Skip to main content
Top
Gepubliceerd in: Psychological Research 2/2012

01-03-2012 | Review

Assimilation and contrast: the two sides of specific interference between action and perception

Auteurs: Jan Zwickel, Wolfgang Prinz

Gepubliceerd in: Psychological Research | Uitgave 2/2012

Log in om toegang te krijgen
share
DELEN

Deel dit onderdeel of sectie (kopieer de link)

  • Optie A:
    Klik op de rechtermuisknop op de link en selecteer de optie “linkadres kopiëren”
  • Optie B:
    Deel de link per e-mail

Abstract

Perception and action have long been treated as relatively independent and serial processes. More recent views, however, consider perception and action as relying on a common set of processes and/or representations. The present paper will focus on a variety of specific (content-based) perception–action interactions that have been taken as support for such views. In particular, the following aspects will be considered: direction of influence (perception on action vs. action on perception), temporal type (concurrent vs. non-concurrent), functional relation (related/unrelated), and type of movements (biological vs. non-biological). Different extant models of the perception-action interface are discussed and a classification schema proposed that tries to explain when contrast and when assimilation effects will arise.
Voetnoten
1
Attraction was found to far away distractors. This pattern was explained within a competitive model and is consistent with the current account, assuming that distance controls amount of overlap (see Zwickel et al. 2010a).
 
2
We thank an anonymous reviewer for raising this point.
 
Literatuur
go back to reference Beets, I. A. M., Rösler, F., & Fiehler, K. (2010). Nonvisual motor learning improves visual motion perception: Evidence from violating the two-thirds power law. Journal of Neurophysiology, 104(3), 1612–1624.PubMedCrossRef Beets, I. A. M., Rösler, F., & Fiehler, K. (2010). Nonvisual motor learning improves visual motion perception: Evidence from violating the two-thirds power law. Journal of Neurophysiology, 104(3), 1612–1624.PubMedCrossRef
go back to reference Beets, I. A. M., ’t Hart, B. M., Rösler, F., Henriques, D. Y. P., Einhäuser, W., & Fiehler, K. (2010). Online action-to-perception transfer: Only percept-dependent action affects perception. Vision Research, 50(24), 2633–2641.PubMedCrossRef Beets, I. A. M., ’t Hart, B. M., Rösler, F., Henriques, D. Y. P., Einhäuser, W., & Fiehler, K. (2010). Online action-to-perception transfer: Only percept-dependent action affects perception. Vision Research, 50(24), 2633–2641.PubMedCrossRef
go back to reference Bekkering, H., & Neggers, S. F. W. (2002). Visual search is modulated by action intentions. Psychological Science, 13(4), 370–374.PubMedCrossRef Bekkering, H., & Neggers, S. F. W. (2002). Visual search is modulated by action intentions. Psychological Science, 13(4), 370–374.PubMedCrossRef
go back to reference Blaesi, S., & Wilson, M. (2010). The mirror reflects both ways: Action influences perception of others. Brain and Cognition, 72(2), 306–309.PubMedCrossRef Blaesi, S., & Wilson, M. (2010). The mirror reflects both ways: Action influences perception of others. Brain and Cognition, 72(2), 306–309.PubMedCrossRef
go back to reference Brass, M., Bekkering, H., & Prinz, W. (2001). Movement observation affects movement execution in a simple response task. Acta Psychologica, 106(1–2), 3–22.PubMedCrossRef Brass, M., Bekkering, H., & Prinz, W. (2001). Movement observation affects movement execution in a simple response task. Acta Psychologica, 106(1–2), 3–22.PubMedCrossRef
go back to reference Buccino, G., Binkofski, F., Fink, G. R., Fadiga, L., Fogassi, L., Gallese, V., Seitz, R. J., Zilles, K., Rizzolatti, G., & Freund, H. J. (2001). Action observation activates premotor and parietal areas in a somatotopic manner: An FMRI study. European Journal of Neuroscience, 13(2), 400–404.PubMed Buccino, G., Binkofski, F., Fink, G. R., Fadiga, L., Fogassi, L., Gallese, V., Seitz, R. J., Zilles, K., Rizzolatti, G., & Freund, H. J. (2001). Action observation activates premotor and parietal areas in a somatotopic manner: An FMRI study. European Journal of Neuroscience, 13(2), 400–404.PubMed
go back to reference Casile, A., & Giese, M. A. (2006). Nonvisual motor training influences biological motion perception. Current Biology, 16(1), 69–74.PubMedCrossRef Casile, A., & Giese, M. A. (2006). Nonvisual motor training influences biological motion perception. Current Biology, 16(1), 69–74.PubMedCrossRef
go back to reference Chartrand, T. L., & Bargh, J. A. (1999). The chameleon effect: The perception-behavior link and social interaction. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 76(6), 893–910.PubMedCrossRef Chartrand, T. L., & Bargh, J. A. (1999). The chameleon effect: The perception-behavior link and social interaction. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 76(6), 893–910.PubMedCrossRef
go back to reference Chua, R., & Weeks, D. J. (1997). Dynamical explorations of compatibility in perception-action coupling. In B. Hommel & W. Prinz (Eds.), Theoretical issues in stimulus-response compatibility, (pp. 373–398). Amsterdam: North-Holland. Chua, R., & Weeks, D. J. (1997). Dynamical explorations of compatibility in perception-action coupling. In B. Hommel & W. Prinz (Eds.), Theoretical issues in stimulus-response compatibility, (pp. 373–398). Amsterdam: North-Holland.
