Skip to main content
Top
Gepubliceerd in: Psychological Research 3/2018

20-01-2017 | Original Article

Numerical magnitude affects online execution, and not planning of visuomotor control

Auteurs: Gal Namdar, Tzvi Ganel

Gepubliceerd in: Psychological Research | Uitgave 3/2018

Log in om toegang te krijgen
share
DELEN

Deel dit onderdeel of sectie (kopieer de link)

  • Optie A:
    Klik op de rechtermuisknop op de link en selecteer de optie “linkadres kopiëren”
  • Optie B:
    Deel de link per e-mail

Abstract

Recent literature has established a directional influence of irrelevant numerical magnitude on actions performed toward neutral objects. For example, fingers’ aperture during grasping is larger when associated with large compared with small numerical digits. This interaction between symbolic magnitude and visuomotor control has been attributed to the planning stage of the action prior to motor execution. However, this assumption has not been directly tested. In two experiments, we tested whether the effects of numerical magnitude on grasping derive from action planning or from action execution. Participants were asked to grasp an object following a short visual (Experiment 1) or auditory (Experiment 2) presentation of small (1/2) or large (8/9) digits. Grasping was performed under either closed-loop (CL) or open-loop (OL) visuomotor control, for which online vision was prevented during action execution. Digit magnitude affected grip apertures in the CL condition, when online vision was allowed. However, magnitude had no effects on grip aperture in the OL condition. This pattern of results strongly suggests that the processing of numerical magnitude originates from interactions between numerical magnitude and real object size during online motor execution. Unlike previously assumed, the findings also suggest that the effect of magnitude on visuomotor control is not likely to be attributed to the motor planning stage prior to action initiation.
Literatuur
go back to reference Alards-Tomalin, D., Walker, A. C., Shaw, J. D. M., & Leboe-McGowan, L. C. (2015). Is 9 louder than 1? Audiovisual cross-modal interactions between number magnitude and judged sound loudness. Acta Psychologica, 160, 95–103.CrossRefPubMed Alards-Tomalin, D., Walker, A. C., Shaw, J. D. M., & Leboe-McGowan, L. C. (2015). Is 9 louder than 1? Audiovisual cross-modal interactions between number magnitude and judged sound loudness. Acta Psychologica, 160, 95–103.CrossRefPubMed
go back to reference Andres, M., Ostry, D. J., Nicol, F., & Paus, T. (2008). Time course of number magnitude interference during grasping. Cortex; a journal devoted to the study of the nervous system and behavior, 44(4), 414–419.CrossRefPubMed Andres, M., Ostry, D. J., Nicol, F., & Paus, T. (2008). Time course of number magnitude interference during grasping. Cortex; a journal devoted to the study of the nervous system and behavior, 44(4), 414–419.CrossRefPubMed
go back to reference Badets, A., Andres, M., Di Luca, S., & Pesenti, M. (2007). Number magnitude potentiates action judgements. Experimental Brain Research, 180(3), 525–534.CrossRefPubMed Badets, A., Andres, M., Di Luca, S., & Pesenti, M. (2007). Number magnitude potentiates action judgements. Experimental Brain Research, 180(3), 525–534.CrossRefPubMed
go back to reference Badets, A., & Pesenti, M. (2011). Finger-number interaction. Experimental Psychology, 58(4), 287–292.CrossRefPubMed Badets, A., & Pesenti, M. (2011). Finger-number interaction. Experimental Psychology, 58(4), 287–292.CrossRefPubMed
go back to reference Binetti, N., Hagura, N., Fadipe, C., Tomassini, A., Walsh, V., & Bestmann, S. (2015). Binding space and time through action. Proceedings of the Royal Society of London B: Biological Sciences, 282(1805), 20150381. Binetti, N., Hagura, N., Fadipe, C., Tomassini, A., Walsh, V., & Bestmann, S. (2015). Binding space and time through action. Proceedings of the Royal Society of London B: Biological Sciences, 282(1805), 20150381.
