Skip to main content
Top
Gepubliceerd in: Psychological Research 3/2018

01-03-2017 | Original Article

Delta plots with negative-going slopes as a potential marker of decreasing response activation in masked semantic priming

Auteurs: Ruben Ellinghaus, Jeff Miller

Gepubliceerd in: Psychological Research | Uitgave 3/2018

Log in om toegang te krijgen
share
DELEN

Deel dit onderdeel of sectie (kopieer de link)

  • Optie A:
    Klik op de rechtermuisknop op de link en selecteer de optie “linkadres kopiëren”
  • Optie B:
    Deel de link per e-mail

Abstract

Delta plots with negative-going slopes (nDPs) reflect the phenomenon that an RT difference between two conditions is greater for relatively fast than for relatively slow responses. This unusual distributional pattern has predominantly been observed in the spatial Simon task, where it has been interpreted as reflecting the selective inhibition of an automatically activated response. The literature suggesting that a similar fading mechanism influences RTs in masked identity priming inspired us to check an analogous semantic priming paradigm for nDPs. Consistent with the findings in other paradigms, two masked semantic priming experiments revealed stronger priming effects for relatively fast than for relatively slow responses, thus reflecting an nDP. These findings are compatible with the ideas that the activation produced by masked semantic primes decreases over the course of a trial, such as that of irrelevant spatial information and of masked identity primes, and that nDPs are a general signature of within-trial decreases in response activation across different tasks and paradigms.
Voetnoten
1
As an alternative DP analysis, we carried out a within-subjects ANOVA on RT with RT Bin and congruency as factors. The decrease of the PCE across bins was reflected in the significant interaction, F(4,60) = 4.75, p = .002 However, in this case the choice of bin number potentially influences the outcome strongly, since the sum of squares for the overall effect is divided by the number of bins (minus one), so the mean square for the interaction tends to decrease as the number of bins increases. With the present data we found that the interaction was also significant when the ANOVA was run with 3 bins, 10 bins, or 20 bins, F(2,30) = 5.14, p = .012, F(9,135) = 3.35, p = .002, F(19,285) = 2.97, p < .001, respectively. Furthermore, the reported t test on the slopes was significant not only with 5 bins but also with 3 bins, 10 bins or 20 bins, t(15) = 2.46, p = .026, t(15) = 2.3, p = .037, t(15) = 2.20, p = .043, respectively.
 
2
For DP analysis, we also carried out a three-factor repeated measures ANOVA with SOA, congruency, and RT bin as factors and RT as dependent variable. The main effect of congruency reached significance, F(1,44) = 62.90, p < .001. As one expects, the main effect of bin was highly significant, too, F(4,176) = 664.20, p < .001. Most importantly, the interaction between congruency and bin was highly significant, F(4,176) = 11.17, p < .001, reflecting the expected decrease of the congruency effect across RT bins (averaged across the two SOAs) and thereby replicating the nDP in the data of Ellinghaus (2015). The decrease was significantly steeper for the long SOA compared to the short SOA, as was reflected in the three-way interaction of congruency, bin and SOA, F(4, 176) = 4.63, p = .022.
 
