Skip to main content
Top
Gepubliceerd in: Psychological Research 5/2012

01-09-2012 | Original Article

Practice makes transfer of motor skills imperfect

Auteurs: Arnaud Boutin, Arnaud Badets, Robin N. Salesse, Udo Fries, Stefan Panzer, Yannick Blandin

Gepubliceerd in: Psychological Research | Uitgave 5/2012

Log in om toegang te krijgen
share
DELEN

Deel dit onderdeel of sectie (kopieer de link)

  • Optie A:
    Klik op de rechtermuisknop op de link en selecteer de optie “linkadres kopiëren”
  • Optie B:
    Deel de link per e-mail

Abstract

We investigated the practice-effects on motor skill transfer and the associated representational memory changes that occur during the within-practice and between-practice phases. In two experiments, participants produced extension–flexion movements with their dominant right arm for a limited or prolonged practice session arranged in either a single- or multi-session format. We tested the ability of participants to transfer the original pattern (extrinsic transformation) or the mirrored one (intrinsic transformation) to the non-dominant left arm, 10 min and 24 h after the practice sessions. Results showed that practice induces rapid motor skill improvements that are non-transferable irrespective of the amount of acquisition trials. Furthermore, the extrinsic component of the skill develops early and remains the dominant coding system during practice. Conversely, we found distinct between-practice memory changes: a limited practice induces an off-line development of the extrinsic component, whereas a prolonged practice session subserves the off-line development of the intrinsic component (Experiment 2). We provided further evidence that the long-term representation of the motor skill also depends on the nature of the practice session itself: the parsing of practice into multiple sessions narrows the effector-transfer capacities in comparison to a single session (Experiment 1). These findings yield theoretical and practical implications that are discussed in the context of recent motor skill learning models.
Voetnoten
1
1. We compared retention and transfer performances of the NRT groups following a prolonged practice session arranged in a single and multisession format (Experiments 2 and 1, respectively). We conducted a 2 (practice formats: multisession, single session) × 3 (tests: retention, extrinsic transformation, intrinsic transformation) ANOVA with repeated measures on test using data from the NRT groups. The analysis revealed a main effect of test, F(2, 56) = 18.28, η p 2  = 0.39, and a practice format × test interaction, F(2, 56) = 3.46, η p 2  = 0.11. Duncan’s multiple range test indicated that RMSE was significantly higher on the intrinsic transformation test following multisession practice (M = 79.06) than following a single practice session (M = 66.96). No performance difference was observed between groups on the retention (M = 56.20 and 50.59 following a single and multisession practice, respectively) and extrinsic transformation tests (M = 65.69 and 68.06 following a single and multisession practice, respectively). The main effect of practice format was not significant, F(1, 28) < 1.
 