go back to reference Cant, J. S., Westwood, D. A., Valyear, K. F., & Goodale, M. A. (2005). No evidence for visuomotor priming in a visually guided action task. Neuropsychologia, 43(2), 216–226.CrossRefPubMed Cant, J. S., Westwood, D. A., Valyear, K. F., & Goodale, M. A. (2005). No evidence for visuomotor priming in a visually guided action task. Neuropsychologia, 43(2), 216–226.CrossRefPubMed
go back to reference Castelli, F., Glaser, D. E., & Butterworth, B. (2006). Discrete and analogue quantity processing in the parietal lobe: A functional MRI study. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America, 103(12), 4693–4698.CrossRefPubMedPubMedCentral Castelli, F., Glaser, D. E., & Butterworth, B. (2006). Discrete and analogue quantity processing in the parietal lobe: A functional MRI study. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America, 103(12), 4693–4698.CrossRefPubMedPubMedCentral
go back to reference Chiou, R. Y.-C., Chang, E. C., Tzeng, O. J.-L., & Wu, D. H. (2009). The common magnitude code underlying numerical and size processing for action but not for perception. Experimental Brain Research, 194(4), 553–562.CrossRefPubMed Chiou, R. Y.-C., Chang, E. C., Tzeng, O. J.-L., & Wu, D. H. (2009). The common magnitude code underlying numerical and size processing for action but not for perception. Experimental Brain Research, 194(4), 553–562.CrossRefPubMed
go back to reference Chiou, R. Y.-C., Wu, D. H., Tzeng, O. J.-L., Hung, D. L., & Chang, E. C. (2012). Relative size of numerical magnitude induces a size-contrast effect on the grip scaling of reach-to-grasp movements. Cortex; a journal devoted to the study of the nervous system and behavior, 48(8), 1043–1051.CrossRefPubMed Chiou, R. Y.-C., Wu, D. H., Tzeng, O. J.-L., Hung, D. L., & Chang, E. C. (2012). Relative size of numerical magnitude induces a size-contrast effect on the grip scaling of reach-to-grasp movements. Cortex; a journal devoted to the study of the nervous system and behavior, 48(8), 1043–1051.CrossRefPubMed
go back to reference Dormal, V., & Pesenti, M. (2012). Processing magnitudes within the parietal cortex. Horizons in Neuroscience Research, 8, 107–140. Dormal, V., & Pesenti, M. (2012). Processing magnitudes within the parietal cortex. Horizons in Neuroscience Research, 8, 107–140.
go back to reference Faul, F., Erdfelder, E., Lang, A. G., & Buchner, A. (2007). G* Power 3: A flexible statistical power analysis program for the social, behavioral, and biomedical sciences. Behavior research methods, 39(2), 175–191.CrossRefPubMed Faul, F., Erdfelder, E., Lang, A. G., & Buchner, A. (2007). G* Power 3: A flexible statistical power analysis program for the social, behavioral, and biomedical sciences. Behavior research methods, 39(2), 175–191.CrossRefPubMed
go back to reference Fias, W., Lammertyn, J., Reynvoet, B., Dupont, P., & Orban, G. A. (2003). Parietal representation of symbolic and nonsymbolic magnitude. Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience, 15(1), 47–56.CrossRefPubMed Fias, W., Lammertyn, J., Reynvoet, B., Dupont, P., & Orban, G. A. (2003). Parietal representation of symbolic and nonsymbolic magnitude. Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience, 15(1), 47–56.CrossRefPubMed
go back to reference Franz, V. H., Hesse, C., & Kollath, S. (2009). Visual illusions, delayed grasping, and memory: no shift from dorsal to ventral control. Neuropsychologia, 47(6), 1518–1531.CrossRefPubMed Franz, V. H., Hesse, C., & Kollath, S. (2009). Visual illusions, delayed grasping, and memory: no shift from dorsal to ventral control. Neuropsychologia, 47(6), 1518–1531.CrossRefPubMed
go back to reference Frey, S. H., Vinton, D., Norlund, R., & Grafton, S. T. (2005). Cortical topography of human anterior intraparietal cortex active during visually guided grasping. Cognitive Brain Research, 23(2–3), 397–405.CrossRefPubMed Frey, S. H., Vinton, D., Norlund, R., & Grafton, S. T. (2005). Cortical topography of human anterior intraparietal cortex active during visually guided grasping. Cognitive Brain Research, 23(2–3), 397–405.CrossRefPubMed
