Skip to main content
Top
Gepubliceerd in: Psychological Research 1/2007

01-01-2007 | Original Article

Intentional control of attention: action planning primes action-related stimulus dimensions

Auteurs: Sabrina Fagioli, Bernhard Hommel, Ricarda Ines Schubotz

Gepubliceerd in: Psychological Research | Uitgave 1/2007

Log in om toegang te krijgen
share
DELEN

Deel dit onderdeel of sectie (kopieer de link)

  • Optie A:
    Klik op de rechtermuisknop op de link en selecteer de optie “linkadres kopiëren”
  • Optie B:
    Deel de link per e-mail

Abstract

Neurophysiological observations suggest that attending to a particular perceptual dimension, such as location or shape, engages dimension-related action, such as reaching and prehension networks. Here we reversed the perspective and hypothesized that activating action systems may prime the processing of stimuli defined on perceptual dimensions related to these actions. Subjects prepared for a reaching or grasping action and, before carrying it out, were presented with location- or size-defined stimulus events. As predicted, performance on the stimulus event varied with action preparation: planning a reaching action facilitated detecting deviants in location sequences whereas planning a grasping action facilitated detecting deviants in size sequences. These findings support the theory of event coding, which claims that perceptual codes and action plans share a common representational medium, which presumably involves the human premotor cortex.
Voetnoten
1
One might wonder how our prediction that action planning facilitates perceptual processing fits with previous observations of action-induced blindness, that is, the finding that preparing a left- or right-hand action impairs the processing of a spatially compatible stimulus, such as a left- or right-pointing arrow (Müsseler & Hommel, 1997). The explanation is central for our present argument: We assume that planning an action binds the codes that represent this action’s features, which occupies these particular codes and makes them less available for other purposes like coding a perceptual event (the blindness effect; see Hommel & Müsseler, 2005). That is, occupying a code primes it and prevents it not from being further activated by other events, but it does prevent it (to some degree) from being bound with other features and to other representations. The new assumption we make here is that planning an action may not only activate the relevant feature codes but may also prime whole feature dimensions, such as size or shape in the case of grasping and location in the case of pointing. If true this would mean that any feature on the primed dimension would benefit from preparing a particular action. But please note that whether this benefit turns into a measurable behavioral advantage always depends on whether the particular feature is already bound to another event or not (Hommel, 2004)
 
