Skip to main content
Top
Gepubliceerd in: Psychological Research 3/2020

31-08-2018 | Original Article

A meta-analysis of contingent-capture effects

Auteurs: Christian Büsel, Martin Voracek, Ulrich Ansorge

Gepubliceerd in: Psychological Research | Uitgave 3/2020

Log in om toegang te krijgen
share
DELEN

Deel dit onderdeel of sectie (kopieer de link)

  • Optie A:
    Klik op de rechtermuisknop op de link en selecteer de optie “linkadres kopiëren”
  • Optie B:
    Deel de link per e-mail

Abstract

The present meta-analyses investigated the widely used contingent-capture protocol. Contingent-capture theory postulates that only top-down matching stimuli capture attention. Evidence comes from the contingent-capture protocol, in which participants search for a predefined target stimulus preceded by a spatial cue. The cue is typically uninformative of the target’s position but either presented at target position (valid condition) or away from the target (invalid condition). The common finding is that seemingly only top-down matching cues capture attention as shown by a selective cueing effect (faster responses in valid than invalid conditions) for cues with a feature similar to the searched-for target only, but not for cues without target-similar feature. The origin of this “contingent-capture effect” is, however, debated. One alternative explanation is that intertrial priming—the priming of attention capture by the cue in a given trial by attending to a feature-similar target in the preceding trial—mediates the contingent-capture effect. Alternatively, the rapid-disengagement account argues that all salient stimuli capture attention initially, but that the disengagement from non-matching cues is rapid. The present meta-analyses shed light on this debate by (a) identifying moderators of the size of reported contingent-capture effects (64 experiments) and (b) analyzing pure (blocked) versus mixed presentation of different targets as well as summarizing results of published intertrial priming studies (12 experiments) in the contingent-capture protocol. We found target-singleton versus non-singleton status and pure versus mixed presentation of different targets to be reliable moderators. Furthermore, results indicated the presence of publication bias. Otherwise, the contingent-capture theory was supported, but we discuss additional factors that must be taken into account for a full account of the results.
Voetnoten
1
With longer cue–target intervals exceeding about 300 ms, cueing effects often revert (for an overview, see Klein, 2000). However, regarding the contingent-capture effect, this reversion is typically only found with top-down matching abrupt onset cues and with non-matching cues but not with top-down matching color cues (Gibson & Amelio, 2000).
 
2
It should be noted here that the three-way interactions between cue color, target color, and validity (or cue location) yielded the same result in both Experiments 2 (N = 24) and 3 (N = 16): F(1, 22) = 34.33 of Folk and Remington (1998), an impossible result in the second case (see degrees of freedom). Although visual inspection of the figures does not imply an overly different result, the second F value appears to be a copy-and-paste error.
 
3
An alternative would have been to find a typical correlation between RTs of the conditions and then use this correlation to correct the effect size for each experiment. This alternative was not chosen for two reasons: first, the imputed correlation would again have been an estimate and hence probably not correct for each experiment; and, second, order of effect sizes from small to large would have remained unaffected. Therefore, a meta-analysis based on imputed correlations would have yielded relatively similar results.
 
4
For the interested reader, the interactions concerning the influence of intertrial priming on contingent-capture effects are easy to spot in the “Results” sections of all but one study, the study by Eimer and Kiss (2010). In Eimer and Kiss (2010), the interactions are reported in the “Discussion” sections on p. 956 (Experiment 1) and p. 960 (Experiment 2), respectively.
 
5
Lamy et al. labeled the cues distractors, arguing that they do not indicate the correct target position and, hence, are a distraction.
 