go back to reference Gentilucci, M. (2003). Object motor representation and language. Experimental Brain Research, 153(2), 260–265.CrossRefPubMed Gentilucci, M. (2003). Object motor representation and language. Experimental Brain Research, 153(2), 260–265.CrossRefPubMed
go back to reference Gentilucci, M., Benuzzi, F., Bertolani, L., Daprati, E., & Gangitano, M. (2000). Language and motor control. Experimental Brain Research, 133(4), 468–490.CrossRefPubMed Gentilucci, M., Benuzzi, F., Bertolani, L., Daprati, E., & Gangitano, M. (2000). Language and motor control. Experimental Brain Research, 133(4), 468–490.CrossRefPubMed
go back to reference Gentilucci, M., & Gangitano, M. (1998). Influence of automatic word reading on motor control. European Journal of Neuroscience, 10(2), 752–756.CrossRefPubMed Gentilucci, M., & Gangitano, M. (1998). Influence of automatic word reading on motor control. European Journal of Neuroscience, 10(2), 752–756.CrossRefPubMed
go back to reference Gianelli, C., Ranzini, M., Marzocchi, M., Micheli, L. R., & Borghi, A. M. (2012). Influence of numerical magnitudes on the free choice of an object position. Cognitive Processing, 13(1), 185–188.CrossRef Gianelli, C., Ranzini, M., Marzocchi, M., Micheli, L. R., & Borghi, A. M. (2012). Influence of numerical magnitudes on the free choice of an object position. Cognitive Processing, 13(1), 185–188.CrossRef
go back to reference Glover, S. (2002). Visual illusions affect planning but not control. Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 6(7), 288–292.CrossRefPubMed Glover, S. (2002). Visual illusions affect planning but not control. Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 6(7), 288–292.CrossRefPubMed
go back to reference Glover, S. (2004). Separate visual representations in the planning and control of action. Behavioral and Brain Sciences, 27(01), 3–24.PubMed Glover, S. (2004). Separate visual representations in the planning and control of action. Behavioral and Brain Sciences, 27(01), 3–24.PubMed
go back to reference Glover, S., & Dixon, P. (2001a). Motor adaptation to an optical illusion. Experimental Brain Research, 137(2), 254–258. Glover, S., & Dixon, P. (2001a). Motor adaptation to an optical illusion. Experimental Brain Research, 137(2), 254–258.
go back to reference Glover, S., & Dixon, P. (2001b). The role of vision in the on-line correction of illusion effects on action. Canadian Journal of Experimental Psychology/Revue Canadienne de Psychologie Expérimentale, 55(2), 96. Glover, S., & Dixon, P. (2001b). The role of vision in the on-line correction of illusion effects on action. Canadian Journal of Experimental Psychology/Revue Canadienne de Psychologie Expérimentale, 55(2), 96.
go back to reference Glover, S., & Dixon, P. (2002a). Dynamic effects of the Ebbinghaus illusion in grasping: support for a planning/control model of action. Perception & Psychophysics, 64(2), 266–278. Glover, S., & Dixon, P. (2002a). Dynamic effects of the Ebbinghaus illusion in grasping: support for a planning/control model of action. Perception & Psychophysics, 64(2), 266–278.
go back to reference Glover, S., & Dixon, P. (2002b). Semantics affect the planning but not control of grasping. Experimental Brain Research, 146(3), 383–387. Glover, S., & Dixon, P. (2002b). Semantics affect the planning but not control of grasping. Experimental Brain Research, 146(3), 383–387.
go back to reference Glover, S., Rosenbaum, D. A., Graham, J., & Dixon, P. (2003). Grasping the meaning of words. Experimental Brain Research, 154(1), 103–108.CrossRefPubMed Glover, S., Rosenbaum, D. A., Graham, J., & Dixon, P. (2003). Grasping the meaning of words. Experimental Brain Research, 154(1), 103–108.CrossRefPubMed
go back to reference Glover, S. R., & Dixon, P. (2001c). Dynamic illusion effects in a reaching task: Evidence for separate visual representations in the planning and control of reaching. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 27(3), 560–572. Glover, S. R., & Dixon, P. (2001c). Dynamic illusion effects in a reaching task: Evidence for separate visual representations in the planning and control of reaching. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 27(3), 560–572.
