Introduction
Although interventions to improve social cognitive skills are widely used for children with Autism Spectrum Disorder (autism from here on) the effects are variable and, even in successful trials, not all children respond to the same extent (Chester et al.
2019; Fletcher-Watson et al.
2014). Given the heterogeneity of autism, recent reports have highlighted the importance of investigating mediators and moderators of intervention (Vivanti et al.
2018) and the need to identify individual factors that predict treatment response (Hudry et al.
2018). However, there has been little systematic research on how family and child characteristics predict treatment response in autism. The current study aimed to investigate whether sibling configuration may moderate the effectiveness of a social cognition training aimed at improving Theory of Mind (ToM) skills in autistic children
1.
ToM refers to the understanding of mental states, such as desires, beliefs, emotions, and intentions, and how these are related to behavior. As such, it is generally considered a cornerstone of social competence (e.g. Wellman
2017). Indeed, ToM has been linked to many different aspects of children’s social behavior, including joint planning (e.g. proposals for what to play next) and role assignment (e.g. assigning a pretend play role to themselves or another child; Jenkins and Astington
2000); the ability to play games such as hide-and-seek; keeping secrets (Peskin and Ardino
2003), as well as having mutually reciprocated friendships (Fink et al.
2015). Although there are large individual differences in age of ToM acquisition, typically developing children generally acquire basic ToM understanding in the pre-school years (Wellman
2017). In contrast, in autism, ToM development is often impaired or delayed (Hobson
2019). For this reason, several interventions have been developed to enhance ToM development in autistic children (e.g. Beaumont and Sofronoff
2008; Begeer et al.
2015; Einfeld et al.
2018) but, as for other social and/or cognitive treatment programmes, not all children benefit equally and little is known about the child and family characteristics that may moderate outcome.
In typically developing preschool children, sibling configuration (i.e. number and ages of siblings) has been linked to ToM development, with better ToM performance associated with larger family size (e.g. Matthews and Goldberg
2018; Perner et al.
1994), having a “child-aged” sibling (i.e. aged between 12 months and 12 years; McAlister and Peterson
2006,
2007,
2013; Peterson
2000), and having an older sibling (Ruffman et al.
1998). A meta-analysis of studies exploring the relation between siblings and ToM ability in typically developing children indicated a positive relationship between family size and ToM development, and this relationship was strongest when siblings were “child-aged” (Devine and Hughes
2018). A study by Kennedy et al. (
2015), involving 4- to 11-year old children, also reported a positive effect of older siblings on ToM task performance. The most common explanation for these findings is that interactions with siblings provide more opportunities for ToM development (e.g. Devine and Hughes
2018; McAlister and Peterson
2007), with older brothers and sisters providing the greatest advantage by serving as role-models or even mentors for their younger siblings (Kennedy et al.
2015).
There is some evidence that ToM ability in autistic children may also be related to sibling configuration, although the findings are somewhat inconsistent. For instance, one study (O’Brien et al.
2011) found that autistic children with an older sibling scored lower on ToM tasks than those with younger or no siblings. The authors suggest that, by trying to over-compensate for the social impairments of their younger autistic siblings, older siblings may limit the siblings’ social–cognitive growth. However, other research has reported enhanced ToM acquisition in autistic children with older siblings (Matthews et al.
2013; Matthews and Goldberg
2018). These authors argued that children with autism may benefit from scaffolding provided by older siblings who have more mature mentalizing abilities (Matthews et al.
2013; Matthews and Goldberg
2018).
Current uncertainty about the potential role of siblings in ToM development in autism inspired us to use the data from our randomized control trial (RCT) of a ToM intervention to explore whether having a(n older) sibling might be related to better treatment outcomes. Previous data from this RCT showed that ToM training improved children’s knowledge of ToM. ToM-related behavior (e.g., understanding a joke, comforting somebody, asking about someone’s feelings) also increased and autistic features decreased (Begeer et al.
2015). A larger sample of participants from the same RCT has since become available, allowing us to test the potential moderating effects of sibling configuration. On the basis of existing literature, we hypothesized that, post ToM training:
(1)
having more siblings would be positively related to ToM outcomes. This is because having more siblings would provide autistic children with greater opportunity to practice the taught skills in the home environment (e.g. by means of the homework assignments), and
(2)
children with at least one older sibling would show better ToM outcomes than those without an older sibling. This is because older siblings are expected to have more mature mentalizing abilities allowing them better to support their younger autistic siblings while practicing the taught skills.