Literatuur
go back to reference *Adamo, M., Pun, C., & Ferber, S. (2010). Multiple attentional control settings influence late attentional selection but do not provide an early attentional filter. Cognitive Neuroscience, 1, 102–110.PubMedCrossRef *Adamo, M., Pun, C., & Ferber, S. (2010). Multiple attentional control settings influence late attentional selection but do not provide an early attentional filter. Cognitive Neuroscience, 1, 102–110.PubMedCrossRef
go back to reference *Adamo, M., Pun, C., Pratt, J., & Ferber, S. (2008). Your divided attention, please! The maintenance of multiple attentional control sets over distinct regions in space. Cognition, 107, 295–303.PubMedCrossRef *Adamo, M., Pun, C., Pratt, J., & Ferber, S. (2008). Your divided attention, please! The maintenance of multiple attentional control sets over distinct regions in space. Cognition, 107, 295–303.PubMedCrossRef
go back to reference *Adamo, M., Wozny, S., Pratt, J., & Ferber, S. (2010). Parallel, independent attentional control settings for colors and shapes. Attention, Perception, & Psychophysics, 72, 1730–1735.CrossRef *Adamo, M., Wozny, S., Pratt, J., & Ferber, S. (2010). Parallel, independent attentional control settings for colors and shapes. Attention, Perception, & Psychophysics, 72, 1730–1735.CrossRef
go back to reference *Anderson, B. A., & Folk, C. L. (2012). Dissociating location-specific inhibition and attention shifts: Evidence against the disengagement account of contingent capture. Attention, Perception, & Psychophysics, 74, 1183–1198.CrossRef *Anderson, B. A., & Folk, C. L. (2012). Dissociating location-specific inhibition and attention shifts: Evidence against the disengagement account of contingent capture. Attention, Perception, & Psychophysics, 74, 1183–1198.CrossRef
go back to reference Ansorge, U., & Becker, S. I. (2012). Automatic priming of attentional control by relevant colors. Attention, Perception, & Psychophysics, 74, 83–104.CrossRef Ansorge, U., & Becker, S. I. (2012). Automatic priming of attentional control by relevant colors. Attention, Perception, & Psychophysics, 74, 83–104.CrossRef
go back to reference *Ansorge, U., & Becker, S. I. (2014). Contingent capture in cueing: The role of color search templates and cue-target color relations. Psychological Research Psychologische Forschung, 78, 209–221.PubMedCrossRef *Ansorge, U., & Becker, S. I. (2014). Contingent capture in cueing: The role of color search templates and cue-target color relations. Psychological Research Psychologische Forschung, 78, 209–221.PubMedCrossRef
go back to reference *Ansorge, U., & Heumann, M. (2003). Top-down contingencies in peripheral cuing: The roles of color and location. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 29, 937–948.PubMed *Ansorge, U., & Heumann, M. (2003). Top-down contingencies in peripheral cuing: The roles of color and location. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 29, 937–948.PubMed
go back to reference *Ansorge, U., & Heumann, M. (2004). Peripheral cuing by abrupt-onset cues: The influence of color in S–R corresponding conditions. Acta Psychologica, 116, 115–143.PubMedCrossRef *Ansorge, U., & Heumann, M. (2004). Peripheral cuing by abrupt-onset cues: The influence of color in S–R corresponding conditions. Acta Psychologica, 116, 115–143.PubMedCrossRef
go back to reference Ansorge, U., & Horstmann, G. (2007). Preemptive control of attentional capture by colour: Evidence from trial-by-trial analyses and orderings of onsets of capture effects in reaction time distributions. Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology, 60, 952–975.CrossRef Ansorge, U., & Horstmann, G. (2007). Preemptive control of attentional capture by colour: Evidence from trial-by-trial analyses and orderings of onsets of capture effects in reaction time distributions. Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology, 60, 952–975.CrossRef
go back to reference Ansorge, U., Kiss, M., & Eimer, M. (2009). Goal-driven attentional capture by invisible colors: Evidence from event-related potentials. Psychonomic Bulletin & Review, 16, 648–653.CrossRef Ansorge, U., Kiss, M., & Eimer, M. (2009). Goal-driven attentional capture by invisible colors: Evidence from event-related potentials. Psychonomic Bulletin & Review, 16, 648–653.CrossRef