go back to reference Craighero, L., Bello, A., Fadiga, L., & Rizzolatti G. (2002) Hand action preparation influences the responses to hand pictures. Neuropsychologia, 40(5), 492–502.PubMedCrossRef Craighero, L., Bello, A., Fadiga, L., & Rizzolatti G. (2002) Hand action preparation influences the responses to hand pictures. Neuropsychologia, 40(5), 492–502.PubMedCrossRef
go back to reference Craighero, L., Fadiga, L., Rizzolatti, G., & Umiltà, C. (1998). Visuomotor priming. Visual Cognition, 5, 109–125.CrossRef Craighero, L., Fadiga, L., Rizzolatti, G., & Umiltà, C. (1998). Visuomotor priming. Visual Cognition, 5, 109–125.CrossRef
go back to reference Craighero, L., Fadiga, L., Rizzolatti, G., & Umiltà, C. (1999). Action for perception: A motor-visual attentional effect. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 25(6), 1673–1692.PubMedCrossRef Craighero, L., Fadiga, L., Rizzolatti, G., & Umiltà, C. (1999). Action for perception: A motor-visual attentional effect. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 25(6), 1673–1692.PubMedCrossRef
go back to reference Decety, J., & Michel, F. (1989). Comparative analysis of actual and mental movement times in two graphic tasks. Brain and Cognition, 11(1), 87–97.PubMedCrossRef Decety, J., & Michel, F. (1989). Comparative analysis of actual and mental movement times in two graphic tasks. Brain and Cognition, 11(1), 87–97.PubMedCrossRef
go back to reference Edwards, M. G., Humphreys, G. W., & Castiello, U. (2003). Motor facilitation following action observation: A behavioural study in prehensile action. Brain and Cognition, 53(3), 495–502.PubMedCrossRef Edwards, M. G., Humphreys, G. W., & Castiello, U. (2003). Motor facilitation following action observation: A behavioural study in prehensile action. Brain and Cognition, 53(3), 495–502.PubMedCrossRef
go back to reference Ellis, R., & Tucker, M. (2000). Micro-affordance: The potentiation of components of action by seen objects. British Journal of Psychology, 91, 451–471.PubMedCrossRef Ellis, R., & Tucker, M. (2000). Micro-affordance: The potentiation of components of action by seen objects. British Journal of Psychology, 91, 451–471.PubMedCrossRef
go back to reference Fagioli, S., Hommel, B., & Schubotz, R. I. (2007). Intentional control of attention: Action planning primes action-related stimulus dimensions. Psychological Research/Psychologische Forschung, 71(1), 22–29.CrossRef Fagioli, S., Hommel, B., & Schubotz, R. I. (2007). Intentional control of attention: Action planning primes action-related stimulus dimensions. Psychological Research/Psychologische Forschung, 71(1), 22–29.CrossRef
go back to reference Fowler, C. A., Galantucci, B., & Saltzman, E. (2003). Motor theories of perception. In M. A. Arbib (Ed.), The handbook of brain theory & neural networks. New York: MIT. Fowler, C. A., Galantucci, B., & Saltzman, E. (2003). Motor theories of perception. In M. A. Arbib (Ed.), The handbook of brain theory & neural networks. New York: MIT.