go back to reference Goodale, M. A., & Ganel, T. (2016). Different modes of visual organization for perception and for action. In J. Wagemans (Ed.), The Oxford handbook of perceptual organization (pp. 672–690). Oxford: Oxford University Press. Goodale, M. A., & Ganel, T. (2016). Different modes of visual organization for perception and for action. In J. Wagemans (Ed.), The Oxford handbook of perceptual organization (pp. 672–690). Oxford: Oxford University Press.
go back to reference Goodale, M. A., Jakobson, L. S., & Keillor, J. M. (1994). Differences in the visual control of pantomimed and natural grasping movements. Neuropsychologia, 32(10), 1159–1178.CrossRefPubMed Goodale, M. A., Jakobson, L. S., & Keillor, J. M. (1994). Differences in the visual control of pantomimed and natural grasping movements. Neuropsychologia, 32(10), 1159–1178.CrossRefPubMed
go back to reference Goodale, M. A., & Milner, A. D. (1992). Separate visual pathways for perception and action. Trends in Neurosciences, 15(1), 20–25.CrossRefPubMed Goodale, M. A., & Milner, A. D. (1992). Separate visual pathways for perception and action. Trends in Neurosciences, 15(1), 20–25.CrossRefPubMed
go back to reference Heath, M., Rival, C., Westwood, D. A., & Neely, K. (2005). Time Course Analysis of Closed- and Open-Loop Grasping of the Müller-Lyer Illusion. Journal of Motor Behavior, 37(3), 179–185.CrossRefPubMed Heath, M., Rival, C., Westwood, D. A., & Neely, K. (2005). Time Course Analysis of Closed- and Open-Loop Grasping of the Müller-Lyer Illusion. Journal of Motor Behavior, 37(3), 179–185.CrossRefPubMed
go back to reference Hesse, C., & Franz, V. H. (2009). Memory mechanisms in grasping. Neuropsychologia, 47(6), 1532–1545.CrossRefPubMed Hesse, C., & Franz, V. H. (2009). Memory mechanisms in grasping. Neuropsychologia, 47(6), 1532–1545.CrossRefPubMed
go back to reference Hu, Y., & Goodale, M. A. (2000). Grasping after a delay shifts size-scaling from absolute to relative metrics. Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience, 12(5), 856–868.CrossRefPubMed Hu, Y., & Goodale, M. A. (2000). Grasping after a delay shifts size-scaling from absolute to relative metrics. Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience, 12(5), 856–868.CrossRefPubMed
go back to reference Jeannerod, M. (1988). The neural and behavioural organization of goal-directed movements (Vol. xii). New York: Clarendon Press/Oxford University Press. Jeannerod, M. (1988). The neural and behavioural organization of goal-directed movements (Vol. xii). New York: Clarendon Press/Oxford University Press.
go back to reference Jeannerod, M., Arbib, M. A., Rizzolatti, G., & Sakata, H. (1995). Grasping objects: The cortical mechanisms of visuomotor transformation. Trends in Neurosciences, 18(7), 314–320.CrossRefPubMed Jeannerod, M., Arbib, M. A., Rizzolatti, G., & Sakata, H. (1995). Grasping objects: The cortical mechanisms of visuomotor transformation. Trends in Neurosciences, 18(7), 314–320.CrossRefPubMed
go back to reference Kadosh, R. C., Henik, A., Rubinsten, O., Mohr, H., Dori, H., van de Ven, V., et al. (2005). Are numbers special?: The comparison systems of the human brain investigated by fMRI. Neuropsychologia, 43(9), 1238–1248.CrossRef Kadosh, R. C., Henik, A., Rubinsten, O., Mohr, H., Dori, H., van de Ven, V., et al. (2005). Are numbers special?: The comparison systems of the human brain investigated by fMRI. Neuropsychologia, 43(9), 1238–1248.CrossRef
go back to reference Lindemann, O., Abolafia, J. M., Girardi, G., & Bekkering, H. (2007). Getting a grip on numbers: numerical magnitude priming in object grasping. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 33(6), 1400.PubMed Lindemann, O., Abolafia, J. M., Girardi, G., & Bekkering, H. (2007). Getting a grip on numbers: numerical magnitude priming in object grasping. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 33(6), 1400.PubMed
go back to reference Moretto, G., & Pellegrino, G. di (2008). Grasping numbers. Experimental Brain Research, 188(4), 505–515.CrossRefPubMed Moretto, G., & Pellegrino, G. di (2008). Grasping numbers. Experimental Brain Research, 188(4), 505–515.CrossRefPubMed