Literatuur
go back to reference Atas, A., & Cleeremans, A. (2015). The temporal dynamic of automatic inhibition of irrelevant actions. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 41, 289–305.PubMed Atas, A., & Cleeremans, A. (2015). The temporal dynamic of automatic inhibition of irrelevant actions. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 41, 289–305.PubMed
go back to reference Balota, D. A., Yap, M. J., Cortese, M. J., & Watson, J. M. (2008). Beyond mean response latency: Response time distributional analyses of semantic priming. Journal of Memory and Language, 59, 495–523.CrossRef Balota, D. A., Yap, M. J., Cortese, M. J., & Watson, J. M. (2008). Beyond mean response latency: Response time distributional analyses of semantic priming. Journal of Memory and Language, 59, 495–523.CrossRef
go back to reference Brainard, D. H. (1997). The Psychophysics Toolbox. Spatial Vision, 10(4), 433–436. Brainard, D. H. (1997). The Psychophysics Toolbox. Spatial Vision, 10(4), 433–436.
go back to reference Burle, B., Possamaï, C. A., Vidal, F., Bonnet, M., & Hasbroucq, T. (2002). Executive control in the Simon effect: an electromyographic and distributional analysis. Psychological Research Psychologische Forschung, 66, 324–336. doi:10.1007/s00426-002-0105-6.CrossRefPubMed Burle, B., Possamaï, C. A., Vidal, F., Bonnet, M., & Hasbroucq, T. (2002). Executive control in the Simon effect: an electromyographic and distributional analysis. Psychological Research Psychologische Forschung, 66, 324–336. doi:10.​1007/​s00426-002-0105-6.CrossRefPubMed
go back to reference De Jong, R., Liang, C. C., & Lauber, E. (1994). Conditional and unconditional automaticity: a dual-process model of effects of spatial stimulus-response correspondence. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 20, 731–750.PubMed De Jong, R., Liang, C. C., & Lauber, E. (1994). Conditional and unconditional automaticity: a dual-process model of effects of spatial stimulus-response correspondence. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 20, 731–750.PubMed
go back to reference De Wit, B., & Kinoshita, S. (2014a). Relatedness proportion effects in semantic categorization: reconsidering the automatic spreading activation process. Journal of Experimental Psychology-Learning Memory and Cognition, 40(6), 1733–1744.CrossRef De Wit, B., & Kinoshita, S. (2014a). Relatedness proportion effects in semantic categorization: reconsidering the automatic spreading activation process. Journal of Experimental Psychology-Learning Memory and Cognition, 40(6), 1733–1744.CrossRef
go back to reference De Wit, B., & Kinoshita, S. (2014b). An RT distribution analysis of relatedness proportion effects in lexical decision and semantic categorization reveals different mechanisms. Memory & Cognition, 99–110. De Wit, B., & Kinoshita, S. (2014b). An RT distribution analysis of relatedness proportion effects in lexical decision and semantic categorization reveals different mechanisms. Memory & Cognition, 99–110.
go back to reference De Wit, B., & Kinoshita, S. (2015). The masked semantic priming effect is task dependent: Reconsidering the automatic spreading activation process. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition, 41(4), 1062–1075.PubMed De Wit, B., & Kinoshita, S. (2015). The masked semantic priming effect is task dependent: Reconsidering the automatic spreading activation process. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition, 41(4), 1062–1075.PubMed
go back to reference Debey, E., Ridderinkhof, R. K., De Houwer, J., De Schryver, M., & Verschuere, B. (2015). Suppressing the truth as a mechanism of deception: Delta plots reveal the role of response inhibition in lying. Consciousness and Cognition, 37, 148–159.CrossRefPubMed Debey, E., Ridderinkhof, R. K., De Houwer, J., De Schryver, M., & Verschuere, B. (2015). Suppressing the truth as a mechanism of deception: Delta plots reveal the role of response inhibition in lying. Consciousness and Cognition, 37, 148–159.CrossRefPubMed
go back to reference Donders, F. C. (1969/1868). On the speed of mental processes. Acta Psychologica, 30, 412–431. Donders, F. C. (1969/1868). On the speed of mental processes. Acta Psychologica, 30, 412–431.
go back to reference Eimer, M. (1999). Facilitatory and inhibitory effects of masked prime stimuli on motor activation and behavioural performance. Acta Psychologica, 101, 293–313.CrossRefPubMed Eimer, M. (1999). Facilitatory and inhibitory effects of masked prime stimuli on motor activation and behavioural performance. Acta Psychologica, 101, 293–313.CrossRefPubMed
go back to reference Eimer, M., & Schlaghecken, F. (1998). Effects of masked stimuli on motor activation: behavioral and electrophysiological evidence. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 24, 1737–1747.PubMed Eimer, M., & Schlaghecken, F. (1998). Effects of masked stimuli on motor activation: behavioral and electrophysiological evidence. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 24, 1737–1747.PubMed