Literatuur
go back to reference Albouy, G., Sterpenich, V., Balteau, E., Vandewalle, G., Desseilles, M., Dang-Vu, T., et al. (2008). Both the hippocampus and striatum are involved in consolidation of motor sequence memory. Neuron, 58, 261–272.PubMedCrossRef Albouy, G., Sterpenich, V., Balteau, E., Vandewalle, G., Desseilles, M., Dang-Vu, T., et al. (2008). Both the hippocampus and striatum are involved in consolidation of motor sequence memory. Neuron, 58, 261–272.PubMedCrossRef
go back to reference Bapi, R. S., Doya, K., & Harner, A. M. (2000). Evidence for effector independent and dependent representations and their differential time course of acquisition during motor sequence learning. Experimental Brain Research, 132, 149–162.CrossRef Bapi, R. S., Doya, K., & Harner, A. M. (2000). Evidence for effector independent and dependent representations and their differential time course of acquisition during motor sequence learning. Experimental Brain Research, 132, 149–162.CrossRef
go back to reference Boutin, A., Fries, U., Panzer, S., Shea, C. H., & Blandin, Y. (2010). Role of action observation and action in sequence learning and coding. Acta Psychologica, 135, 240–251.PubMedCrossRef Boutin, A., Fries, U., Panzer, S., Shea, C. H., & Blandin, Y. (2010). Role of action observation and action in sequence learning and coding. Acta Psychologica, 135, 240–251.PubMedCrossRef
go back to reference Butler, A. C. (2010). Repeated testing produces superior transfer of learning relative to repeated studying. Journal of Experimental Psychology. Learning, Memory, and Cognition, 36, 1118–1133.PubMedCrossRef Butler, A. C. (2010). Repeated testing produces superior transfer of learning relative to repeated studying. Journal of Experimental Psychology. Learning, Memory, and Cognition, 36, 1118–1133.PubMedCrossRef
go back to reference Carpenter, S. K. (2009). Cue strength as a moderator of the testing effect: The benefits of elaborative retrieval. Journal of Experimental Psychology. Learning, Memory, and Cognition, 35, 1563–1569.PubMedCrossRef Carpenter, S. K. (2009). Cue strength as a moderator of the testing effect: The benefits of elaborative retrieval. Journal of Experimental Psychology. Learning, Memory, and Cognition, 35, 1563–1569.PubMedCrossRef
go back to reference Censor, N., Dimyan, M. A., & Cohen, L. G. (2010). Modification of existing human motor memories is enabled by primary cortical processing during memory reactivation. Current Biology, 20, 1545–1649.PubMedCrossRef Censor, N., Dimyan, M. A., & Cohen, L. G. (2010). Modification of existing human motor memories is enabled by primary cortical processing during memory reactivation. Current Biology, 20, 1545–1649.PubMedCrossRef
go back to reference Cohen, J. (1988). Statistical power analysis for the behavioral sciences (2nd ed.). Hilsdale, Nj: Erlbaum. Cohen, J. (1988). Statistical power analysis for the behavioral sciences (2nd ed.). Hilsdale, Nj: Erlbaum.
go back to reference Cohen, D. A., Pascual-Leone, A., Press, D. Z., & Robertson, E. M. (2005). Off-line learning of motor skill memory: a double dissociation of goal and movement. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America, 102, 18237–18241.PubMedCrossRef Cohen, D. A., Pascual-Leone, A., Press, D. Z., & Robertson, E. M. (2005). Off-line learning of motor skill memory: a double dissociation of goal and movement. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America, 102, 18237–18241.PubMedCrossRef
go back to reference Colby, C. L., & Goldberg, M. E. (1999). Space and attention in parietal cortex. Annual Review of Neuroscience, 22, 319–349.PubMedCrossRef Colby, C. L., & Goldberg, M. E. (1999). Space and attention in parietal cortex. Annual Review of Neuroscience, 22, 319–349.PubMedCrossRef
go back to reference Criscimagna-Hemminger, S. E., Donchin, O., Gazzaniga, M. S., & Shadmehr, R. (2003). Learned dynamics of reaching movements generalize from dominant to nondominant arm. Journal of Neurophysiology, 89, 168–176.PubMedCrossRef Criscimagna-Hemminger, S. E., Donchin, O., Gazzaniga, M. S., & Shadmehr, R. (2003). Learned dynamics of reaching movements generalize from dominant to nondominant arm. Journal of Neurophysiology, 89, 168–176.PubMedCrossRef
go back to reference Diekelmann, S., & Born, J. (2010). The memory function of sleep. Nature Reviews Neuroscience, 11, 114–126.PubMedCrossRef Diekelmann, S., & Born, J. (2010). The memory function of sleep. Nature Reviews Neuroscience, 11, 114–126.PubMedCrossRef