go back to reference *Ansorge, U., Kiss, M., Worschech, F., & Eimer, M. (2011). The initial stage of visual selection is controlled by top-down task set: New ERP evidence. Attention, Perception, & Psychophysics, 73, 113–122.CrossRef *Ansorge, U., Kiss, M., Worschech, F., & Eimer, M. (2011). The initial stage of visual selection is controlled by top-down task set: New ERP evidence. Attention, Perception, & Psychophysics, 73, 113–122.CrossRef
go back to reference Ansorge, U., Priess, H.-W., & Kerzel, D. (2013). Effects of relevant and irrelevant color singletons on inhibition of return and attentional capture. Attention, Perception, & Psychophysics, 75, 1687–1702.CrossRef Ansorge, U., Priess, H.-W., & Kerzel, D. (2013). Effects of relevant and irrelevant color singletons on inhibition of return and attentional capture. Attention, Perception, & Psychophysics, 75, 1687–1702.CrossRef
go back to reference Awh, E., Belopolsky, A. V., & Theeuwes, J. (2012). Top-down versus bottom-up attentional control: A failed theoretical dichotomy. Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 16, 437–443.PubMedPubMedCentralCrossRef Awh, E., Belopolsky, A. V., & Theeuwes, J. (2012). Top-down versus bottom-up attentional control: A failed theoretical dichotomy. Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 16, 437–443.PubMedPubMedCentralCrossRef
go back to reference Bacon, W. F., & Egeth, H. E. (1994). Overriding stimulus-driven attentional capture. Perception & Psychophysics, 55, 485–496.CrossRef Bacon, W. F., & Egeth, H. E. (1994). Overriding stimulus-driven attentional capture. Perception & Psychophysics, 55, 485–496.CrossRef
go back to reference Becker, S. I. (2010). The role of target–distractor relationships in guiding attention and the eyes in visual search. Journal of Experimental Psychology: General, 139, 247–265.CrossRef Becker, S. I. (2010). The role of target–distractor relationships in guiding attention and the eyes in visual search. Journal of Experimental Psychology: General, 139, 247–265.CrossRef
go back to reference *Becker, S. I., Folk, C. L., & Remington, R. W. (2010). The role of relational information in contingent capture. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 36, 1460–1476.PubMed *Becker, S. I., Folk, C. L., & Remington, R. W. (2010). The role of relational information in contingent capture. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 36, 1460–1476.PubMed
go back to reference *Becker, S. I., Folk, C. L., & Remington, R. W. (2013). Attentional capture does not depend on feature similarity, but on target-nontarget relations. Psychological Science, 24, 634–647.PubMedCrossRef *Becker, S. I., Folk, C. L., & Remington, R. W. (2013). Attentional capture does not depend on feature similarity, but on target-nontarget relations. Psychological Science, 24, 634–647.PubMedCrossRef
go back to reference Begg, C. B., & Mazumdar, M. (1994). Operating characteristics of a rank correlation test for publication bias. Biometrics, 50, 1088–1101.PubMedCrossRef Begg, C. B., & Mazumdar, M. (1994). Operating characteristics of a rank correlation test for publication bias. Biometrics, 50, 1088–1101.PubMedCrossRef
go back to reference *Belopolsky, A. V., Schreij, D., & Theeuwes, J. (2010). What is top-down about contingent capture? Attention, Perception, & Psychophysics, 72, 326–341.CrossRef *Belopolsky, A. V., Schreij, D., & Theeuwes, J. (2010). What is top-down about contingent capture? Attention, Perception, & Psychophysics, 72, 326–341.CrossRef
go back to reference Borenstein, M., Hedges, L. V., Higgins, J. P. T., & Rothstein, H. R. (2009). Introduction to meta-analysis. Chichester: Wiley.CrossRef Borenstein, M., Hedges, L. V., Higgins, J. P. T., & Rothstein, H. R. (2009). Introduction to meta-analysis. Chichester: Wiley.CrossRef