go back to reference Ellinghaus, R. (2015). The conditional automaticity of unconscious vision: Attentional sensitization of masked prime processing (Unpublished Master’s Thesis). Georg-August-Universität, Göttingen, Germany. Ellinghaus, R. (2015). The conditional automaticity of unconscious vision: Attentional sensitization of masked prime processing (Unpublished Master’s Thesis). Georg-August-Universität, Göttingen, Germany.
go back to reference Gomez, P., Perea, M., & Ratcliff, R. (2013). A diffusion model account of masked versus unmasked priming: are they qualitatively different? Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 39(6), 1731–1740.PubMedPubMedCentral Gomez, P., Perea, M., & Ratcliff, R. (2013). A diffusion model account of masked versus unmasked priming: are they qualitatively different? Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 39(6), 1731–1740.PubMedPubMedCentral
go back to reference Gratton, G., Coles, M. G., & Donchin, E. (1992). Optimizing the use of information: strategic control of activation of responses. Journal of Experimental Psychology: General, 121, 480–506.CrossRef Gratton, G., Coles, M. G., & Donchin, E. (1992). Optimizing the use of information: strategic control of activation of responses. Journal of Experimental Psychology: General, 121, 480–506.CrossRef
go back to reference Heathcote, A., Popiel, S. J., & Mewhort, D. J. (1991). Analysis of response time distributions: an example using the Stroop task. Psychological Bulletin, 109, 340–347.CrossRef Heathcote, A., Popiel, S. J., & Mewhort, D. J. (1991). Analysis of response time distributions: an example using the Stroop task. Psychological Bulletin, 109, 340–347.CrossRef
go back to reference Hutchison, K. A., Heap, S. J., Neely, J. H., & Thomas, M. A. (2014). Attentional control and asymmetric associative priming. Journal of Experimental Psychology. Learning, Memory, and Cognition, 40(3), 844–856.CrossRefPubMed Hutchison, K. A., Heap, S. J., Neely, J. H., & Thomas, M. A. (2014). Attentional control and asymmetric associative priming. Journal of Experimental Psychology. Learning, Memory, and Cognition, 40(3), 844–856.CrossRefPubMed
go back to reference Kleiner, M., Brainard, D.H., & Pelli, D.G. (2007). What's new in psychtoolbox-3? Perception, 36(14), 1. (36 ECVP Abstract Supplement). Kleiner, M., Brainard, D.H., & Pelli, D.G. (2007). What's new in psychtoolbox-3? Perception, 36(14), 1. (36 ECVP Abstract Supplement).
go back to reference Kouider, S., & Dehaene, S. (2007). Levels of processing during non-conscious perception: a critical review of visual masking. Philosophical Transactions of the Royal Society of London B: Biological Sciences, 362, 857–875.CrossRefPubMedPubMedCentral Kouider, S., & Dehaene, S. (2007). Levels of processing during non-conscious perception: a critical review of visual masking. Philosophical Transactions of the Royal Society of London B: Biological Sciences, 362, 857–875.CrossRefPubMedPubMedCentral
go back to reference Luce, R. D. (1986). Response times: Their role in inferring elementary mental organization. Oxford: Oxford University Press. Luce, R. D. (1986). Response times: Their role in inferring elementary mental organization. Oxford: Oxford University Press.
go back to reference Neely, J. H. (1977). Semantic priming and retrieval from lexical memory: Roles of inhibitionless spreading activation and limited-capacity attention. Journal of Experimental Psychology General, 106(3), 226–254.CrossRef Neely, J. H. (1977). Semantic priming and retrieval from lexical memory: Roles of inhibitionless spreading activation and limited-capacity attention. Journal of Experimental Psychology General, 106(3), 226–254.CrossRef
go back to reference Pelli, D.G. (1997). The VideoToolbox software for visual psychophysics: transforming numbers into movies. Spatial Vision, 10(4), 437–442. Pelli, D.G. (1997). The VideoToolbox software for visual psychophysics: transforming numbers into movies. Spatial Vision, 10(4), 437–442.
go back to reference Popper, K. R. (1959). The logic of scientific discovery. London: Hutchinson. Popper, K. R. (1959). The logic of scientific discovery. London: Hutchinson.
go back to reference Pratte, M. S., Rouder, J. N., Morey, R. D., & Feng, C. (2010). Exploring the differences in distributional properties between Stroop and Simon effects using delta plots. Attention, Perception, & Psychophysics, 72, 2013–2025.CrossRef Pratte, M. S., Rouder, J. N., Morey, R. D., & Feng, C. (2010). Exploring the differences in distributional properties between Stroop and Simon effects using delta plots. Attention, Perception, & Psychophysics, 72, 2013–2025.CrossRef
go back to reference Quinn, W. M., & Kinoshita, S. (2008). Congruence effect in semantic categorization with masked primes with narrow and broad categories. Journal of Memory and Language, 58(2), 286–306.CrossRef Quinn, W. M., & Kinoshita, S. (2008). Congruence effect in semantic categorization with masked primes with narrow and broad categories. Journal of Memory and Language, 58(2), 286–306.CrossRef
go back to reference Ridderinkhof, K. R., Scheres, A., Oosterlaan, J., & Sergeant, J. A. (2005). Delta plots in the study of individual differences: new tools reveal response inhibition deficits in AD/HD that are eliminated by methylphenidate treatment. Journal of Abnormal Psychology, 114, 197–215.CrossRefPubMed Ridderinkhof, K. R., Scheres, A., Oosterlaan, J., & Sergeant, J. A. (2005). Delta plots in the study of individual differences: new tools reveal response inhibition deficits in AD/HD that are eliminated by methylphenidate treatment. Journal of Abnormal Psychology, 114, 197–215.CrossRefPubMed