go back to reference Franz, V. H., Gegenfurtner, K. R., Bülthoff, H. H., & Fahle, M. (2000). Grasping visual illusions: No evidence for a dissociation between perception and action. Psychological Science, 11(1), 20–25.PubMedCrossRef Franz, V. H., Gegenfurtner, K. R., Bülthoff, H. H., & Fahle, M. (2000). Grasping visual illusions: No evidence for a dissociation between perception and action. Psychological Science, 11(1), 20–25.PubMedCrossRef
go back to reference Freeman, J. E., & Ellis, J. A. (2003). The representation of delayed intentions: A prospective subject-performed task?. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory and Cognition, 29(5), 976–992.CrossRef Freeman, J. E., & Ellis, J. A. (2003). The representation of delayed intentions: A prospective subject-performed task?. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory and Cognition, 29(5), 976–992.CrossRef
go back to reference Gallese, V., Fadiga, L., Fogassi, L., & Rizzolatti, G. (1996). Action recognition in the premotor cortex. Brain, 119, 593–609.PubMedCrossRef Gallese, V., Fadiga, L., Fogassi, L., & Rizzolatti, G. (1996). Action recognition in the premotor cortex. Brain, 119, 593–609.PubMedCrossRef
go back to reference Gallese, V., & Goldman, A. (1998). Mirror neurons and the simulation theory of mind-reading. Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 2(12), 493–501.PubMedCrossRef Gallese, V., & Goldman, A. (1998). Mirror neurons and the simulation theory of mind-reading. Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 2(12), 493–501.PubMedCrossRef
go back to reference Gazzola, V., Rizzolatti, G., Wicker, B., & Keysers, C. (2007). The anthropomorphic brain: The mirror neuron system responds to human and robotic actions. Neuroimage, 35(4), 1674–1684.PubMedCrossRef Gazzola, V., Rizzolatti, G., Wicker, B., & Keysers, C. (2007). The anthropomorphic brain: The mirror neuron system responds to human and robotic actions. Neuroimage, 35(4), 1674–1684.PubMedCrossRef
go back to reference Gibson, J. J. (Ed.) (1979). The ecological approach to visual perception. New York: Houghton Mifflin. Gibson, J. J. (Ed.) (1979). The ecological approach to visual perception. New York: Houghton Mifflin.
go back to reference Glover, S. (2002). Visual illusions affect planning but not control. Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 6(7), 288–292.PubMedCrossRef Glover, S. (2002). Visual illusions affect planning but not control. Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 6(7), 288–292.PubMedCrossRef
go back to reference Gomi, H., Abekawa, N., & Nishida, S. (2006). Spatiotemporal tuning of rapid interactions between visual-motion analysis and reaching movement. Journal of Neuroscience, 26(20), 5301–5308.PubMedCrossRef Gomi, H., Abekawa, N., & Nishida, S. (2006). Spatiotemporal tuning of rapid interactions between visual-motion analysis and reaching movement. Journal of Neuroscience, 26(20), 5301–5308.PubMedCrossRef
go back to reference Goodale, M. A., & Milner, A. D. (1992). Separate visual pathways for perception and action. Trends in Neurosciences, 15(1), 20–25.PubMedCrossRef Goodale, M. A., & Milner, A. D. (1992). Separate visual pathways for perception and action. Trends in Neurosciences, 15(1), 20–25.PubMedCrossRef
go back to reference Graf, M., Reitzner, B., Corves, C., Casile, A., Giese, M., & Prinz, W. (2007). Predicting point-light actions in real-time. Neuroimage, 36(Suppl 2), T22–T32.PubMedCrossRef Graf, M., Reitzner, B., Corves, C., Casile, A., Giese, M., & Prinz, W. (2007). Predicting point-light actions in real-time. Neuroimage, 36(Suppl 2), T22–T32.PubMedCrossRef
go back to reference Greenwald, A. G. (1972). On doing two things at once: Time sharing as a function of ideomotor compatibility. Journal of Experimental Psychology, 94(1), 52–57.PubMedCrossRef Greenwald, A. G. (1972). On doing two things at once: Time sharing as a function of ideomotor compatibility. Journal of Experimental Psychology, 94(1), 52–57.PubMedCrossRef
go back to reference Grosjean, M., Shiffrar, M., & Knoblich, G. (2007). Fitts’s law holds for action perception. Psychological Science, 18(2), 95–99.PubMedCrossRef Grosjean, M., Shiffrar, M., & Knoblich, G. (2007). Fitts’s law holds for action perception. Psychological Science, 18(2), 95–99.PubMedCrossRef
go back to reference Grosjean, M., Zwickel, J., & Prinz, W. (2009). Acting while perceiving: Assimilation precedes contrast. Psychological Research, 73(1), 3–13.PubMedCrossRef Grosjean, M., Zwickel, J., & Prinz, W. (2009). Acting while perceiving: Assimilation precedes contrast. Psychological Research, 73(1), 3–13.PubMedCrossRef
go back to reference Hamilton, A., Wolpert, D., & Frith, U. (2004). Your own action influences how you perceive another person’s action. Current Biology, 14(6), 493–498.PubMedCrossRef Hamilton, A., Wolpert, D., & Frith, U. (2004). Your own action influences how you perceive another person’s action. Current Biology, 14(6), 493–498.PubMedCrossRef
