Skip to main content
Top
Gepubliceerd in: Psychological Research 3/2020

28-08-2018 | Original Article

Retrieval-mediated directed forgetting in the item-method paradigm: the effect of semantic cues

Auteurs: Ivan Marevic, Jan Rummel

Gepubliceerd in: Psychological Research | Uitgave 3/2020

Log in om toegang te krijgen
share
DELEN

Deel dit onderdeel of sectie (kopieer de link)

  • Optie A:
    Klik op de rechtermuisknop op de link en selecteer de optie “linkadres kopiëren”
  • Optie B:
    Deel de link per e-mail

Abstract

Item-method directed forgetting is widely considered a storage phenomenon. However, by applying a multinomial model, which separates storage and retrieval effect components, Rummel et al. (J Exp Psychol Learn Mem Cogn 42(10):1526–1543, 2016) recently provided evidence that item-method directed forgetting effects are reflected by both storage and retrieval changes. The current investigation demonstrates that supposedly intentionally forgotten information can still be retrieved to some extent when semantic cuing facilitates retrieval of this information. Participants studied word pairs, with some pairs being followed by a “forget” and others by a “remember” instruction. A subset of items shared the same superordinate semantic category. In Experiment 1, a sub-portion of to-be-forgotten items was semantically related and less forgetting occurred selectively for these items when the category was reinstated during test. This finding was replicated and extended to reinstatement effects for to-be-remembered items in Experiment 2. The application of the storage–retrieval model confirmed that providing a category cue facilitates retrieval of to-be-forgotten as well as to-be-remembered information. The results are discussed in light of existing theories of directed forgetting.
Bijlagen
Alleen toegankelijk voor geautoriseerde gebruikers
Voetnoten
1
Parameters s and u indicate singleton recall of items stored as pairs or singletons. Parameter l accounts for the possibility that some items are lost from memory during the delay between the first and the second memory test.
 
2
The present results would not have differed had the full sample of 81 participants been used.
 
3
We chose to reinstate the shared category for TBF and TBR items in separate experimental groups, because an initial pretest with N = 76, in which both TBF and TBR items shared two distinct superordinate categories for the same participants, suggested that participants noticed the relatedness of the items also in the absence of a category cue—probably because the proportion of items sharing a category was too high in the to-be-studied item set.
 
4
Note that in the Category-Cue Forget and No-Category-Cue Forget groups, semantically related items were post-cued as TBF and in the Category-Cue Remember and No-Category-Cue Remember Groups they were post-cued as TBR.
 
5
Setting the s and u parameters equal for each item type and condition resulted in a worse model fit, ∆G2(3) = 38.33, p < .001. The results of Experiment 1 did not change when we re-ran the analyses with the same restrictions as in Experiment 2.
 
6
When applying logistic mixed models with the predictors item type and an effect-coded contrast comparing semantically related items with the respective semantically unrelated items to the No-Category groups, the results are as follows: in the No-Category-Cue Forget groups of Experiments 1 and 2, there were no significant differences between the TBFSR and TBFUR items for free recall, z = 0.72, p = .467 and z = 1.45, p = .146, and cued recall,, z = 0.79, p = .429 and z = 0.51, p = .604. In the No-Category-Cue Remember group of Experiment 2, there also was no significant difference between the TBRSR and TBRUR items for free recall, z = 0.21, p = .831, and cued recall, z = 1.27, p = .203.
 
