., & Houghton, G. (1997). Externally cued and internallygenerated selection: Differences in distractor analysis and inhibition. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 23, 1617–1630.PubMedCrossRef
go back to reference Buckolz, E., & Rugins, O. (1981). Evidence of response bias facilitation on choice reaction time within a many:1 stimulus-response paradigm. Journal of Human Movement Studies, 5, 97–104. Buckolz, E., & Rugins, O. (1981). Evidence of response bias facilitation on choice reaction time within a many:1 stimulus-response paradigm. Journal of Human Movement Studies, 5, 97–104.
go back to reference Buckolz, E., Boulougouris, A., & Khan, M. (2002a). The influence of probe-trial selection requirements on the location negative priming effect. Canadian Journal of Experimental Psychology, 56, 274–283. Buckolz, E., Boulougouris, A., & Khan, M. (2002a). The influence of probe-trial selection requirements on the location negative priming effect. Canadian Journal of Experimental Psychology, 56, 274–283.
go back to reference Buckolz, E., Boulougouris, A., O‘Donnell, C., & Pratt, J. (2002b). Disengaging the negative priming mechanism in location tasks. European Journal of Cognitive Psychology, 14, 207–225.CrossRef Buckolz, E., Boulougouris, A., O‘Donnell, C., & Pratt, J. (2002b). Disengaging the negative priming mechanism in location tasks. European Journal of Cognitive Psychology, 14, 207–225.CrossRef
go back to reference Buckolz, E., Goldfarb, A., & Khan, M. (2004). The use of a distractor-assigned response slows later responding in a location negative priming task. Perception & Psychophysics, 66, 837–845. Buckolz, E., Goldfarb, A., & Khan, M. (2004). The use of a distractor-assigned response slows later responding in a location negative priming task. Perception & Psychophysics, 66, 837–845.
go back to reference Buckolz, E., Guy, S., Khan, M., & Lawrence, G. (2005). Can the location negative priming process operate in a proactive manner. Psychological Research (on line). Buckolz, E., Guy, S., Khan, M., & Lawrence, G. (2005). Can the location negative priming process operate in a proactive manner. Psychological Research (on line).
go back to reference Christie, J., & Klein, R. (2001). Negative priming for spatial location? Canadian Journal of Experimental Psychology, 55, 24–38.PubMed Christie, J., & Klein, R. (2001). Negative priming for spatial location? Canadian Journal of Experimental Psychology, 55, 24–38.PubMed
go back to reference De Jong, R., Liang, C-C., & Lauber, E. (1994) Conditional and unconditional automaticity: A dual-process model of effects of spatial stimulus-response correspondence. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 20, 731–750.PubMedCrossRef De Jong, R., Liang, C-C., & Lauber, E. (1994) Conditional and unconditional automaticity: A dual-process model of effects of spatial stimulus-response correspondence. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 20, 731–750.PubMedCrossRef
go back to reference Eimer, M. (1995). Stimulus-response compatibility and automatic response activation: Evidence from psychophysiological studies. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 21, 837–854.PubMedCrossRef Eimer, M. (1995). Stimulus-response compatibility and automatic response activation: Evidence from psychophysiological studies. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 21, 837–854.PubMedCrossRef
go back to reference Eimer, M., Schubo, A., & Schlaghecken, F. (2002). Locus of inhibition in the masked priming of response alternatives. Journal of Motor Behavior, 34, 3–10.PubMedCrossRef Eimer, M., Schubo, A., & Schlaghecken, F. (2002). Locus of inhibition in the masked priming of response alternatives. Journal of Motor Behavior, 34, 3–10.PubMedCrossRef
go back to reference Eriksen, C. W., Coles, M. G. H., Morris, L. R., & O’Hara, W. P. (1985). An electromyographic examination of response competition. Bulletin of the Psychonomic Society, 23, 165–168. Eriksen, C. W., Coles, M. G. H., Morris, L. R., & O’Hara, W. P. (1985). An electromyographic examination of response competition. Bulletin of the Psychonomic Society, 23, 165–168.
