Skip to main content
Top
Gepubliceerd in: Psychological Research 2/2012

01-03-2012 | Original Article

The mental number line electrified: brain potentials in a numerical flanker task

Auteurs: Anja Fellbrich, Wido Nager, Thomas F. Münte

Gepubliceerd in: Psychological Research | Uitgave 2/2012

Log in om toegang te krijgen
share
DELEN

Deel dit onderdeel of sectie (kopieer de link)

  • Optie A:
    Klik op de rechtermuisknop op de link en selecteer de optie “linkadres kopiëren”
  • Optie B:
    Deel de link per e-mail

Abstract

It has been suggested that the mental representation of numbers is spatial in nature such that numbers are ordered on a mental number line. In the present investigation we use a variant of the Eriksen flanker task requiring a magnitude decision (smaller or larger than 5) for a central target number by pressing a response button with the right or left hand. The target number is flanked by irrelevant distracters that are either identical to the target, different from the target but biasing the same response, or different from the target and biasing a different response. Response latencies and event-related brain potentials were obtained in a group of healthy adults. Besides the typical response congruency effects on response latency and the N2 component of the ERP, we observed several other effects. First, numerical distance of the target to the standard 5 influenced decision latencies and amplitude and latency of the P3 component with smaller distances leading to longer decision latencies, longer P3 latencies and smaller P3 amplitudes. Second, smaller numerical distance between target and distracters led to faster decisions for response congruent and to slower decisions for response-incongruent trials. For response-incongruent trials P3 amplitude was small/large and P3 latency was long/short for small/large distances. These findings underscore the spatial character of number representation and further show that the relation between targets and distracters, although task irrelevant, is assessed automatically with facilitatory and inhibitory effects driven by spatial distance on the mental number line.
Literatuur
go back to reference Botvinick, M. M., Carter, C. S., Braver, T. S., Barch, D. M., & Cohen, J. D. (2001). Conflict monitoring and cognitive control. Psychological Review, 108, 624–652.PubMedCrossRef Botvinick, M. M., Carter, C. S., Braver, T. S., Barch, D. M., & Cohen, J. D. (2001). Conflict monitoring and cognitive control. Psychological Review, 108, 624–652.PubMedCrossRef
go back to reference Botvinick, M. M., Cohen, J. D., & Carter, C. S. (2004). Conflict monitoring and anterior cingulate cortex: an update. Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 8, 539–546.PubMedCrossRef Botvinick, M. M., Cohen, J. D., & Carter, C. S. (2004). Conflict monitoring and anterior cingulate cortex: an update. Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 8, 539–546.PubMedCrossRef
go back to reference Dehaene, S., Bossini, S., & Giraux, P. (1993). The mental representation of parity and number magnitude. Journal of Experimental Psychology: General, 122, 371–396.CrossRef Dehaene, S., Bossini, S., & Giraux, P. (1993). The mental representation of parity and number magnitude. Journal of Experimental Psychology: General, 122, 371–396.CrossRef
go back to reference Fias, W. (2001). Two routes for the processing of verbal numbers: evidence from the SNARC effect. Psychological Research, 12, 415–423. Fias, W. (2001). Two routes for the processing of verbal numbers: evidence from the SNARC effect. Psychological Research, 12, 415–423.
go back to reference Fias, W., Brysbaert, M., Geypens, F., & D’Ydewalle, G. (1996). The importance of magnitude information in numerical processing: evidence from the SNARC effect. Mathematical Cognition, 2, 95–110.CrossRef Fias, W., Brysbaert, M., Geypens, F., & D’Ydewalle, G. (1996). The importance of magnitude information in numerical processing: evidence from the SNARC effect. Mathematical Cognition, 2, 95–110.CrossRef