go back to reference Hannus, A., Cornelissen, F. W., Lindemann, O., & Bekkering, H. (2005). Selection-for-action in visual search. Acta Psychologica (Amst), 118(1–2), 171–191.CrossRef Hannus, A., Cornelissen, F. W., Lindemann, O., & Bekkering, H. (2005). Selection-for-action in visual search. Acta Psychologica (Amst), 118(1–2), 171–191.CrossRef
go back to reference Helbig, H. B., Steinwender, J., Graf, M., & Kiefer, M. (2010). Action observation can prime visual object recognition. Experimental Brain Research, 200(3–4), 251–258.CrossRef Helbig, H. B., Steinwender, J., Graf, M., & Kiefer, M. (2010). Action observation can prime visual object recognition. Experimental Brain Research, 200(3–4), 251–258.CrossRef
go back to reference Hommel, B., Müsseler, J., Aschersleben, G., & Prinz, W. (2001). The theory of event coding (TEC): A framework for perception and action planning. Behavioral & Brain Sciences, 24(5), 849–937.CrossRef Hommel, B., Müsseler, J., Aschersleben, G., & Prinz, W. (2001). The theory of event coding (TEC): A framework for perception and action planning. Behavioral & Brain Sciences, 24(5), 849–937.CrossRef
go back to reference Hommel, B., & Prinz, W. (1997). Theoretical issues in stimulus-response compatibility: An editor’s introduction. In B. Hommel & W. Prinz (Eds.), Theoretical issues in stimulus-response compatibility (pp. 3–8). Amsterdam: North-Holland. Hommel, B., & Prinz, W. (1997). Theoretical issues in stimulus-response compatibility: An editor’s introduction. In B. Hommel & W. Prinz (Eds.), Theoretical issues in stimulus-response compatibility (pp. 3–8). Amsterdam: North-Holland.
go back to reference Jacobs, A., & Shiffrar, M. (2005). Walking perception by walking observers. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 31(1), 157–169.PubMedCrossRef Jacobs, A., & Shiffrar, M. (2005). Walking perception by walking observers. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 31(1), 157–169.PubMedCrossRef
go back to reference Kahneman, D., Beatty, J., & Pollack, I. (1967). Perceptual deficit during a mental task. Science, 157(3785), 218–219.PubMedCrossRef Kahneman, D., Beatty, J., & Pollack, I. (1967). Perceptual deficit during a mental task. Science, 157(3785), 218–219.PubMedCrossRef
go back to reference Keller, P. E., Knoblich, G., & Repp, B. H. (2007). Pianists duet better when they play with themselves: On the possible role of action simulation in synchronization. Consciousness and Cognition, 16(1), 102–111.PubMedCrossRef Keller, P. E., Knoblich, G., & Repp, B. H. (2007). Pianists duet better when they play with themselves: On the possible role of action simulation in synchronization. Consciousness and Cognition, 16(1), 102–111.PubMedCrossRef
go back to reference Kerzel, D. (2001). Visual short-term memory is influenced by haptic perception. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning Memory and Cognition, 27(4), 1101–1109.CrossRef Kerzel, D. (2001). Visual short-term memory is influenced by haptic perception. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning Memory and Cognition, 27(4), 1101–1109.CrossRef
go back to reference Kilner, J. M., de C. Hamilton, A. F., & Blakemore, S.-J. (2007). Interference effect of observed human movement on action is due to velocity profile of biological motion. Social Neuroscience, 2(3–4), 158–166.PubMedCrossRef Kilner, J. M., de C. Hamilton, A. F., & Blakemore, S.-J. (2007). Interference effect of observed human movement on action is due to velocity profile of biological motion. Social Neuroscience, 2(3–4), 158–166.PubMedCrossRef
go back to reference Kilner, J. M., Paulignan, Y., & Blakemore, S. J. (2003). An interference effect of observed biological movement on action. Current Biology, 13(6), 522–525.PubMedCrossRef Kilner, J. M., Paulignan, Y., & Blakemore, S. J. (2003). An interference effect of observed biological movement on action. Current Biology, 13(6), 522–525.PubMedCrossRef
go back to reference Kornblum, S., Hasbroucq, T., & Osman, A. (1990). Dimensional overlap: Cognitive basis for stimulus-response compatibility. A model and taxonomy. Psychological Review, 97(2), 253–270.PubMedCrossRef Kornblum, S., Hasbroucq, T., & Osman, A. (1990). Dimensional overlap: Cognitive basis for stimulus-response compatibility. A model and taxonomy. Psychological Review, 97(2), 253–270.PubMedCrossRef
go back to reference Liepelt, R., Prinz, W., & Brass, M. (2010). When do we simulate non-human agents? Dissociating communicative and non-communicative actions. Cognition, 115(3), 426–434. Liepelt, R., Prinz, W., & Brass, M. (2010). When do we simulate non-human agents? Dissociating communicative and non-communicative actions. Cognition, 115(3), 426–434.