go back to reference Fitts, P., & Seeger, C. M. (1953). SR compatibility: Spatial characteristics of stimulus and response codes. Journal of Experimental Psychology, 46, 199–210.PubMedCrossRef Fitts, P., & Seeger, C. M. (1953). SR compatibility: Spatial characteristics of stimulus and response codes. Journal of Experimental Psychology, 46, 199–210.PubMedCrossRef
go back to reference Geller, E. S. (1974). Preceding prediction outcome and prediction outcome probability: interacting determinants of choice reaction time. Journal of Experimental Psychology, 103, 426–430.CrossRef Geller, E. S. (1974). Preceding prediction outcome and prediction outcome probability: interacting determinants of choice reaction time. Journal of Experimental Psychology, 103, 426–430.CrossRef
go back to reference Geller, E. S. (1975). Prediction outcome and choice reaction time: inhibition versus facilitation effects. Acta Psychologica, 39, 69–82.CrossRef Geller, E. S. (1975). Prediction outcome and choice reaction time: inhibition versus facilitation effects. Acta Psychologica, 39, 69–82.CrossRef
go back to reference Grison, S., Tipper, S. P., & Hewitt, O. (2005). Long-term negative priming: support for a retrieval of prior attentional processes. Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology: Section A, (in press). Grison, S., Tipper, S. P., & Hewitt, O. (2005). Long-term negative priming: support for a retrieval of prior attentional processes. Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology: Section A, (in press).
go back to reference Guy, S., & Buckolz, E. (2004). Cueing the response in a location negative priming task: Locus implications. Presented to the North American Society for the Psychology of Sport and Physical Activity, Vancouver, British Columbia, Canada. (June) Guy, S., & Buckolz, E. (2004). Cueing the response in a location negative priming task: Locus implications. Presented to the North American Society for the Psychology of Sport and Physical Activity, Vancouver, British Columbia, Canada. (June)
go back to reference Guy, S., Buckolz, E., & Pratt, J. (2004). The influence of distractor-only prime trials on the location negative priming mechanism. Experimental Psychology, 51, 1–11.CrossRef Guy, S., Buckolz, E., & Pratt, J. (2004). The influence of distractor-only prime trials on the location negative priming mechanism. Experimental Psychology, 51, 1–11.CrossRef
go back to reference Guy, S., Buckolz, E. & Khan, M. (2005). The locus of location repetition latency effects. Manuscript under review. Guy, S., Buckolz, E. & Khan, M. (2005). The locus of location repetition latency effects. Manuscript under review.
go back to reference Houghton, G., & Tipper, S. P. (1994). A model of inhibitory mechanisms in selective attention. In Inhibitory mechanisms in attention, memory, and language (ed. D. Dagenbach & T. Carr), pp. 53–112. Orlando, FL: Academic Press. Houghton, G., & Tipper, S. P. (1994). A model of inhibitory mechanisms in selective attention. In Inhibitory mechanisms in attention, memory, and language (ed. D. Dagenbach & T. Carr), pp. 53–112. Orlando, FL: Academic Press.
go back to reference Keele, S. (1969). Repetition effect: A memory dependent process. Journal of Experimental Psychology, 80, 243–248.CrossRef Keele, S. (1969). Repetition effect: A memory dependent process. Journal of Experimental Psychology, 80, 243–248.CrossRef
go back to reference Keele, S. W. (1973). Attention and human performance. Pacific Palisades, California: Goodyear. Keele, S. W. (1973). Attention and human performance. Pacific Palisades, California: Goodyear.
go back to reference Milliken, B., Tipper, S. P., Houghton, G., & Lupianez, J. (2000). Attending, ignoring, and repetition: On the relation between negative priming and inhibition of return. Perception & Psychophysics, 62, 1289–1296. Milliken, B., Tipper, S. P., Houghton, G., & Lupianez, J. (2000). Attending, ignoring, and repetition: On the relation between negative priming and inhibition of return. Perception & Psychophysics, 62, 1289–1296.