Literatuur
go back to reference Anderson, M. C. (2005). The role of inhibitory control in forgetting unwanted memories: A consideration of three methods. In C. M. MacLeod & B. Uttl (Eds.), Dynamic cognitive processes (pp. 159–190). Tokyo: Springer.CrossRef Anderson, M. C. (2005). The role of inhibitory control in forgetting unwanted memories: A consideration of three methods. In C. M. MacLeod & B. Uttl (Eds.), Dynamic cognitive processes (pp. 159–190). Tokyo: Springer.CrossRef
go back to reference Basden, B. H., & Basden, D. R. (1998). Directed forgetting: A contrast of methods and interpretations. In J. M. Golding & C. M. MacLeod (Eds.), Intentional forgetting: Interdisciplinary approaches (pp. 139–172). Mahwah: Erlbaum. Basden, B. H., & Basden, D. R. (1998). Directed forgetting: A contrast of methods and interpretations. In J. M. Golding & C. M. MacLeod (Eds.), Intentional forgetting: Interdisciplinary approaches (pp. 139–172). Mahwah: Erlbaum.
go back to reference Batchelder, W. H., & Riefer, D. M. (1986). The statistical analysis of a model for storage and retrieval processes in human memory. British Journal of Mathematical and Statistical Psychology, 39, 129–149.CrossRef Batchelder, W. H., & Riefer, D. M. (1986). The statistical analysis of a model for storage and retrieval processes in human memory. British Journal of Mathematical and Statistical Psychology, 39, 129–149.CrossRef
go back to reference Bäuml, K.-H. (2008). Inhibitory processes. In H. L. Roediger III (Ed.), Cognitive psychology of memory (pp. 195–220). Oxford: Elsevier. Bäuml, K.-H. (2008). Inhibitory processes. In H. L. Roediger III (Ed.), Cognitive psychology of memory (pp. 195–220). Oxford: Elsevier.
go back to reference Bjork, R. A. (1972). Theoretical implications of directed forgetting. In A. W. Melton & E. Martin (Eds.), Coding processes in human memory (pp. 217–235). Washington, DC: Winston. Bjork, R. A. (1972). Theoretical implications of directed forgetting. In A. W. Melton & E. Martin (Eds.), Coding processes in human memory (pp. 217–235). Washington, DC: Winston.
go back to reference Bjork, R. A. (1989). Retrieval inhibition as an adaptive mechanism in human memory. In H. L. Roediger III & F. I. M. Craik (Eds.), Varieties of memory and consciousness: Essays in honor of Endel Tulving (pp. 309–330). Hillsdale: Erlbaum. Bjork, R. A. (1989). Retrieval inhibition as an adaptive mechanism in human memory. In H. L. Roediger III & F. I. M. Craik (Eds.), Varieties of memory and consciousness: Essays in honor of Endel Tulving (pp. 309–330). Hillsdale: Erlbaum.
go back to reference Fawcett, J. M., Lawrence, M. A., & Taylor, T. L. (2016). The representational consequences of intentional forgetting: Impairments to both the probability and fidelity of long-term memory. Journal of Experimental Psychology: General, 145(1), 56–81. https://doi.org/10.1037/xge0000128.CrossRef Fawcett, J. M., Lawrence, M. A., & Taylor, T. L. (2016). The representational consequences of intentional forgetting: Impairments to both the probability and fidelity of long-term memory. Journal of Experimental Psychology: General, 145(1), 56–81. https://​doi.​org/​10.​1037/​xge0000128.CrossRef
go back to reference Goernert, P. N., & Larson, M. E. (1993). The initiation and release of retrieval inhibition. The Journal of General Psychology, 121(1), 61–66.CrossRef Goernert, P. N., & Larson, M. E. (1993). The initiation and release of retrieval inhibition. The Journal of General Psychology, 121(1), 61–66.CrossRef
go back to reference Golding, J. M., & MacLeod, C. M. (1998). Intentional forgetting: Interdisciplinary approaches. Mahwah: Erlbaum. Golding, J. M., & MacLeod, C. M. (1998). Intentional forgetting: Interdisciplinary approaches. Mahwah: Erlbaum.
go back to reference MacLeod, C. M. (1998). Directed forgetting. In J. M. Golding & C. M. MacLeod (Eds.), Intentional forgetting: Interdisciplinary approaches (pp. 1–57). Mahwah: Erlbaum. MacLeod, C. M. (1998). Directed forgetting. In J. M. Golding & C. M. MacLeod (Eds.), Intentional forgetting: Interdisciplinary approaches (pp. 1–57). Mahwah: Erlbaum.