Literatuur
go back to reference Bekkering, H., & Neggers, S. F. W. (2002). Visual search is modulated by action intentions. Psychological Science, 13, 370–374.PubMedCrossRef Bekkering, H., & Neggers, S. F. W. (2002). Visual search is modulated by action intentions. Psychological Science, 13, 370–374.PubMedCrossRef
go back to reference Byrne, R. W., & Russon, A. E. (1998). Learning by imitation: A hierarchical approach. Behavioral and Brain Sciences, 21, 667–684.CrossRefPubMed Byrne, R. W., & Russon, A. E. (1998). Learning by imitation: A hierarchical approach. Behavioral and Brain Sciences, 21, 667–684.CrossRefPubMed
go back to reference Craighero, L., Fadiga, L., Rizzolatti, G., & Umiltà, C. A. (1999). Action for perception: A motor-visual attentional effect. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 25, 1673–1692.CrossRefPubMed Craighero, L., Fadiga, L., Rizzolatti, G., & Umiltà, C. A. (1999). Action for perception: A motor-visual attentional effect. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 25, 1673–1692.CrossRefPubMed
go back to reference Elsner, B., Hommel, B., Mentschel, C., Drzezga, A., Prinz, W., Conrad, B. et al. (2002). Linking actions and their perceivable consequences in the human brain. Neuroimage, 17, 364–372.CrossRefPubMed Elsner, B., Hommel, B., Mentschel, C., Drzezga, A., Prinz, W., Conrad, B. et al. (2002). Linking actions and their perceivable consequences in the human brain. Neuroimage, 17, 364–372.CrossRefPubMed
go back to reference Fadiga, L., Fogassi, L., Gallese, V., & Rizzolatti, G. (2000). Visuomotor neurons: Ambiguity of the discharge or ‘motor’ perception? International Journal of Psychophysiology, 35, 165–177.PubMed Fadiga, L., Fogassi, L., Gallese, V., & Rizzolatti, G. (2000). Visuomotor neurons: Ambiguity of the discharge or ‘motor’ perception? International Journal of Psychophysiology, 35, 165–177.PubMed
go back to reference Genzano, V. R., Di Nocera, F., & Ferlazzo, F. (2001). Upper/lower visual field asymmetry on a spatial relocation memory task. Neuroreport, 12, 1227–1230.CrossRefPubMed Genzano, V. R., Di Nocera, F., & Ferlazzo, F. (2001). Upper/lower visual field asymmetry on a spatial relocation memory task. Neuroreport, 12, 1227–1230.CrossRefPubMed
go back to reference Hamilton, A., Joyce, D. W., Flanagan, R., Frith, C. D., & Wolpert, D. M. (2005). Kinematic cues in perceptual weight judgment and their origins in box lifting. Psychological Research, this volume. Hamilton, A., Joyce, D. W., Flanagan, R., Frith, C. D., & Wolpert, D. M. (2005). Kinematic cues in perceptual weight judgment and their origins in box lifting. Psychological Research, this volume.
go back to reference Hannus, A., Cornelissen, F. W., Lindemann, O., & Bekkering, H. (2005). Selection-for-action in visual search. Acta Psychologica, 118, 171–191.CrossRefPubMed Hannus, A., Cornelissen, F. W., Lindemann, O., & Bekkering, H. (2005). Selection-for-action in visual search. Acta Psychologica, 118, 171–191.CrossRefPubMed
go back to reference Hommel, B. (1998). Perceiving one’s own action—and what it leads to. In J. S. Jordan (Ed.), Systems theory and apriori aspects of perception (pp. 143–179). Amsterdam: North-Holland.CrossRef Hommel, B. (1998). Perceiving one’s own action—and what it leads to. In J. S. Jordan (Ed.), Systems theory and apriori aspects of perception (pp. 143–179). Amsterdam: North-Holland.CrossRef
go back to reference Hommel, B. (2004). Event files: Feature binding in and across perception and action. Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 8, 494–500.CrossRefPubMed Hommel, B. (2004). Event files: Feature binding in and across perception and action. Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 8, 494–500.CrossRefPubMed
go back to reference Hommel, B. (2005a). How we do what we want: A neuro-cognitive perspective on human action planning. In R. J. Jorna, W. van Wezel, & A. Meystel (Eds.), Planning in intelligent systems: Aspects, motivations and methods. New York: Wiley. In press Hommel, B. (2005a). How we do what we want: A neuro-cognitive perspective on human action planning. In R. J. Jorna, W. van Wezel, & A. Meystel (Eds.), Planning in intelligent systems: Aspects, motivations and methods. New York: Wiley. In press
go back to reference Hommel, B. (2005b). Feature integration across perception and action: Event files affect response choice. Psychological Research, this volume. Hommel, B. (2005b). Feature integration across perception and action: Event files affect response choice. Psychological Research, this volume.