go back to reference Doyon, J., Song, A. W., Karni, A., Lalonde, F., Adams, M. M., & Ungerleider, L. G. (2002). Experience-dependent changes in cerebellar contributions to motor sequence learning. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America, 99, 1017–1022.PubMedCrossRef Doyon, J., Song, A. W., Karni, A., Lalonde, F., Adams, M. M., & Ungerleider, L. G. (2002). Experience-dependent changes in cerebellar contributions to motor sequence learning. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America, 99, 1017–1022.PubMedCrossRef
go back to reference Dudai, Y. (2006). Reconsolidation: the advantage of being refocused. Current Opinion in Neurobiology, 16, 174–178.PubMedCrossRef Dudai, Y. (2006). Reconsolidation: the advantage of being refocused. Current Opinion in Neurobiology, 16, 174–178.PubMedCrossRef
go back to reference Dudai, Y., & Eisenberg, M. (2004). Rites of passage of the engram: reconsolidation and the lingering consolidation hypothesis. Neuron, 44, 93–100.PubMedCrossRef Dudai, Y., & Eisenberg, M. (2004). Rites of passage of the engram: reconsolidation and the lingering consolidation hypothesis. Neuron, 44, 93–100.PubMedCrossRef
go back to reference Hikosaka, O., Nakahara, H., Rand, M. K., Sakai, K., Lu, X., Nakamura, K., et al. (1999). Parallel neural networks for learning sequential procedures. Trends in Neuroscience, 22, 464–471.CrossRef Hikosaka, O., Nakahara, H., Rand, M. K., Sakai, K., Lu, X., Nakamura, K., et al. (1999). Parallel neural networks for learning sequential procedures. Trends in Neuroscience, 22, 464–471.CrossRef
go back to reference Hikosaka, O., Rand, M. K., Nakamura, K., Miyachi, S., Kitaguchi, K., Sakai, K., et al. (2002). Long-term retention of motor skill in macaque monkeys and humans. Experimental Brain Research, 147, 494–504.CrossRef Hikosaka, O., Rand, M. K., Nakamura, K., Miyachi, S., Kitaguchi, K., Sakai, K., et al. (2002). Long-term retention of motor skill in macaque monkeys and humans. Experimental Brain Research, 147, 494–504.CrossRef
go back to reference Karni, A., Meyer, G., Jezzard, P., Adams, M. M., Turner, R., & Ungerleider, L. G. (1995). Functional MRI evidence for adult motor cortex plasticity during motor skill learning. Nature, 377, 155–158.PubMedCrossRef Karni, A., Meyer, G., Jezzard, P., Adams, M. M., Turner, R., & Ungerleider, L. G. (1995). Functional MRI evidence for adult motor cortex plasticity during motor skill learning. Nature, 377, 155–158.PubMedCrossRef
go back to reference Karni, A., Meyer, G., Rey-Hipolito, C., Jezzard, P., Adams, M. M., Turner, R., et al. (1998). The acquisition of skilled motor performance: fast and slow experience-driven changes in primary motor cortex. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America, 95, 861–868.PubMedCrossRef Karni, A., Meyer, G., Rey-Hipolito, C., Jezzard, P., Adams, M. M., Turner, R., et al. (1998). The acquisition of skilled motor performance: fast and slow experience-driven changes in primary motor cortex. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America, 95, 861–868.PubMedCrossRef
go back to reference Keele, S. W., Ivry, R., Mayr, U., Hazeltine, E., & Heuer, H. (2003). The cognitive and neural architecture of sequence representation. Psychological Review, 110, 316–339.PubMedCrossRef Keele, S. W., Ivry, R., Mayr, U., Hazeltine, E., & Heuer, H. (2003). The cognitive and neural architecture of sequence representation. Psychological Review, 110, 316–339.PubMedCrossRef
go back to reference Keele, S. W., Jennings, P., Jones, S., Caulton, D., & Cohen, A. (1995). On the modularity of sequence representation. Journal of Motor Behavior, 27, 17–30.CrossRef Keele, S. W., Jennings, P., Jones, S., Caulton, D., & Cohen, A. (1995). On the modularity of sequence representation. Journal of Motor Behavior, 27, 17–30.CrossRef
go back to reference Kleiner, M., Brainard, D., & Pelli, D. G. (2007). What’s new in Psychtoolbox-3? Perception, 36 (ECVP Abstract Supplement). Kleiner, M., Brainard, D., & Pelli, D. G. (2007). What’s new in Psychtoolbox-3? Perception, 36 (ECVP Abstract Supplement).