go back to reference Lindemann, O., & Bekkering, H. (2009). Object manipulation and motion perception: Evidence of an influence of action planning on visual processing. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 35(4), 1062–1071.PubMedCrossRef Lindemann, O., & Bekkering, H. (2009). Object manipulation and motion perception: Evidence of an influence of action planning on visual processing. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 35(4), 1062–1071.PubMedCrossRef
go back to reference Mahon, B. Z., Milleville, S. C., Negri, G. A. L., Rumiati, R. I., Caramazza, A., & Martin, A. (2007). Action-related properties shape object representations in the ventral stream. Neuron, 55(3), 507–520. Mahon, B. Z., Milleville, S. C., Negri, G. A. L., Rumiati, R. I., Caramazza, A., & Martin, A. (2007). Action-related properties shape object representations in the ventral stream. Neuron, 55(3), 507–520.
go back to reference Mantas, A., Evdokimidis, I., & Smyrnis, N. (2008). Perception action interaction: The oblique effect in the evolving trajectory of arm pointing movements. Experimental Brain Research, 184(4), 605–616.CrossRef Mantas, A., Evdokimidis, I., & Smyrnis, N. (2008). Perception action interaction: The oblique effect in the evolving trajectory of arm pointing movements. Experimental Brain Research, 184(4), 605–616.CrossRef
go back to reference Miall, R. C., Stanley, J., Todhunter, S., Levick, C., Lindo, S., & Miall, J. D. (2006). Performing hand actions assists the visual discrimination of similar hand postures. Neuropsychologia, 44(6), 966–976.PubMedCrossRef Miall, R. C., Stanley, J., Todhunter, S., Levick, C., Lindo, S., & Miall, J. D. (2006). Performing hand actions assists the visual discrimination of similar hand postures. Neuropsychologia, 44(6), 966–976.PubMedCrossRef
go back to reference Müsseler, J. (1999). How independent from action control is perception? An event-coding account for more equally-ranked crosstalks. In G. Aschersleben, T. Bachmann, & J. Müsseler (Eds.), Cognitive contributions to the perception of spatial and temporal events. Advances in psychology (Vol. 129, Chap. 6, pp. 121–147). Amsterdam: Elsevier. Müsseler, J. (1999). How independent from action control is perception? An event-coding account for more equally-ranked crosstalks. In G. Aschersleben, T. Bachmann, & J. Müsseler (Eds.), Cognitive contributions to the perception of spatial and temporal events. Advances in psychology (Vol. 129, Chap. 6, pp. 121–147). Amsterdam: Elsevier.
go back to reference Müsseler, J., & Hommel, B. (1997). Blindness to response-compatible stimuli. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 23(3), 861–872.PubMedCrossRef Müsseler, J., & Hommel, B. (1997). Blindness to response-compatible stimuli. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 23(3), 861–872.PubMedCrossRef
go back to reference Parsons, L. M. (1987). Imagined spatial transformations of one’s hands and feet. Cognitive Psychology, 19(2), 178–241.PubMedCrossRef Parsons, L. M. (1987). Imagined spatial transformations of one’s hands and feet. Cognitive Psychology, 19(2), 178–241.PubMedCrossRef
go back to reference Prinz, W. (1997). Perception and action planning. European Journal of Cognitive Psychology, 9(2), 129–154.CrossRef Prinz, W. (1997). Perception and action planning. European Journal of Cognitive Psychology, 9(2), 129–154.CrossRef
go back to reference Proctor, R. W., & Reeve, T. G. (Eds.) (1990). Stimulus-response compatibility: An integrated perspective. Amsterdam: North-Holland. Proctor, R. W., & Reeve, T. G. (Eds.) (1990). Stimulus-response compatibility: An integrated perspective. Amsterdam: North-Holland.
go back to reference Pylyshyn, Z. (1999). Is vision continuous with cognition? The case for cognitive impenetrability of visual perception. Behavioural and Brain Science, 22(3), 341–365; discussion 366–423. Pylyshyn, Z. (1999). Is vision continuous with cognition? The case for cognitive impenetrability of visual perception. Behavioural and Brain Science, 22(3), 341–365; discussion 366–423.