go back to reference Hommel, B., & Knuf, L. (2000). Action related determinants of spatial coding in perception and memory. In C. Freksa, W. Brauer, C. Habel, & K. F. Wender (Eds.), Spatial cognition II: Integrating abstract theories, empirical studies, formal methods, and practical applications (pp. 387–398). Berlin Heidelberg New York: Springer. Hommel, B., & Knuf, L. (2000). Action related determinants of spatial coding in perception and memory. In C. Freksa, W. Brauer, C. Habel, & K. F. Wender (Eds.), Spatial cognition II: Integrating abstract theories, empirical studies, formal methods, and practical applications (pp. 387–398). Berlin Heidelberg New York: Springer.
go back to reference Hommel, B., & Müsseler, J. (2005). Action-feature integration blinds to feature-overlapping perceptual events: Evidence from manual and vocal actions. Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology (A). In press Hommel, B., & Müsseler, J. (2005). Action-feature integration blinds to feature-overlapping perceptual events: Evidence from manual and vocal actions. Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology (A). In press
go back to reference Hommel, B., Müsseler, J., Aschersleben, G., & Prinz, W. (2001). The theory of event coding (TEC): A framework for perception and action planning. Behavioral and Brain Sciences, 24, 849–878.PubMedCrossRef Hommel, B., Müsseler, J., Aschersleben, G., & Prinz, W. (2001). The theory of event coding (TEC): A framework for perception and action planning. Behavioral and Brain Sciences, 24, 849–878.PubMedCrossRef
go back to reference Hommel, B., & Schneider, W. X. (2002). Visual attention and manual response selection: Distinct mechanisms operating on the same codes. Visual Cognition, 9, 392–420.CrossRef Hommel, B., & Schneider, W. X. (2002). Visual attention and manual response selection: Distinct mechanisms operating on the same codes. Visual Cognition, 9, 392–420.CrossRef
go back to reference Hoshi, E., & Tanji, J. (2002). Contrasting neuronal activity in the dorsal and ventral premotor areas during preparation to reach. Journal of Neurophysiology, 87, 1123–1128.PubMed Hoshi, E., & Tanji, J. (2002). Contrasting neuronal activity in the dorsal and ventral premotor areas during preparation to reach. Journal of Neurophysiology, 87, 1123–1128.PubMed
go back to reference Kornblum, S., Hasbroucq, T., & Osman, A. (1990). Dimensional overlap: Cognitive basis for stimulus–response compatibility—a model and taxonomy. Psychological Review, 97, 253–270.CrossRefPubMed Kornblum, S., Hasbroucq, T., & Osman, A. (1990). Dimensional overlap: Cognitive basis for stimulus–response compatibility—a model and taxonomy. Psychological Review, 97, 253–270.CrossRefPubMed
go back to reference Lupiáñez, J., Ruz, M., Funes, M. J., & Milliken, B. (2005). The manifestation of attentional capture: Facilitation or IOR depending on task demands. Psychological Research, this volume. Lupiáñez, J., Ruz, M., Funes, M. J., & Milliken, B. (2005). The manifestation of attentional capture: Facilitation or IOR depending on task demands. Psychological Research, this volume.
go back to reference Milliken, B., & Lupiáñez, J. (2005). Repetition costs in word identification: Evaluating a stimulus–response integration account. Psychological Research, this volume. Milliken, B., & Lupiáñez, J. (2005). Repetition costs in word identification: Evaluating a stimulus–response integration account. Psychological Research, this volume.
go back to reference Müller, H. J., Reimann, B., & Krummenacher, J. (2003). Visual search for singleton feature targets across dimensions: Stimulus- and expectancy-driven effects in dimensional weighting. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 29, 1021–1035.CrossRefPubMed Müller, H. J., Reimann, B., & Krummenacher, J. (2003). Visual search for singleton feature targets across dimensions: Stimulus- and expectancy-driven effects in dimensional weighting. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 29, 1021–1035.CrossRefPubMed
go back to reference Murata, A., Fadiga, L., Fogassi, L., Gallese, V., Raos, V., & Rizzolatti, G. (1997). Object representation in the ventral premotor cortex (area F5) of the monkey. Journal of Neurophysiology, 78, 2226–2230PubMed Murata, A., Fadiga, L., Fogassi, L., Gallese, V., Raos, V., & Rizzolatti, G. (1997). Object representation in the ventral premotor cortex (area F5) of the monkey. Journal of Neurophysiology, 78, 2226–2230PubMed