go back to reference Borenstein, M., Higgins, J., Hedges, L. V., & Rothstein, H. R. (2017). Basics of meta-analysis: I2 is not an absolute measure of heterogeneity. Research Synthesis Methods, 8, 5–18.PubMedCrossRef Borenstein, M., Higgins, J., Hedges, L. V., & Rothstein, H. R. (2017). Basics of meta-analysis: I2 is not an absolute measure of heterogeneity. Research Synthesis Methods, 8, 5–18.PubMedCrossRef
go back to reference Braver, S. L., Thoemmes, F. J., & Rosenthal, R. (2014). Continuously cumulating meta-analysis and replicability. Perspectives on Psychological Science, 9, 333–343.PubMedCrossRef Braver, S. L., Thoemmes, F. J., & Rosenthal, R. (2014). Continuously cumulating meta-analysis and replicability. Perspectives on Psychological Science, 9, 333–343.PubMedCrossRef
go back to reference Burnham, B. R. (2007). Displaywide visual features associated with a search display’s appearance can mediate attentional capture. Psychonomic Bulletin & Review, 14, 392–422.CrossRef Burnham, B. R. (2007). Displaywide visual features associated with a search display’s appearance can mediate attentional capture. Psychonomic Bulletin & Review, 14, 392–422.CrossRef
go back to reference *Burnham, B. R., Harris, A. M., & Suda, M. T. (2011). Relationship between working memory capacity and contingent involuntary orienting. Visual Cognition, 19, 983–1002.CrossRef *Burnham, B. R., Harris, A. M., & Suda, M. T. (2011). Relationship between working memory capacity and contingent involuntary orienting. Visual Cognition, 19, 983–1002.CrossRef
go back to reference *Carmel, T., & Lamy, D. (2014). The same-location cost is unrelated to attentional settings: An object-updating account. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 40, 1465–1478.PubMed *Carmel, T., & Lamy, D. (2014). The same-location cost is unrelated to attentional settings: An object-updating account. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 40, 1465–1478.PubMed
go back to reference *Carmel, T., & Lamy, D. (2015). Towards a resolution of the attentional-capture debate. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 41, 1772–1782.PubMed *Carmel, T., & Lamy, D. (2015). Towards a resolution of the attentional-capture debate. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 41, 1772–1782.PubMed
go back to reference *Chen, P., & Mordkoff, J. T. (2007). Contingent capture at a very short SOA: Evidence against rapid disengagement. Visual Cognition, 15, 637–646.CrossRef *Chen, P., & Mordkoff, J. T. (2007). Contingent capture at a very short SOA: Evidence against rapid disengagement. Visual Cognition, 15, 637–646.CrossRef
go back to reference Cohen, J. (1988). Statistical power analysis for the behavioral sciences. New York: Routledge Academic. Cohen, J. (1988). Statistical power analysis for the behavioral sciences. New York: Routledge Academic.
go back to reference Duncan, J., & Humphreys, G. W. (1989). Visual search and stimulus similarity. Psychological Review, 96, 433–458.PubMedCrossRef Duncan, J., & Humphreys, G. W. (1989). Visual search and stimulus similarity. Psychological Review, 96, 433–458.PubMedCrossRef
go back to reference Duval, S., & Tweedie, R. (2000). Trim and fill: A simple funnel-plot-based method of testing and adjusting for publication bias in meta-analysis. Biometrics, 56, 455–463.PubMedCrossRef Duval, S., & Tweedie, R. (2000). Trim and fill: A simple funnel-plot-based method of testing and adjusting for publication bias in meta-analysis. Biometrics, 56, 455–463.PubMedCrossRef
go back to reference Egger, M., Smith, G. D., Schneider, M., & Minder, C. (1997). Bias in meta-analysis detected by a simple, graphical test. British Medical Journal, 315, 629–634.PubMedCrossRefPubMedCentral Egger, M., Smith, G. D., Schneider, M., & Minder, C. (1997). Bias in meta-analysis detected by a simple, graphical test. British Medical Journal, 315, 629–634.PubMedCrossRefPubMedCentral
go back to reference Eimer, M., & Kiss, M. (2008). Involuntary attentional capture is determined by task set: Evidence from event-related potentials. Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience, 20, 1423–1433.PubMedPubMedCentralCrossRef Eimer, M., & Kiss, M. (2008). Involuntary attentional capture is determined by task set: Evidence from event-related potentials. Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience, 20, 1423–1433.PubMedPubMedCentralCrossRef