go back to reference Neill, W. T., Terry, K. M., & Valdes, L. A. (1994). Negative priming without probe selection. Psychonomic Bulletin and Review, 1, 119–121. Neill, W. T., Terry, K. M., & Valdes, L. A. (1994). Negative priming without probe selection. Psychonomic Bulletin and Review, 1, 119–121.
go back to reference Neill, W. T., Valdes, L. A., & Terry, K. M. (1995). Selective attention and the inhibitory control of cognition. In Frank M. Dempster & Charles J. Brainerd (Eds.), Interference and Inhibition in Cognition. Academic Press: New York (pp. 207–261). Neill, W. T., Valdes, L. A., & Terry, K. M. (1995). Selective attention and the inhibitory control of cognition. In Frank M. Dempster & Charles J. Brainerd (Eds.), Interference and Inhibition in Cognition. Academic Press: New York (pp. 207–261).
go back to reference Tipper, S. P. (1985). The negative priming effect: Inhibitory priming by ignored objects. Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology, 37(A), 571–590.PubMed Tipper, S. P. (1985). The negative priming effect: Inhibitory priming by ignored objects. Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology, 37(A), 571–590.PubMed
go back to reference Tipper, S. P. (2001). Does negative priming reflect inhibitory mechanisms? A review and integration of conflicting views. The Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology, 54A, 321–343.PubMedCrossRef Tipper, S. P. (2001). Does negative priming reflect inhibitory mechanisms? A review and integration of conflicting views. The Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology, 54A, 321–343.PubMedCrossRef
go back to reference Tipper, S. P., & Cranston, M. (1985). Selective attention and priming: Inhibitory and facilitatory effects of ignore primes. Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology, 37A, 581–611. Tipper, S. P., & Cranston, M. (1985). Selective attention and priming: Inhibitory and facilitatory effects of ignore primes. Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology, 37A, 581–611.
go back to reference Tipper, S. P., Brehaut, J. C., & Driver, J. (1990). Selection of moving and static objects for the control of spatially directed action. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 16, 492–504.PubMedCrossRef Tipper, S. P., Brehaut, J. C., & Driver, J. (1990). Selection of moving and static objects for the control of spatially directed action. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 16, 492–504.PubMedCrossRef
go back to reference Tipper, S. W., Weaver, B., Cameron, S., Brehaut, J., & Bastedo, J. (1991). Inhibitory mechanisms of attention in identification and localization tasks: Time course and disruption. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition, 17, 681–692.PubMedCrossRef Tipper, S. W., Weaver, B., Cameron, S., Brehaut, J., & Bastedo, J. (1991). Inhibitory mechanisms of attention in identification and localization tasks: Time course and disruption. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition, 17, 681–692.PubMedCrossRef
go back to reference Valle-Inclan, F., & Redondo, M. (1998). On the automaticity of ipsilateral response activation in the Simon effect. Psychophysiology, 35, 366–371.PubMedCrossRef Valle-Inclan, F., & Redondo, M. (1998). On the automaticity of ipsilateral response activation in the Simon effect. Psychophysiology, 35, 366–371.PubMedCrossRef
go back to reference Whitman, C. P. & Geller, E. S. (1974). Prediction outcome probabilities as determinants of choice reaction time. Perception & Psychophysics, 13, 105–109. Whitman, C. P. & Geller, E. S. (1974). Prediction outcome probabilities as determinants of choice reaction time. Perception & Psychophysics, 13, 105–109.
Metagegevens
Titel
The locus and modulation of the location negative priming effect
Auteurs
Sarah Guy
Eric Buckolz
Publicatiedatum
01-03-2007
Uitgeverij
Springer-Verlag
Gepubliceerd in
Psychological Research / Uitgave 2/2007
Print ISSN: 0340-0727
Elektronisch ISSN: 1430-2772
DOI
https://doi.org/10.1007/s00426-005-0003-9

Andere artikelen Uitgave 2/2007

Psychological Research 2/2007 Naar de uitgave