go back to reference Kenemans, J. L., Kok, A., & Smulders, F. T. (1993). Event-related potentials to conjunctions of spatial frequency and orientation as a function of stimulus parameters and response requirements. Electroencephalography and Clinical Neurophysiology, 88, 51–63.PubMedCrossRef Kenemans, J. L., Kok, A., & Smulders, F. T. (1993). Event-related potentials to conjunctions of spatial frequency and orientation as a function of stimulus parameters and response requirements. Electroencephalography and Clinical Neurophysiology, 88, 51–63.PubMedCrossRef
go back to reference Kok, A. (2001). On the utility of P3 amplitude as a measure of processing capacity. Psychophysiology, 31, 557–577. Kok, A. (2001). On the utility of P3 amplitude as a measure of processing capacity. Psychophysiology, 31, 557–577.
go back to reference Kopp, B., Rist, F., & Mattler, U. (1996). N200 in the flanker task as a neurobehavioral tool for investigating executive control. Psychophysiology, 33, 282–294.PubMedCrossRef Kopp, B., Rist, F., & Mattler, U. (1996). N200 in the flanker task as a neurobehavioral tool for investigating executive control. Psychophysiology, 33, 282–294.PubMedCrossRef
go back to reference Liotti, M., Woldorff, M. G., Perez, R., & Mayberg, H. S. (2000). An ERP study of the temporal course of the Stroop color–word interference effect. Neuropsychologia, 38, 701–711.PubMedCrossRef Liotti, M., Woldorff, M. G., Perez, R., & Mayberg, H. S. (2000). An ERP study of the temporal course of the Stroop color–word interference effect. Neuropsychologia, 38, 701–711.PubMedCrossRef
go back to reference Moyer, R. S., & Landauer, T. K. (1967). Time required for judgements of numerical inequality [47]. Nature, 215, 1519–1520.PubMedCrossRef Moyer, R. S., & Landauer, T. K. (1967). Time required for judgements of numerical inequality [47]. Nature, 215, 1519–1520.PubMedCrossRef
go back to reference Niedeggen, M., & Rösler, F. (1999). N400 effects reflect activation spread during retrieval of arithmetic facts. Psychological Science, 10, 271–276.CrossRef Niedeggen, M., & Rösler, F. (1999). N400 effects reflect activation spread during retrieval of arithmetic facts. Psychological Science, 10, 271–276.CrossRef
go back to reference Niedeggen, M., Rösler, F., & Jost, K. (1999). Processing of incongruous mental calculation problems: evidence for an arithmetic N400 effect. Psychophysiology, 36, 307–324.PubMedCrossRef Niedeggen, M., Rösler, F., & Jost, K. (1999). Processing of incongruous mental calculation problems: evidence for an arithmetic N400 effect. Psychophysiology, 36, 307–324.PubMedCrossRef
go back to reference Nuerk, H. C., Bauer, F., Krummenacher, J., Heller, D., & Willmes, K. (2005). The power of the mental number line: how the magnitude of unattended numbers affects performance in an Eriksen task. Psychology Science, 47, 34–50. Nuerk, H. C., Bauer, F., Krummenacher, J., Heller, D., & Willmes, K. (2005). The power of the mental number line: how the magnitude of unattended numbers affects performance in an Eriksen task. Psychology Science, 47, 34–50.
go back to reference Parasuraman, R., & Beatty, J. (1980). Brain events underlying detection and recognition of weak sensory signals. Science, 210, 80–83.PubMedCrossRef Parasuraman, R., & Beatty, J. (1980). Brain events underlying detection and recognition of weak sensory signals. Science, 210, 80–83.PubMedCrossRef
go back to reference Polich, J. (2007). Updating P300: an integrative theory of P3a and P3b. Clinical Neurophysiology, 118, 2128–2148.PubMedCrossRef Polich, J. (2007). Updating P300: an integrative theory of P3a and P3b. Clinical Neurophysiology, 118, 2128–2148.PubMedCrossRef
go back to reference Priftis, K., Piccione, F., Giorgi, F., Meneghello, F., UmiltÃ, C., & Zorzi, M. (2008). Lost in number space after right brain damage: a neural signature of representational neglect. Cortex, 44, 449–453.PubMedCrossRef Priftis, K., Piccione, F., Giorgi, F., Meneghello, F., UmiltÃ, C., & Zorzi, M. (2008). Lost in number space after right brain damage: a neural signature of representational neglect. Cortex, 44, 449–453.PubMedCrossRef