go back to reference *Eimer, M., & Kiss, M. (2010). Top-down search strategies determine attentional capture in visual search: Behavioral and electrophysiological evidence. Attention, Perception, & Psychophysics, 72, 951–962.CrossRef *Eimer, M., & Kiss, M. (2010). Top-down search strategies determine attentional capture in visual search: Behavioral and electrophysiological evidence. Attention, Perception, & Psychophysics, 72, 951–962.CrossRef
go back to reference Fecteau, J. H. (2007). Priming of pop-out depends on the current goals of observers. Journal of Vision, 7(6), 1.PubMedCrossRef Fecteau, J. H. (2007). Priming of pop-out depends on the current goals of observers. Journal of Vision, 7(6), 1.PubMedCrossRef
go back to reference *Folk, C. L., & Anderson, B. A. (2010). Target-uncertainty effects in attentional capture: Color-singleton set or multiple attentional control settings? Psychonomic Bulletin & Review, 13, 421–426.CrossRef *Folk, C. L., & Anderson, B. A. (2010). Target-uncertainty effects in attentional capture: Color-singleton set or multiple attentional control settings? Psychonomic Bulletin & Review, 13, 421–426.CrossRef
go back to reference Folk, C. L., Leber, A. B., & Egeth, H. E. (2002). Made you blink! Contingent attentional capture produces a spatial blink. Attention, Perception, & Psychophysics, 64, 741–753.CrossRef Folk, C. L., Leber, A. B., & Egeth, H. E. (2002). Made you blink! Contingent attentional capture produces a spatial blink. Attention, Perception, & Psychophysics, 64, 741–753.CrossRef
go back to reference *Folk, C. L., & Remington, R. (1998). Selectivity in distraction by irrelevant featural singletons: Evidence for two forms of attentional capture. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 24, 847–858.PubMed *Folk, C. L., & Remington, R. (1998). Selectivity in distraction by irrelevant featural singletons: Evidence for two forms of attentional capture. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 24, 847–858.PubMed
go back to reference *Folk, C. L., & Remington, R. W. (2008). Bottom-up priming of top-down attentional control settings. Visual Cognition, 16, 215–231.CrossRef *Folk, C. L., & Remington, R. W. (2008). Bottom-up priming of top-down attentional control settings. Visual Cognition, 16, 215–231.CrossRef
go back to reference *Folk, C. L., Remington, R. W., & Johnston, J. C. (1992). Involuntary covert orienting is contingent on attentional control settings. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 18, 1030–1044.PubMed *Folk, C. L., Remington, R. W., & Johnston, J. C. (1992). Involuntary covert orienting is contingent on attentional control settings. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 18, 1030–1044.PubMed
go back to reference Gaspelin, N., Ruthruff, E., & Lien, M. C. (2016). The problem of latent attentional capture: Easy visual search conceals capture by task-irrelevant abrupt onsets. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 42, 1104–1120.PubMed Gaspelin, N., Ruthruff, E., & Lien, M. C. (2016). The problem of latent attentional capture: Easy visual search conceals capture by task-irrelevant abrupt onsets. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 42, 1104–1120.PubMed
go back to reference *Gaspelin, N., Ruthruff, E., Lien, M. C., & Jung, K. (2012). Breaking through the attentional window: Capture by abrupt onsets versus color singletons. Attention, Perception, & Psychophysics, 74, 1461–1474.CrossRef *Gaspelin, N., Ruthruff, E., Lien, M. C., & Jung, K. (2012). Breaking through the attentional window: Capture by abrupt onsets versus color singletons. Attention, Perception, & Psychophysics, 74, 1461–1474.CrossRef
go back to reference Gibson, B. S., & Amelio, J. (2000). Inhibition of return and attentional control settings. Perception & Psychophysics, 62, 496–504.CrossRef Gibson, B. S., & Amelio, J. (2000). Inhibition of return and attentional control settings. Perception & Psychophysics, 62, 496–504.CrossRef