go back to reference Repp, B. H. (2006). Does an auditory distractor sequence affect self-paced tapping? Acta Psychologica, 121(1), 81–107.PubMedCrossRef Repp, B. H. (2006). Does an auditory distractor sequence affect self-paced tapping? Acta Psychologica, 121(1), 81–107.PubMedCrossRef
go back to reference Repp, B. H., & Knoblich, G. (2007). Action can affect auditory perception. Psychological Science, 18(1), 6–7.PubMedCrossRef Repp, B. H., & Knoblich, G. (2007). Action can affect auditory perception. Psychological Science, 18(1), 6–7.PubMedCrossRef
go back to reference Richardson, M. J., Marsh, K. L., Isenhower, R. W., Goodman, J. R. L., & Schmidt, R. C. (2007). Rocking together: Dynamics of intentional and unintentional interpersonal coordination. Human Movement Science, 26(6), 867–891.PubMedCrossRef Richardson, M. J., Marsh, K. L., Isenhower, R. W., Goodman, J. R. L., & Schmidt, R. C. (2007). Rocking together: Dynamics of intentional and unintentional interpersonal coordination. Human Movement Science, 26(6), 867–891.PubMedCrossRef
go back to reference Riddoch, M. J., Humphreys, G. W., Edwards, S., Baker, T., & Willson, K. (2003). Seeing the action: Neuropsychological evidence for action-based effects on object selection. Nature Neuroscience, 6(1), 82–89.PubMedCrossRef Riddoch, M. J., Humphreys, G. W., Edwards, S., Baker, T., & Willson, K. (2003). Seeing the action: Neuropsychological evidence for action-based effects on object selection. Nature Neuroscience, 6(1), 82–89.PubMedCrossRef
go back to reference Rizzolatti, G., & Craighero, L. (1998). Spatial attention: Mechanisms and theories. In M. Sabourin, F. Craik, & M. Robert (Eds.), Advances in psychological science 2: Biological and cognitive aspects (pp. 171–198). Hove: Psychology. Rizzolatti, G., & Craighero, L. (1998). Spatial attention: Mechanisms and theories. In M. Sabourin, F. Craik, & M. Robert (Eds.), Advances in psychological science 2: Biological and cognitive aspects (pp. 171–198). Hove: Psychology.
go back to reference Rizzolatti, G., & Craighero, L. (2004). The mirror-neuron system. Annual Review of Neuroscience, 27, 169–192.PubMedCrossRef Rizzolatti, G., & Craighero, L. (2004). The mirror-neuron system. Annual Review of Neuroscience, 27, 169–192.PubMedCrossRef
go back to reference Rizzolatti, G., Fogassi, L., & Gallese, V. (2001). Neurophysiological mechanisms underlying the understanding and imitation of action. Nature Reviews Neuroscience, 2(9), 661–670.PubMedCrossRef Rizzolatti, G., Fogassi, L., & Gallese, V. (2001). Neurophysiological mechanisms underlying the understanding and imitation of action. Nature Reviews Neuroscience, 2(9), 661–670.PubMedCrossRef
go back to reference Rusconi, E., Kwan, B., Giordano, B. L., Umiltà, C., & Butterworth, B. (2006). Spatial representation of pitch height: The smarc effect. Cognition, 99(2), 113–129.PubMedCrossRef Rusconi, E., Kwan, B., Giordano, B. L., Umiltà, C., & Butterworth, B. (2006). Spatial representation of pitch height: The smarc effect. Cognition, 99(2), 113–129.PubMedCrossRef
go back to reference Sanders, A. F. (1983). Towards a model of stress and human performance. Acta Psychologica, 53(1), 61–97.PubMedCrossRef Sanders, A. F. (1983). Towards a model of stress and human performance. Acta Psychologica, 53(1), 61–97.PubMedCrossRef
go back to reference Schneider, W., & Deubel, H. (2002). Selection-for-perception and selection-for-spatial-motor-action are coupled by visual attention: A review of recent findings and new evidence from stimulus-driven saccade control. In W. Prinz & B. Hommel (Eds.), Attention and performance XIX: Common mechanisms in perception and action, number 19 in Attention and performance (pp. 609–627). Oxford: Oxford University Press. Schneider, W., & Deubel, H. (2002). Selection-for-perception and selection-for-spatial-motor-action are coupled by visual attention: A review of recent findings and new evidence from stimulus-driven saccade control. In W. Prinz & B. Hommel (Eds.), Attention and performance XIX: Common mechanisms in perception and action, number 19 in Attention and performance (pp. 609–627). Oxford: Oxford University Press.