go back to reference Müsseler, J., & Hommel, B. (1997). Blindness to response-compatible stimuli. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 23, 861–872.CrossRefPubMed Müsseler, J., & Hommel, B. (1997). Blindness to response-compatible stimuli. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 23, 861–872.CrossRefPubMed
go back to reference Oriet, C., Stevanovski, B., & Jolicoeur, P. (2005). Feature binding and episodic retrieval in blindness for congruent stimuli: Evidence from analyses of sequential congruency. Psychological Research, this volume. Oriet, C., Stevanovski, B., & Jolicoeur, P. (2005). Feature binding and episodic retrieval in blindness for congruent stimuli: Evidence from analyses of sequential congruency. Psychological Research, this volume.
go back to reference Pickering, S. E., Gathercole, M., Hall, S. A., & Lloyd, S. A. (2001). Development of memory for pattern and path: Further evidence for the fractionation of visuo-spatial memory. Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology, 54A, 397–420.CrossRefPubMed Pickering, S. E., Gathercole, M., Hall, S. A., & Lloyd, S. A. (2001). Development of memory for pattern and path: Further evidence for the fractionation of visuo-spatial memory. Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology, 54A, 397–420.CrossRefPubMed
go back to reference Prinz, W. (1990). A common coding approach to perception and action. In O. Neumann, & W. Prinz (Eds.), Relationships between perception and action (pp. 167–201). Berlin Heidelberg New York: Springer. Prinz, W. (1990). A common coding approach to perception and action. In O. Neumann, & W. Prinz (Eds.), Relationships between perception and action (pp. 167–201). Berlin Heidelberg New York: Springer.
go back to reference Prinz, W. (1997). Perception and action planning. European Journal of Cognitive Psychology, 9, 129–154.CrossRef Prinz, W. (1997). Perception and action planning. European Journal of Cognitive Psychology, 9, 129–154.CrossRef
go back to reference Rizzolatti, G., Camarda, R., Fogassi, L., Gentilucci, M., Luppino, G., & Matelli, M. (1988). Functional organization of inferior area 6 in the macaque monkey. II. Area F5 and the control of distal movements. Experimental Brain Research, 71, 491–507.CrossRef Rizzolatti, G., Camarda, R., Fogassi, L., Gentilucci, M., Luppino, G., & Matelli, M. (1988). Functional organization of inferior area 6 in the macaque monkey. II. Area F5 and the control of distal movements. Experimental Brain Research, 71, 491–507.CrossRef
go back to reference Rizzolatti, G., & Fadiga, L. (1998). Grasping objects and grasping action meanings: The dual role of monkey rostroventral premotor cortex (area F5). Novartis Foundation Symposion, 218, 81–95.CrossRef Rizzolatti, G., & Fadiga, L. (1998). Grasping objects and grasping action meanings: The dual role of monkey rostroventral premotor cortex (area F5). Novartis Foundation Symposion, 218, 81–95.CrossRef
go back to reference Rizzolatti, G., Riggio, L., & Sheliga, B. M. (1994). Space and selective attention. In C. A. Umiltà, & M. Moscovitch (Eds.), Attention and performance, XV. Conscious and nonconscious information processing (pp. 231–265), Cambridge: MIT Press. Rizzolatti, G., Riggio, L., & Sheliga, B. M. (1994). Space and selective attention. In C. A. Umiltà, & M. Moscovitch (Eds.), Attention and performance, XV. Conscious and nonconscious information processing (pp. 231–265), Cambridge: MIT Press.
go back to reference Schubotz, R. I., & von Cramon, D. Y. (2001). Functional organization of the lateral premotor cortex: fMRI reveals different regions activated by anticipation of object properties, location and speed. Cognitive Brain Research, 11, 97–112.CrossRefPubMed Schubotz, R. I., & von Cramon, D. Y. (2001). Functional organization of the lateral premotor cortex: fMRI reveals different regions activated by anticipation of object properties, location and speed. Cognitive Brain Research, 11, 97–112.CrossRefPubMed
go back to reference Schubotz, R. I., & von Cramon, D. Y. (2002). Predicting perceptual events activates corresponding motor schemes in lateral premotor cortex: An fMRI study. Neuroimage, 15, 787–796.CrossRefPubMed Schubotz, R. I., & von Cramon, D. Y. (2002). Predicting perceptual events activates corresponding motor schemes in lateral premotor cortex: An fMRI study. Neuroimage, 15, 787–796.CrossRefPubMed