go back to reference Goh, J. X., Hall, J. A., & Rosenthal, R. (2016). Mini meta-analysis of your own studies: Some arguments on why and a primer on how. Social and Personality Psychology Compass, 10, 535–549.CrossRef Goh, J. X., Hall, J. A., & Rosenthal, R. (2016). Mini meta-analysis of your own studies: Some arguments on why and a primer on how. Social and Personality Psychology Compass, 10, 535–549.CrossRef
go back to reference *Goller, F., & Ansorge, U. (2015). There is more to trial history than priming in attentional capture experiments. Attention, Perception, & Psychophysics, 77, 1574–1584.CrossRef *Goller, F., & Ansorge, U. (2015). There is more to trial history than priming in attentional capture experiments. Attention, Perception, & Psychophysics, 77, 1574–1584.CrossRef
go back to reference *Goller, F., Ditye, T., & Ansorge, U. (2016). The contribution of color to attention capture effects during search for onset targets. Attention, Perception, & Psychophysics, 78, 789–807.CrossRef *Goller, F., Ditye, T., & Ansorge, U. (2016). The contribution of color to attention capture effects during search for onset targets. Attention, Perception, & Psychophysics, 78, 789–807.CrossRef
go back to reference Grubert, A., & Eimer, M. (2013). Qualitative differences in the guidance of attention during single-colour and multiple-colour visual search: Behavioural and electrophysiological evidence. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 39, 1433–1442.PubMed Grubert, A., & Eimer, M. (2013). Qualitative differences in the guidance of attention during single-colour and multiple-colour visual search: Behavioural and electrophysiological evidence. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 39, 1433–1442.PubMed
go back to reference *Grubert, A., & Eimer, M. (2016). All set, indeed! N2pc components reveal simultaneous attentional control settings for multiple target colors. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 42, 1215–1230.PubMed *Grubert, A., & Eimer, M. (2016). All set, indeed! N2pc components reveal simultaneous attentional control settings for multiple target colors. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 42, 1215–1230.PubMed
go back to reference Grubert, A., Righi, L. L., & Eimer, M. (2013). A unitary focus of spatial attention during attentional capture: Evidence from event-related brain potentials. Journal of Vision, 13, 9.PubMedCrossRef Grubert, A., Righi, L. L., & Eimer, M. (2013). A unitary focus of spatial attention during attentional capture: Evidence from event-related brain potentials. Journal of Vision, 13, 9.PubMedCrossRef
go back to reference Harris, A. M., Becker, S.I., & Remington, R. W. (2015). Capture by colour: Evidence for dimension-specific singleton capture. Attention, Perception, & Psychophysics, 77, 2305–2321.CrossRef Harris, A. M., Becker, S.I., & Remington, R. W. (2015). Capture by colour: Evidence for dimension-specific singleton capture. Attention, Perception, & Psychophysics, 77, 2305–2321.CrossRef
go back to reference *Harris, A. M., Dux, P. E., Jones, C. N., & Mattingley, J. B. (2017). Distinct roles of theta and alpha oscillations in the involuntary capture of goal-directed attention. NeuroImage, 152, 171–183.PubMedCrossRef *Harris, A. M., Dux, P. E., Jones, C. N., & Mattingley, J. B. (2017). Distinct roles of theta and alpha oscillations in the involuntary capture of goal-directed attention. NeuroImage, 152, 171–183.PubMedCrossRef
go back to reference Higgins, J. P., Thompson, S. G., Deeks, J. J., & Altman, D. G. (2003). Measuring inconsistency in meta-analyses. British Medical Journal, 327, 557–560.PubMedCrossRefPubMedCentral Higgins, J. P., Thompson, S. G., Deeks, J. J., & Altman, D. G. (2003). Measuring inconsistency in meta-analyses. British Medical Journal, 327, 557–560.PubMedCrossRefPubMedCentral
go back to reference Ioannidis, J. P. A., Munafò, M. R., Fusar-Poli, P., Nosek, B. A., & David, S. P. (2014). Publication and other reporting biases in cognitive sciences: Detection, prevalence and prevention. Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 18, 235–241.PubMedPubMedCentralCrossRef Ioannidis, J. P. A., Munafò, M. R., Fusar-Poli, P., Nosek, B. A., & David, S. P. (2014). Publication and other reporting biases in cognitive sciences: Detection, prevalence and prevention. Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 18, 235–241.PubMedPubMedCentralCrossRef