go back to reference Schubö, A., Aschersleben, G., & Prinz, W. (2001). Interactions between perception and action in a reaction task with overlapping S-R assignments. Psychological Research/Psychologische Forschung, 65(3), 145–157.CrossRef Schubö, A., Aschersleben, G., & Prinz, W. (2001). Interactions between perception and action in a reaction task with overlapping S-R assignments. Psychological Research/Psychologische Forschung, 65(3), 145–157.CrossRef
go back to reference Schütz-Bosbach, S., & Prinz, W. (2007). Perceptual resonance: Action-induced modulation of perception. Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 11(8), 349–355.PubMedCrossRef Schütz-Bosbach, S., & Prinz, W. (2007). Perceptual resonance: Action-induced modulation of perception. Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 11(8), 349–355.PubMedCrossRef
go back to reference Shiffrar, M., & Freyd, J. J. (1990). Apparent motion of the human body. Psychological Science, 1(4), 257–264.CrossRef Shiffrar, M., & Freyd, J. J. (1990). Apparent motion of the human body. Psychological Science, 1(4), 257–264.CrossRef
go back to reference Simon, J. R. (1968). Effect of ear stimulated on reaction time and movement time. Journal of Experimental Psychology, 78(2), 344–346.PubMedCrossRef Simon, J. R. (1968). Effect of ear stimulated on reaction time and movement time. Journal of Experimental Psychology, 78(2), 344–346.PubMedCrossRef
go back to reference Stanley, J., Gowen, E., & Miall, R. C. (2007). Effects of agency on movement interference during observation of a moving dot stimulus. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 33(4), 915–926.PubMedCrossRef Stanley, J., Gowen, E., & Miall, R. C. (2007). Effects of agency on movement interference during observation of a moving dot stimulus. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 33(4), 915–926.PubMedCrossRef
go back to reference Stevanovski, B., Oriet, C., & Jolicoeur, P. (2002). Blinded by headlights. Canadian Journal of Experimental Psychology, 56(2), 65–74.PubMedCrossRef Stevanovski, B., Oriet, C., & Jolicoeur, P. (2002). Blinded by headlights. Canadian Journal of Experimental Psychology, 56(2), 65–74.PubMedCrossRef
go back to reference Tipper, S. P., Howard, L. A., & Jackson, S. R. (1997). Selective reaching to grasp: Evidence for distractor interference effects. Visual Cognition, 4(1), 1–38.CrossRef Tipper, S. P., Howard, L. A., & Jackson, S. R. (1997). Selective reaching to grasp: Evidence for distractor interference effects. Visual Cognition, 4(1), 1–38.CrossRef
go back to reference Tucker, M., & Ellis, R. (1998). On the relations between seen objects and components of potential actions. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 24(3), 830–846.PubMedCrossRef Tucker, M., & Ellis, R. (1998). On the relations between seen objects and components of potential actions. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 24(3), 830–846.PubMedCrossRef
go back to reference Viviani, P. (2002). Motor competence in the perception of dynamic events: A tutorial. In W. Prinz & B. Hommel (Eds.), Common mechanisms in perception and action: Attention and performance XIX (Chap. 21, pp. 406–442). New York: Oxford University Press. Viviani, P. (2002). Motor competence in the perception of dynamic events: A tutorial. In W. Prinz & B. Hommel (Eds.), Common mechanisms in perception and action: Attention and performance XIX (Chap. 21, pp. 406–442). New York: Oxford University Press.
go back to reference Wallace, R. J. (1971). S-R compatibility and the idea of a response code. Journal of Experimental Psychology, 88(3), 354–360.PubMedCrossRef Wallace, R. J. (1971). S-R compatibility and the idea of a response code. Journal of Experimental Psychology, 88(3), 354–360.PubMedCrossRef
go back to reference Ward, R. (2002). Independence and integration of perception and action: An introduction. Visual Cognition, 9, 385–391.CrossRef Ward, R. (2002). Independence and integration of perception and action: An introduction. Visual Cognition, 9, 385–391.CrossRef
go back to reference Whitney, D., Westwood, D., & Goodale, M. (2003). The influence of visual motion on fast reaching movements to a stationary object. Nature, 423, 869–873.PubMedCrossRef Whitney, D., Westwood, D., & Goodale, M. (2003). The influence of visual motion on fast reaching movements to a stationary object. Nature, 423, 869–873.PubMedCrossRef
go back to reference Wickens, C. D. (1980). The structure of attentional resources. In Attention and performance VIII. Lawrence Erlbaum Associates: Hillsdale, N.J. Wickens, C. D. (1980). The structure of attentional resources. In Attention and performance VIII. Lawrence Erlbaum Associates: Hillsdale, N.J.