go back to reference Korman, M., Raz, N., Flash, T., & Karni, A. (2003). Multiple shifts in the representation of a motor sequence during the acquisition of skilled performance. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America, 100, 12492–12497.PubMedCrossRef Korman, M., Raz, N., Flash, T., & Karni, A. (2003). Multiple shifts in the representation of a motor sequence during the acquisition of skilled performance. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America, 100, 12492–12497.PubMedCrossRef
go back to reference Kovacs, A. J., Boyle, J., Gruetzmatcher, N., & Shea, C. H. (2010). Coding of on-line and pre-planned movement sequences. Acta Psychologica, 133, 119–126.PubMedCrossRef Kovacs, A. J., Boyle, J., Gruetzmatcher, N., & Shea, C. H. (2010). Coding of on-line and pre-planned movement sequences. Acta Psychologica, 133, 119–126.PubMedCrossRef
go back to reference Kovacs, A. J., Han, D.-W., & Shea, C. H. (2009a). The representation of movement sequences is related to task characteristics. Acta Psychologica, 132, 54–61.PubMedCrossRef Kovacs, A. J., Han, D.-W., & Shea, C. H. (2009a). The representation of movement sequences is related to task characteristics. Acta Psychologica, 132, 54–61.PubMedCrossRef
go back to reference Kovacs, A. J., Muehlbauer, T., & Shea, C. H. (2009b). The coding of movement sequences. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 35, 390–407.PubMedCrossRef Kovacs, A. J., Muehlbauer, T., & Shea, C. H. (2009b). The coding of movement sequences. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 35, 390–407.PubMedCrossRef
go back to reference Krakauer, J. W., Ghilardi, M. F., & Ghez, C. (1999). Independent learning of internal models for kinematic and dynamic control of reaching. Nature Neuroscience, 2, 1026–1031.PubMedCrossRef Krakauer, J. W., Ghilardi, M. F., & Ghez, C. (1999). Independent learning of internal models for kinematic and dynamic control of reaching. Nature Neuroscience, 2, 1026–1031.PubMedCrossRef
go back to reference Krakauer, J. W., & Shadmehr, R. (2006). Consolidation of motor memory. Trends in Neurosciences, 29, 58–64.PubMedCrossRef Krakauer, J. W., & Shadmehr, R. (2006). Consolidation of motor memory. Trends in Neurosciences, 29, 58–64.PubMedCrossRef
go back to reference Lange, R. K., Godde, B., & Braun, C. (2004). EEG correlates of coordinate processing during intermanual transfer. Experimental Brain Research, 159, 161–171.CrossRef Lange, R. K., Godde, B., & Braun, C. (2004). EEG correlates of coordinate processing during intermanual transfer. Experimental Brain Research, 159, 161–171.CrossRef
go back to reference Li, C. S. R., Padoa-Schioppa, C., & Bizzi, E. (2001). Neuronal correlates of motor performance and motor learning in the primary motor cortex of monkeys adapting to an external force field. Neuron, 30, 593–607.PubMedCrossRef Li, C. S. R., Padoa-Schioppa, C., & Bizzi, E. (2001). Neuronal correlates of motor performance and motor learning in the primary motor cortex of monkeys adapting to an external force field. Neuron, 30, 593–607.PubMedCrossRef
go back to reference McDaniel, M. A., & Masson, M. E. J. (1985). Alterning memory representations through retrieval. Journal of Experimental Psychology. Learning, Memory, and Cognition, 11, 371–385.CrossRef McDaniel, M. A., & Masson, M. E. J. (1985). Alterning memory representations through retrieval. Journal of Experimental Psychology. Learning, Memory, and Cognition, 11, 371–385.CrossRef
go back to reference McDaniel, M. A., Roediger, H. L., I. I. I., & McDermott, K. B. (2007). Generalizing test-enhanced learning from the laboratory to the classroom. Psychonomic Bulletin & Review, 14, 200–206.CrossRef McDaniel, M. A., Roediger, H. L., I. I. I., & McDermott, K. B. (2007). Generalizing test-enhanced learning from the laboratory to the classroom. Psychonomic Bulletin & Review, 14, 200–206.CrossRef
go back to reference Panzer, S., Krueger, M., Muehlbauer, T., Kovacs, A. J., & Shea, C. H. (2009). Inter-manual transfer and practice: coding of simple motor sequences. Acta Psychologica, 131, 99–109.PubMedCrossRef Panzer, S., Krueger, M., Muehlbauer, T., Kovacs, A. J., & Shea, C. H. (2009). Inter-manual transfer and practice: coding of simple motor sequences. Acta Psychologica, 131, 99–109.PubMedCrossRef
go back to reference Pelli, D. G. (1997). The VideoToolbox software for visual psychophysics: transforming numbers into movies. Spatial Vision, 10, 437–442.PubMedCrossRef Pelli, D. G. (1997). The VideoToolbox software for visual psychophysics: transforming numbers into movies. Spatial Vision, 10, 437–442.PubMedCrossRef