go back to reference Ridderinkhof, K. R., van den Wildenberg, W. P., Wijnen, J., & Burle, B. (2004). Response inhibition in conflict tasks is revealed in delta plots. In M. Posner (Ed.), Cognitive neuroscience of attention (pp. 369–377). New York: Guilford Press. Ridderinkhof, K. R., van den Wildenberg, W. P., Wijnen, J., & Burle, B. (2004). Response inhibition in conflict tasks is revealed in delta plots. In M. Posner (Ed.), Cognitive neuroscience of attention (pp. 369–377). New York: Guilford Press.
go back to reference Schlaghecken, F., Birak, K. S., & Maylor, E. A. (2011). Age-related deficits in low-level inhibitory motor control. Psychology and Aging, 26, 905–918.CrossRefPubMed Schlaghecken, F., Birak, K. S., & Maylor, E. A. (2011). Age-related deficits in low-level inhibitory motor control. Psychology and Aging, 26, 905–918.CrossRefPubMed
go back to reference Schlaghecken, F., & Eimer, M. (1997). The influence of subliminally presented primes on response preparation. Sprache & Kognition, 16, 166–175. Schlaghecken, F., & Eimer, M. (1997). The influence of subliminally presented primes on response preparation. Sprache & Kognition, 16, 166–175.
go back to reference Schwarz, W., & Miller, J. (2012). Response time models of delta plots with negative-going slopes. Psychonomic Bulletin & Review, 19, 555–574.CrossRef Schwarz, W., & Miller, J. (2012). Response time models of delta plots with negative-going slopes. Psychonomic Bulletin & Review, 19, 555–574.CrossRef
go back to reference Sternberg, S. (1969). The discovery of processing stages: extensions of Donders’ method. Acta Psychologica, 30, 276–315.CrossRef Sternberg, S. (1969). The discovery of processing stages: extensions of Donders’ method. Acta Psychologica, 30, 276–315.CrossRef
go back to reference Thomas, M. A., Neely, J. H., & O’Connor, P. (2012). When word identification gets tough, retrospective semantic processing comes to the rescue. Journal of Memory and Language, 66(4), 623–643.CrossRef Thomas, M. A., Neely, J. H., & O’Connor, P. (2012). When word identification gets tough, retrospective semantic processing comes to the rescue. Journal of Memory and Language, 66(4), 623–643.CrossRef
go back to reference Townsend, J. T., & Ashby, F. G. (1983). Stochastic modeling of elementary psychological processes. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press. Townsend, J. T., & Ashby, F. G. (1983). Stochastic modeling of elementary psychological processes. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.
go back to reference Ulrich, R., Schröter, H., Leuthold, H., & Birngruber, T. (2015). Automatic and controlled stimulus processing in conflict tasks: Superimposed diffusion processes and delta functions. Cognitive Psychology, 78, 148–174.CrossRefPubMed Ulrich, R., Schröter, H., Leuthold, H., & Birngruber, T. (2015). Automatic and controlled stimulus processing in conflict tasks: Superimposed diffusion processes and delta functions. Cognitive Psychology, 78, 148–174.CrossRefPubMed
go back to reference Vallesi, A., Mapelli, D., Schiff, S., Amodio, P., & Umilta, C. (2005). Horizontal and vertical Simon effect: different underlying mechanisms? Cognition, 96, B33–B43.CrossRefPubMed Vallesi, A., Mapelli, D., Schiff, S., Amodio, P., & Umilta, C. (2005). Horizontal and vertical Simon effect: different underlying mechanisms? Cognition, 96, B33–B43.CrossRefPubMed
go back to reference Wernicke, M. (2014). Approaching consciousness with masked priming (Doctoral dissertation). Georg-August-Universität, Göttingen, Germany. Wernicke, M. (2014). Approaching consciousness with masked priming (Doctoral dissertation). Georg-August-Universität, Göttingen, Germany.
go back to reference Xiong, A., & Proctor, R. W. (2016). Decreasing auditory Simon effects across reaction time distributions. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 42, 23–38.PubMed Xiong, A., & Proctor, R. W. (2016). Decreasing auditory Simon effects across reaction time distributions. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 42, 23–38.PubMed
go back to reference Zhang, J., & Kornblum, S. (1997). Distributional analysis and De Jong, Liang, and Lauber’s (1994) dual-process model of the Simon effect. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 23, 1543–1551. Zhang, J., & Kornblum, S. (1997). Distributional analysis and De Jong, Liang, and Lauber’s (1994) dual-process model of the Simon effect. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 23, 1543–1551.
Metagegevens
Titel
Delta plots with negative-going slopes as a potential marker of decreasing response activation in masked semantic priming
Auteurs
Ruben Ellinghaus
Jeff Miller
Publicatiedatum
01-03-2017
Uitgeverij
Springer Berlin Heidelberg
Gepubliceerd in
Psychological Research / Uitgave 3/2018
Print ISSN: 0340-0727
Elektronisch ISSN: 1430-2772
DOI
https://doi.org/10.1007/s00426-017-0844-z

Andere artikelen Uitgave 3/2018

Psychological Research 3/2018 Naar de uitgave