go back to reference Namdar, G., & Ganel, T. (2015). Cross-modal effects of auditory magnitude on visually guided grasping. Journal of Vision, 15(8), 2–2.CrossRefPubMed Namdar, G., & Ganel, T. (2015). Cross-modal effects of auditory magnitude on visually guided grasping. Journal of Vision, 15(8), 2–2.CrossRefPubMed
go back to reference Namdar, G., Tzelgov, J., Algom, D., & Ganel, T. (2014). Grasping numbers: evidence for automatic influence of numerical magnitude on grip aperture. Psychonomic Bulletin & Review, 21(3), 830–835.CrossRef Namdar, G., Tzelgov, J., Algom, D., & Ganel, T. (2014). Grasping numbers: evidence for automatic influence of numerical magnitude on grip aperture. Psychonomic Bulletin & Review, 21(3), 830–835.CrossRef
go back to reference Rinaldi, L., Lega, C., Cattaneo, Z., Girelli, L., & Bernardi, N. F. (2016). Grasping the sound: Auditory pitch influences size processing in motor planning. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 42(1), 11–22.PubMed Rinaldi, L., Lega, C., Cattaneo, Z., Girelli, L., & Bernardi, N. F. (2016). Grasping the sound: Auditory pitch influences size processing in motor planning. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 42(1), 11–22.PubMed
go back to reference Seegelke, C., Güldenpenning, I., Dettling, J., & Schack, T. (2016). Visuomotor priming of action preparation and motor programming is similar in visually guided and memory-guided actions. Neuropsychologia. Seegelke, C., Güldenpenning, I., Dettling, J., & Schack, T. (2016). Visuomotor priming of action preparation and motor programming is similar in visually guided and memory-guided actions. Neuropsychologia.
go back to reference Walsh, V. (2003). A theory of magnitude: common cortical metrics of time, space and quantity. Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 7(11), 483–488.CrossRefPubMed Walsh, V. (2003). A theory of magnitude: common cortical metrics of time, space and quantity. Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 7(11), 483–488.CrossRefPubMed
go back to reference Westwood, D. A., & Goodale, M. A. (2003). A haptic size-contrast illusion affects size perception but not grasping. Experimental Brain Research, 153(2), 253–259.CrossRefPubMed Westwood, D. A., & Goodale, M. A. (2003). A haptic size-contrast illusion affects size perception but not grasping. Experimental Brain Research, 153(2), 253–259.CrossRefPubMed
go back to reference Westwood, D. A., Heath, M., & Roy, E. A. (2000). The effect of a pictorial illusion on closed-loop and open-loop prehension. Experimental Brain Research, 134(4), 456–463.CrossRefPubMed Westwood, D. A., Heath, M., & Roy, E. A. (2000). The effect of a pictorial illusion on closed-loop and open-loop prehension. Experimental Brain Research, 134(4), 456–463.CrossRefPubMed
go back to reference Westwood, D. A., Heath, M., & Roy, E. A. (2003). No evidence for accurate visuomotor memory: Systematic and variable error in memory-guided reaching. Journal of Motor Behavior, 35(2), 127–133.CrossRefPubMed Westwood, D. A., Heath, M., & Roy, E. A. (2003). No evidence for accurate visuomotor memory: Systematic and variable error in memory-guided reaching. Journal of Motor Behavior, 35(2), 127–133.CrossRefPubMed
go back to reference Westwood, D. A., McEachern, T., & Roy, E. A. (2001). Delayed grasping of a Müller-Lyer figure. Experimental Brain Research, 141(2), 166–173.CrossRefPubMed Westwood, D. A., McEachern, T., & Roy, E. A. (2001). Delayed grasping of a Müller-Lyer figure. Experimental Brain Research, 141(2), 166–173.CrossRefPubMed
Metagegevens
Titel
Numerical magnitude affects online execution, and not planning of visuomotor control
Auteurs
Gal Namdar
Tzvi Ganel
Publicatiedatum
20-01-2017
Uitgeverij
Springer Berlin Heidelberg
Gepubliceerd in
Psychological Research / Uitgave 3/2018
Print ISSN: 0340-0727
Elektronisch ISSN: 1430-2772
DOI
https://doi.org/10.1007/s00426-016-0837-3

Andere artikelen Uitgave 3/2018

Psychological Research 3/2018 Naar de uitgave