go back to reference Schubotz, R. I., & von Cramon, D. Y. (2003). Functional-anatomical concepts of human premotor cortex: Evidence from fMRI and PET studies. Neuroimage, 20, S120–S131.CrossRefPubMed Schubotz, R. I., & von Cramon, D. Y. (2003). Functional-anatomical concepts of human premotor cortex: Evidence from fMRI and PET studies. Neuroimage, 20, S120–S131.CrossRefPubMed
go back to reference Schubotz, R. I., & von Cramon, D. Y. (2004a). Brains have emulators with brains: Emulation economized. Behavioral and Brain Sciences, 27, 414–415.CrossRef Schubotz, R. I., & von Cramon, D. Y. (2004a). Brains have emulators with brains: Emulation economized. Behavioral and Brain Sciences, 27, 414–415.CrossRef
go back to reference Schubotz, R. I., & von Cramon, D. Y. (2004b). Sequences of abstract nonbiological stimuli share ventral premotor cortex with action observation and imagery. Journal of Neuroscience, 24, 5467–5474.CrossRefPubMed Schubotz, R. I., & von Cramon, D. Y. (2004b). Sequences of abstract nonbiological stimuli share ventral premotor cortex with action observation and imagery. Journal of Neuroscience, 24, 5467–5474.CrossRefPubMed
go back to reference Schubotz, R. I., Friederici, A. D., & von Cramon, D. Y. (2000). Time perception and motor timing: A common cortical and subcortical basis revealed by fMRI. Neuroimage, 11, 1–12.CrossRefPubMed Schubotz, R. I., Friederici, A. D., & von Cramon, D. Y. (2000). Time perception and motor timing: A common cortical and subcortical basis revealed by fMRI. Neuroimage, 11, 1–12.CrossRefPubMed
go back to reference Shima, K., & Tanji, J. (2000). Neuronal activity in the supplementary and presupplementary motor areas for temporal organization of multiple movements. Journal of Neurophysiology, 84, 2148–2160.PubMed Shima, K., & Tanji, J. (2000). Neuronal activity in the supplementary and presupplementary motor areas for temporal organization of multiple movements. Journal of Neurophysiology, 84, 2148–2160.PubMed
go back to reference Stoet, G., & Hommel, B. (1999). Action planning and the temporal binding of response codes. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 25, 1625–1640.CrossRef Stoet, G., & Hommel, B. (1999). Action planning and the temporal binding of response codes. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 25, 1625–1640.CrossRef
go back to reference Tipper, S. P., Howard, L. A., & Houghton, G. (1999). Action-based mechanisms of attention. In G. W. Humphreys, J. Duncan, & A. Treisman (Eds.). Attention, space and action (pp. 231–247). Oxford: University Press. Tipper, S. P., Howard, L. A., & Houghton, G. (1999). Action-based mechanisms of attention. In G. W. Humphreys, J. Duncan, & A. Treisman (Eds.). Attention, space and action (pp. 231–247). Oxford: University Press.
go back to reference Ward, R. (2002). Independence and integration of perception and action: An introduction. Visual Cognition, 9, 385–391.CrossRef Ward, R. (2002). Independence and integration of perception and action: An introduction. Visual Cognition, 9, 385–391.CrossRef
go back to reference Wenke, D., Gaschler, R., & Nattkemper, D. (2005). Instruction-induced feature binding. Psychological Research, this volume. Wenke, D., Gaschler, R., & Nattkemper, D. (2005). Instruction-induced feature binding. Psychological Research, this volume.
go back to reference Wohlschläger, A. (2000). Visual motion priming by invisible actions. Vision Research, 40, 925–930.CrossRefPubMed Wohlschläger, A. (2000). Visual motion priming by invisible actions. Vision Research, 40, 925–930.CrossRefPubMed
go back to reference Wolfe, J. M. (1994). Guided Search 2.0: A revised model of visual search. Psychonomic Bulletin and Review, 1, 202–238. Wolfe, J. M. (1994). Guided Search 2.0: A revised model of visual search. Psychonomic Bulletin and Review, 1, 202–238.
Metagegevens
Titel
Intentional control of attention: action planning primes action-related stimulus dimensions
Auteurs
Sabrina Fagioli
Bernhard Hommel
Ricarda Ines Schubotz
Publicatiedatum
01-01-2007
Uitgeverij
Springer-Verlag
Gepubliceerd in
Psychological Research / Uitgave 1/2007
Print ISSN: 0340-0727
Elektronisch ISSN: 1430-2772
DOI
https://doi.org/10.1007/s00426-005-0033-3

Andere artikelen Uitgave 1/2007

Psychological Research 1/2007 Naar de uitgave