go back to reference Przybyslawski, J., & Sara, S. J. (1997). Reconsolidation of memory after its reactivation. Behavioural Brain Research, 84, 241–246.PubMedCrossRef Przybyslawski, J., & Sara, S. J. (1997). Reconsolidation of memory after its reactivation. Behavioural Brain Research, 84, 241–246.PubMedCrossRef
go back to reference Robertson, E. M. (2009). From creation to consolidation: A novel framework for memory processing. PLoS Biology, 7, 11–19.CrossRef Robertson, E. M. (2009). From creation to consolidation: A novel framework for memory processing. PLoS Biology, 7, 11–19.CrossRef
go back to reference Robertson, E. M., Pascual-Leone, A., & Miall, R. C. (2004a). Current concepts in procedural consolidation. Nature Reviews Neuroscience, 5, 576–582.PubMedCrossRef Robertson, E. M., Pascual-Leone, A., & Miall, R. C. (2004a). Current concepts in procedural consolidation. Nature Reviews Neuroscience, 5, 576–582.PubMedCrossRef
go back to reference Robertson, E. M., Pascual-Leone, A., & Press, D. Z. (2004b). Awareness modifies the skill-learning benefits of sleep. Current Biology, 14, 208–212.PubMed Robertson, E. M., Pascual-Leone, A., & Press, D. Z. (2004b). Awareness modifies the skill-learning benefits of sleep. Current Biology, 14, 208–212.PubMed
go back to reference Robertson, E. M., Press, D. Z., & Pascual-Leone, A. (2005). Off-line learning and the primary motor cortex. The Journal of Neuroscience, 25, 6372–6378.PubMedCrossRef Robertson, E. M., Press, D. Z., & Pascual-Leone, A. (2005). Off-line learning and the primary motor cortex. The Journal of Neuroscience, 25, 6372–6378.PubMedCrossRef
go back to reference Roediger, H. L., I. I. I., & Butler, A. C. (2010). The critical role of retrieval practice in long-term retention. Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 15, 20–27.PubMedCrossRef Roediger, H. L., I. I. I., & Butler, A. C. (2010). The critical role of retrieval practice in long-term retention. Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 15, 20–27.PubMedCrossRef
go back to reference Roediger, H. L., I. I. I., & Karpicke, J. D. (2006a). Test-enhanced learning: taking memory tests improves long-term retention. Psychological Science, 17, 249–255.PubMedCrossRef Roediger, H. L., I. I. I., & Karpicke, J. D. (2006a). Test-enhanced learning: taking memory tests improves long-term retention. Psychological Science, 17, 249–255.PubMedCrossRef
go back to reference Roediger, H. L., I. I. I., & Karpicke, J. D. (2006b). The power of testing memory: basic research and implications for educational practice. Perspectives on Psychological Science, 3, 181–210.CrossRef Roediger, H. L., I. I. I., & Karpicke, J. D. (2006b). The power of testing memory: basic research and implications for educational practice. Perspectives on Psychological Science, 3, 181–210.CrossRef
go back to reference Rohrer, D., Taylor, K., & Sholar, B. (2010). Tests enhance the transfer of learning. Journal of Experimental Psychology. Learning, Memory, and Cognition, 36, 233–239.PubMedCrossRef Rohrer, D., Taylor, K., & Sholar, B. (2010). Tests enhance the transfer of learning. Journal of Experimental Psychology. Learning, Memory, and Cognition, 36, 233–239.PubMedCrossRef
go back to reference Sainburg, R. L. (2005). Handedness: differential specializations for control of trajectory and position. Exercise and Sport Sciences Reviews, 33, 206–213.PubMedCrossRef Sainburg, R. L. (2005). Handedness: differential specializations for control of trajectory and position. Exercise and Sport Sciences Reviews, 33, 206–213.PubMedCrossRef
go back to reference Sanes, J. N., & Donoghue, J. P. (2000). Plasticity and primary motor cortex. Annual Review of Neuroscience, 23, 393–415.PubMedCrossRef Sanes, J. N., & Donoghue, J. P. (2000). Plasticity and primary motor cortex. Annual Review of Neuroscience, 23, 393–415.PubMedCrossRef
go back to reference Sara, S. J. (2000). Retrieval and reconsolidation: toward a neurobiology of remembering. Learning and Memory, 7, 73–84.PubMedCrossRef Sara, S. J. (2000). Retrieval and reconsolidation: toward a neurobiology of remembering. Learning and Memory, 7, 73–84.PubMedCrossRef
go back to reference Shadmehr, R., & Holcomb, H. H. (1997). Neural correlates of motor memory consolidation. Science, 277, 821–825.PubMedCrossRef Shadmehr, R., & Holcomb, H. H. (1997). Neural correlates of motor memory consolidation. Science, 277, 821–825.PubMedCrossRef