go back to reference Mannhaupt, H.-R. (1983). Produktionsnormen für verbale Reaktionen zu 40 geläufigen Kategorien. Sprache & Kognition, 2(4), 264–278. Mannhaupt, H.-R. (1983). Produktionsnormen für verbale Reaktionen zu 40 geläufigen Kategorien. Sprache & Kognition, 2(4), 264–278.
go back to reference Murdock, B. B. (1962). The serial position curve of free recall. Journal of Experimental Psychology, 64, 482–488.CrossRef Murdock, B. B. (1962). The serial position curve of free recall. Journal of Experimental Psychology, 64, 482–488.CrossRef
go back to reference Riefer, D. M., & Batchelder, W. H. (1991a). Age differences in storage and retrieval: A multinomial modeling analysis. Bulletin of the Psychonomic Society, 29(5), 415–418.CrossRef Riefer, D. M., & Batchelder, W. H. (1991a). Age differences in storage and retrieval: A multinomial modeling analysis. Bulletin of the Psychonomic Society, 29(5), 415–418.CrossRef
go back to reference Riefer, D. M., & Batchelder, W. H. (1991b). Statistical inference for multinomial tree models. In J.-C. Falmagne & J.-P. Doignon (Eds.), Mathematical psychology: Current developments (pp. 313–336). Berlin: Springer.CrossRef Riefer, D. M., & Batchelder, W. H. (1991b). Statistical inference for multinomial tree models. In J.-C. Falmagne & J.-P. Doignon (Eds.), Mathematical psychology: Current developments (pp. 313–336). Berlin: Springer.CrossRef
go back to reference Rouder, J. N., & Batchelder, W. H. (1998). Multinomial models for measuring storage and retrieval processes in paired associate learning. In C. E. Dowling, F. S. Roberts & P. Theuns (Eds.), Recent progress in mathematical psychology: Psychophysics, knowledge, representation, cognition, and measurement (pp. 195–225). New York: Psychology Press. Rouder, J. N., & Batchelder, W. H. (1998). Multinomial models for measuring storage and retrieval processes in paired associate learning. In C. E. Dowling, F. S. Roberts & P. Theuns (Eds.), Recent progress in mathematical psychology: Psychophysics, knowledge, representation, cognition, and measurement (pp. 195–225). New York: Psychology Press.
go back to reference Sahakyan, L., Delaney, P. F., Foster, N. L., & Abushanab, B. (2014). List-method directed forgetting in cognitive and clinical research: A theoretical and methodological review. In B. H. Ross (Ed.), The psychology of learning and motivation (Vol. 59, pp. 131–190). New York: Elsevier Inc./Academic Press. https://doi.org/10.1016/b978-0-12-407187-2.00004-6.CrossRef Sahakyan, L., Delaney, P. F., Foster, N. L., & Abushanab, B. (2014). List-method directed forgetting in cognitive and clinical research: A theoretical and methodological review. In B. H. Ross (Ed.), The psychology of learning and motivation (Vol. 59, pp. 131–190). New York: Elsevier Inc./Academic Press. https://​doi.​org/​10.​1016/​b978-0-12-407187-2.​00004-6.CrossRef
go back to reference Singmann, H., & Kellen, D. (in press). An introduction to linear mixed modeling in experimental psychology. In D. H. Spieler & E. Schumacher (Eds.), New methods in neuroscience and cognitive psychology. Psychology Press. Singmann, H., & Kellen, D. (in press). An introduction to linear mixed modeling in experimental psychology. In D. H. Spieler & E. Schumacher (Eds.), New methods in neuroscience and cognitive psychology. Psychology Press.
go back to reference Watkins, O. C., & Watkins, M. J. (1975). Build up of proactive inhibition as a cue-overload effect. Journal of Experimental Psychology Human Learning and Memory, 1, 442–452.CrossRef Watkins, O. C., & Watkins, M. J. (1975). Build up of proactive inhibition as a cue-overload effect. Journal of Experimental Psychology Human Learning and Memory, 1, 442–452.CrossRef
Metagegevens
Titel
Retrieval-mediated directed forgetting in the item-method paradigm: the effect of semantic cues
Auteurs
Ivan Marevic
Jan Rummel
Publicatiedatum
28-08-2018
Uitgeverij
Springer Berlin Heidelberg
Gepubliceerd in
Psychological Research / Uitgave 3/2020
Print ISSN: 0340-0727
Elektronisch ISSN: 1430-2772
DOI
https://doi.org/10.1007/s00426-018-1085-5

Andere artikelen Uitgave 3/2020

Psychological Research 3/2020 Naar de uitgave