Discussion
The current study aimed to explore whether sibling configuration was related to ToM outcomes after a ToM intervention for autistic children. Results partly confirmed our hypotheses: both having more siblings as well as having an older sibling were related to better outcomes on measures of ToM-related behavior (e.g., understanding a joke, comforting somebody, asking about someone’s feelings) and social cognition. However, sibling configuration was not associated with treatment outcomes related to ToM knowledge or autistic features as measured by the SRS.
The findings related to ToM-related behavior and social cognition concur with the hypothesized beneficial effects of having a(n older) sibling and these effects are likely attributable to siblings providing enhanced opportunities for ToM development in both autistic and typically developing children (e.g. Devine and Hughes
2018; McAlister and Peterson
2007; Matthews and Goldberg
2018; Perner et al.
1994). In general, sibling interaction has been shown to provide valuable opportunities to practice new skills (for an overview, see Tzuriel and Hanuka-Levy
2014) and having a greater number of siblings also exposes children to a wider range of different perspectives. Older siblings may be particularly advantageous because they can offer higher levels of conversation and play, allowing for more sophisticated and explicit practice opportunities compared to younger siblings (Kennedy et al.
2015). Younger siblings might themselves lack sufficient ToM understanding to provide autistic children with rich opportunities for learning. Since having a(n older) sibling likely provides autistic children with more opportunities to practice the skills targeted in the intervention at home, it might be that these practical skills are particularly sensitive to sibling interaction whereas the input from (older) siblings is less likely to have an impact on (theoretical) ToM knowledge or overall autistic features.
Although, in the current sample, having an older sibling was related to better ToM outcomes, this has not been consistently replicated. A key variable here may be the receptive verbal ability of the children involved in therapy. For example, in the study by O’Brien et al. (
2011), which found an adverse effect of having an older sibling, participants had less advanced receptive verbal abilities than those in the current sample or in two other studies that reported a beneficial effects of having an older sibling (Matthews et al.
2013; Matthews and Goldberg
2018). Thus, having at least average receptive verbal ability may be a prerequisite for autistic children to benefit from their interactions with an older sibling.
However, an alternative interpretation of our results suggests the potential role of parents. For example, parents who are more experienced (i.e. previously had at least one child before the birth of the child with autism) may have had the opportunity to acquire parenting skills that help in consolidating the skills taught during the intervention (cf. Ben-Itzchak et al.
2019). Future studies should include measures of sibling and parenting characteristics to explore the possible impact of these family variables.
An advantage of the current study is the relatively large sample of autistic children from a randomized controlled trial, allowing us to investigate the relation between several sibling variables and ToM outcomes post intervention. Nevertheless, there are also several limitations. First, our sample included few participants with more than one or two siblings. In addition to limiting the generalizability of our findings to larger families, this lack of power for some of the sibling status subsamples might also have influenced our results. Having relatively few large families in our sample also prevented us from investigating nonlinear relationships between the number of siblings and ToM outcomes. It is possible that in large families the presumed beneficial effect of number of siblings is limited or even reversed. For example, in families with more than three children, siblings have ample opportunities to interact with each other while excluding the child with autism. Alternatively, the availability of many siblings might increase the likelihood of ‘overhelping’, thereby limiting the learning opportunities of the child with autism. Future studies incorporating more large families could help to investigate such potential relationships.
Second, while sibling autism status [i.e. having an autism diagnosis or aspects of the broader autism phenotype (BAP)] or gender were not included as moderators in our study due to small samples, comparing our analyses with and without autistic siblings showed all outcomes were the same. Previous studies have found that the association between ToM development in children with autism and sibling configuration was only found when the siblings did not have autism (Matthews and Goldberg
2018). Associations between sibling variables and ToM-related abilities may also vary according to the gender of both the autistic child and non-autistic sibling(s) (Sang and Nelson
2017). However, since almost 90% of our autistic participants were male, it was not possible to explore gender effects. Future studies should include samples large enough to determine whether the relation between sibling configuration and ToM outcomes post-intervention differs by sibling gender or autism diagnosis.
Overall, the results of the current study suggest that the outcomes of a ToM training for autistic children are better when the child has more and/or older siblings. However, these effects were limited to more practical ToM skills, as addressed in the intervention, which suggests that (older) siblings may provide more opportunities for children with autism to practice taught skills in the home environment. This finding also suggests the potential benefits of involving siblings in interventions for children with autism.
Publisher's Note
Springer Nature remains neutral with regard to jurisdictional claims in published maps and institutional affiliations.