go back to reference Soechting, J. F., & Flanders, M. (1989). Errors in pointing are due to approximations in sensorimotor transformations. Journal of Neurophysiology, 62, 595–608.PubMed Soechting, J. F., & Flanders, M. (1989). Errors in pointing are due to approximations in sensorimotor transformations. Journal of Neurophysiology, 62, 595–608.PubMed
go back to reference Steele, C. J., & Penhune, V. B. (2010). Specific increases within global decreases: a functional magnetic resonance imaging investigation of five days of motor sequence learning. The Journal of Neuroscience, 30, 8332–8341.PubMedCrossRef Steele, C. J., & Penhune, V. B. (2010). Specific increases within global decreases: a functional magnetic resonance imaging investigation of five days of motor sequence learning. The Journal of Neuroscience, 30, 8332–8341.PubMedCrossRef
go back to reference Stickgold, R., & Walker, M. P. (2007). Sleep-dependent memory consolidation and reconsolidation. Sleep Medicine, 8, 331–343.PubMedCrossRef Stickgold, R., & Walker, M. P. (2007). Sleep-dependent memory consolidation and reconsolidation. Sleep Medicine, 8, 331–343.PubMedCrossRef
go back to reference Stöckel, T., Weigelt, M., & Krug (2011). Acquisition of a complex basketball dribbling task in school children as a function of bilateral practice order. Research Quarterly for Exercise and Sport, 82, 188–197. Stöckel, T., Weigelt, M., & Krug (2011). Acquisition of a complex basketball dribbling task in school children as a function of bilateral practice order. Research Quarterly for Exercise and Sport, 82, 188–197.
go back to reference Trempe, M., & Proteau, L. (2010). Distinct consolidation outcomes in a visuomotor adaptation task: off-line leaning and persistent after-effect. Brain and Cognition, 73, 135–145.PubMedCrossRef Trempe, M., & Proteau, L. (2010). Distinct consolidation outcomes in a visuomotor adaptation task: off-line leaning and persistent after-effect. Brain and Cognition, 73, 135–145.PubMedCrossRef
go back to reference Ungerleider, L. G., Doyon, J., & Karni, A. (2002). Imaging brain plasticity during motor skill learning. Neurobiology of Learning and Memory, 78, 553–564.PubMedCrossRef Ungerleider, L. G., Doyon, J., & Karni, A. (2002). Imaging brain plasticity during motor skill learning. Neurobiology of Learning and Memory, 78, 553–564.PubMedCrossRef
go back to reference Walker, M. P., Brakefield, T., Hobson, J. A., & Stickgold, R. (2003). Dissociable stages of human memory consolidation and reconsolidation. Nature, 425, 616–620.PubMedCrossRef Walker, M. P., Brakefield, T., Hobson, J. A., & Stickgold, R. (2003). Dissociable stages of human memory consolidation and reconsolidation. Nature, 425, 616–620.PubMedCrossRef
go back to reference Walker, M. P., & Stickgold, R. (2004). Sleep-dependent learning and memory consolidation. Neuron, 44, 121–133.PubMedCrossRef Walker, M. P., & Stickgold, R. (2004). Sleep-dependent learning and memory consolidation. Neuron, 44, 121–133.PubMedCrossRef
go back to reference Wang, J., & Sainburg, R. L. (2006). Interlimb transfer of visuomotor rotations depends on handedness. Experimental Brain Research, 175, 223–230.CrossRef Wang, J., & Sainburg, R. L. (2006). Interlimb transfer of visuomotor rotations depends on handedness. Experimental Brain Research, 175, 223–230.CrossRef
go back to reference Wheeler, M. A., Ewers, M., & Buonanno, J. F. (2003). Different rates of forgetting following study versus test trials. Memory, 11, 571–580.PubMedCrossRef Wheeler, M. A., Ewers, M., & Buonanno, J. F. (2003). Different rates of forgetting following study versus test trials. Memory, 11, 571–580.PubMedCrossRef
go back to reference Witt, K., Margraf, N., Bieber, C., Born, J., & Deuschl, G. (2010). Sleep consolidates the effector-independent representation of a motor skill. Neuroscience, 171, 227–234.PubMedCrossRef Witt, K., Margraf, N., Bieber, C., Born, J., & Deuschl, G. (2010). Sleep consolidates the effector-independent representation of a motor skill. Neuroscience, 171, 227–234.PubMedCrossRef
Metagegevens
Titel
Practice makes transfer of motor skills imperfect
Auteurs
Arnaud Boutin
Arnaud Badets
Robin N. Salesse
Udo Fries
Stefan Panzer
Yannick Blandin
Publicatiedatum
01-09-2012
Uitgeverij
Springer-Verlag
Gepubliceerd in
Psychological Research / Uitgave 5/2012
Print ISSN: 0340-0727
Elektronisch ISSN: 1430-2772
DOI
https://doi.org/10.1007/s00426-011-0355-2

Andere artikelen Uitgave 5/2012

Psychological Research 5/2012 Naar de uitgave