go back to reference Restle, F. (1970). Speed of adding and comparing numbers. Journal of Experimental Psychology, 83, 274–278.CrossRef Restle, F. (1970). Speed of adding and comparing numbers. Journal of Experimental Psychology, 83, 274–278.CrossRef
go back to reference Riba, J., Rodriguez-Fornells, A., Morte, A., Münte, T. F., & Barbanoj, M. J. (2005). Noradrenergic stimulation enhances human action monitoring. Journal of Neuroscience, 25, 4370–4374.PubMedCrossRef Riba, J., Rodriguez-Fornells, A., Morte, A., Münte, T. F., & Barbanoj, M. J. (2005). Noradrenergic stimulation enhances human action monitoring. Journal of Neuroscience, 25, 4370–4374.PubMedCrossRef
go back to reference Simon, J. R., & Rudell, A. P. (1967). Auditory S-R compatibility: the effect of an irrelevant cue on information processing. Journal of Applied Psychology, 51, 300–304.PubMedCrossRef Simon, J. R., & Rudell, A. P. (1967). Auditory S-R compatibility: the effect of an irrelevant cue on information processing. Journal of Applied Psychology, 51, 300–304.PubMedCrossRef
go back to reference van Veen, V., & Carter, C. S. (2002). The timing of action-monitoring processes in the anterior cingulate cortex. Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience, 14, 593–602.PubMedCrossRef van Veen, V., & Carter, C. S. (2002). The timing of action-monitoring processes in the anterior cingulate cortex. Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience, 14, 593–602.PubMedCrossRef
go back to reference Vuillemier, P., Ortigue, S., & Brugger, P. (2004). The number space and neglect. Cortex, 40, 399–410.CrossRef Vuillemier, P., Ortigue, S., & Brugger, P. (2004). The number space and neglect. Cortex, 40, 399–410.CrossRef
go back to reference Wang, Y., Kong, J., Tang, X., Zhuang, D., & Li, S. (2000). Event-related potential N270 is elicited by mental conflict processing in human brain. Neuroscience Letters, 293, 17–20.PubMedCrossRef Wang, Y., Kong, J., Tang, X., Zhuang, D., & Li, S. (2000). Event-related potential N270 is elicited by mental conflict processing in human brain. Neuroscience Letters, 293, 17–20.PubMedCrossRef
go back to reference Wendt, M., Heldmann, M., Münte, T. F., & Kluwe, R. H. (2007). Disentangling sequential effects of stimulus- and response-related conflict and stimulus-response repetition using brain potentials. Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience, 19, 1104–1112.PubMedCrossRef Wendt, M., Heldmann, M., Münte, T. F., & Kluwe, R. H. (2007). Disentangling sequential effects of stimulus- and response-related conflict and stimulus-response repetition using brain potentials. Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience, 19, 1104–1112.PubMedCrossRef
go back to reference Zorzi, M., Priftis, K., Meneghello, F., Marenzi, R., & Umilta, C. (2006). The spatial representation of numerical and non-numerical sequences: evidence from neglect. Neuropsychologia, 44, 1061–1067.PubMedCrossRef Zorzi, M., Priftis, K., Meneghello, F., Marenzi, R., & Umilta, C. (2006). The spatial representation of numerical and non-numerical sequences: evidence from neglect. Neuropsychologia, 44, 1061–1067.PubMedCrossRef
go back to reference Zorzi, M., Priftis, K., & Umilta, C. (2002). Brain damage: neglect disrupts the mental number line. Nature, 417, 138–139.PubMedCrossRef Zorzi, M., Priftis, K., & Umilta, C. (2002). Brain damage: neglect disrupts the mental number line. Nature, 417, 138–139.PubMedCrossRef
Metagegevens
Titel
The mental number line electrified: brain potentials in a numerical flanker task
Auteurs
Anja Fellbrich
Wido Nager
Thomas F. Münte
Publicatiedatum
01-03-2012
Uitgeverij
Springer-Verlag
Gepubliceerd in
Psychological Research / Uitgave 2/2012
Print ISSN: 0340-0727
Elektronisch ISSN: 1430-2772
DOI
https://doi.org/10.1007/s00426-011-0409-5

Andere artikelen Uitgave 2/2012

Psychological Research 2/2012 Naar de uitgave