go back to reference Wickens, C. D. (1984). Processing resources in attention. In R. Parasuraman & D. Davies (Eds.), Varieties of attention (pp. 63–102). Orlando: Academic Press. Wickens, C. D. (1984). Processing resources in attention. In R. Parasuraman & D. Davies (Eds.), Varieties of attention (pp. 63–102). Orlando: Academic Press.
go back to reference Wilson, M., & Fox, G. (2007). Working memory for language is not special. Psychonomic Bulletin & Review, 14(3), 470–473.CrossRef Wilson, M., & Fox, G. (2007). Working memory for language is not special. Psychonomic Bulletin & Review, 14(3), 470–473.CrossRef
go back to reference Wilson, M., & Knoblich, G. (2005). The case for motor involvement in perceiving conspecifics. Psychological Bulletin, 131(3), 460–473.PubMedCrossRef Wilson, M., & Knoblich, G. (2005). The case for motor involvement in perceiving conspecifics. Psychological Bulletin, 131(3), 460–473.PubMedCrossRef
go back to reference Witt, J. K., & Proffitt, D. R. (2008). Action-specific influences on distance perception: A role for motor simulation. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 34(6), 1479–1492.PubMedCrossRef Witt, J. K., & Proffitt, D. R. (2008). Action-specific influences on distance perception: A role for motor simulation. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 34(6), 1479–1492.PubMedCrossRef
go back to reference Wohlschläger, A. (2000). Visual motion priming by invisible actions. Vision Research, 40(8), 925–930.PubMedCrossRef Wohlschläger, A. (2000). Visual motion priming by invisible actions. Vision Research, 40(8), 925–930.PubMedCrossRef
go back to reference Wohlschläger, A., & Wohlschläger, A. (1998). Mental and manual rotation. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 24(2), 397–412.PubMedCrossRef Wohlschläger, A., & Wohlschläger, A. (1998). Mental and manual rotation. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 24(2), 397–412.PubMedCrossRef
go back to reference Wolpert, D. M., Doya, K., & Kawato, M. (2003). A unifying computational framework for motor control and social interaction. Philosophical Transactions of the Royal Society of London. Series B: Biological Sciences, 358(1431), 593–602.PubMedCrossRef Wolpert, D. M., Doya, K., & Kawato, M. (2003). A unifying computational framework for motor control and social interaction. Philosophical Transactions of the Royal Society of London. Series B: Biological Sciences, 358(1431), 593–602.PubMedCrossRef
go back to reference Wood, J. N. (2007). Visual working memory for observed actions. Journal of Experimental Psychology: General, 136(4), 639–652.CrossRef Wood, J. N. (2007). Visual working memory for observed actions. Journal of Experimental Psychology: General, 136(4), 639–652.CrossRef
go back to reference Zwickel, J., Grosjean, M., & Prinz, W. (2007). Seeing while moving: Measuring the online influence of action on perception. Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology, 60(8), 1063–1071.CrossRef Zwickel, J., Grosjean, M., & Prinz, W. (2007). Seeing while moving: Measuring the online influence of action on perception. Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology, 60(8), 1063–1071.CrossRef
go back to reference Zwickel, J., Grosjean, M., & Prinz, W. (2008). A contrast effect between the concurrent production and perception of movement directions. Visual Cognition, 26, 953–978.CrossRef Zwickel, J., Grosjean, M., & Prinz, W. (2008). A contrast effect between the concurrent production and perception of movement directions. Visual Cognition, 26, 953–978.CrossRef
go back to reference Zwickel, J., Grosjean, M., & Prinz, W. (2010a). On interference effects in concurrent perception and action. Psychological Research/Psychologische Forschung, 74, 152–171.CrossRef Zwickel, J., Grosjean, M., & Prinz, W. (2010a). On interference effects in concurrent perception and action. Psychological Research/Psychologische Forschung, 74, 152–171.CrossRef
go back to reference Zwickel, J., Grosjean, M., & Prinz, W. (2010b). What part of an action interferes with ongoing perception? Acta Psychologica, 134(3), 403–409.PubMedCrossRef Zwickel, J., Grosjean, M., & Prinz, W. (2010b). What part of an action interferes with ongoing perception? Acta Psychologica, 134(3), 403–409.PubMedCrossRef
Metagegevens
Titel
Assimilation and contrast: the two sides of specific interference between action and perception
Auteurs
Jan Zwickel
Wolfgang Prinz
Publicatiedatum
01-03-2012
Uitgeverij
Springer-Verlag
Gepubliceerd in
Psychological Research / Uitgave 2/2012
Print ISSN: 0340-0727
Elektronisch ISSN: 1430-2772
DOI
https://doi.org/10.1007/s00426-011-0338-3

Andere artikelen Uitgave 2/2012

Psychological Research 2/2012 Naar de uitgave