go back to reference *Irons, J. L., Folk, C. L., & Remington, R. W. (2012). All set! Evidence of simultaneous attentional control settings for multiple target colors. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 38, 758–775.PubMed *Irons, J. L., Folk, C. L., & Remington, R. W. (2012). All set! Evidence of simultaneous attentional control settings for multiple target colors. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 38, 758–775.PubMed
go back to reference Irons, J. L., & Remington, R. W. (2013). Can attentional control settings be maintained for two color-location conjunctions? Evidence from an RSVP task. Attention, Perception, & Psychophysics, 75, 862–875.CrossRef Irons, J. L., & Remington, R. W. (2013). Can attentional control settings be maintained for two color-location conjunctions? Evidence from an RSVP task. Attention, Perception, & Psychophysics, 75, 862–875.CrossRef
go back to reference Itti, L., Koch, C., & Niebur, E. (1998). A model of saliency-based visual attention for rapid scene analysis. IEEE Transactions on Pattern Analysis and Machine Intelligence, 20, 1254–1259.CrossRef Itti, L., Koch, C., & Niebur, E. (1998). A model of saliency-based visual attention for rapid scene analysis. IEEE Transactions on Pattern Analysis and Machine Intelligence, 20, 1254–1259.CrossRef
go back to reference Jonides, J. (1981). Voluntary versus automatic control over the mind’s eye. In J. Long & A. Baddeley (Eds.), Attention and performance IX (pp. 187–203). Hillsdale: Erlbaum. Jonides, J. (1981). Voluntary versus automatic control over the mind’s eye. In J. Long & A. Baddeley (Eds.), Attention and performance IX (pp. 187–203). Hillsdale: Erlbaum.
go back to reference Kerzel, D., & Barras, C. (2016). Distractor rejection in visual search breaks down with more than a single distractor feature. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 42, 648–657.PubMed Kerzel, D., & Barras, C. (2016). Distractor rejection in visual search breaks down with more than a single distractor feature. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 42, 648–657.PubMed
go back to reference *Kiss, M., Grubert, A., & Eimer, M. (2013). Top-down task sets for combined features: Behavioral and electrophysiological evidence for two stages in attentional object selection. Attention, Perception, & Psychophysics, 75, 216–228.CrossRef *Kiss, M., Grubert, A., & Eimer, M. (2013). Top-down task sets for combined features: Behavioral and electrophysiological evidence for two stages in attentional object selection. Attention, Perception, & Psychophysics, 75, 216–228.CrossRef
go back to reference Kiss, M., Grubert, A., Petersen, A., & Eimer, M. (2012). Attentional capture by salient distractors during visual search is determined by temporal task demands. Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience, 24, 749–759.PubMedCrossRef Kiss, M., Grubert, A., Petersen, A., & Eimer, M. (2012). Attentional capture by salient distractors during visual search is determined by temporal task demands. Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience, 24, 749–759.PubMedCrossRef
go back to reference Kristjánsson, Á, & Campana, G. (2010). Where perception meets memory: A review of repetition priming in visual search tasks. Attention, Perception, & Psychophysics, 72, 5–18.CrossRef Kristjánsson, Á, & Campana, G. (2010). Where perception meets memory: A review of repetition priming in visual search tasks. Attention, Perception, & Psychophysics, 72, 5–18.CrossRef
go back to reference Krujne, W., Brascamp, J. W., Kristjánsson, Á, & Meeter, M. (2015). Can a single short-term mechanism account for priming of pop-out? Vision Research, 115, 17–22.CrossRef Krujne, W., Brascamp, J. W., Kristjánsson, Á, & Meeter, M. (2015). Can a single short-term mechanism account for priming of pop-out? Vision Research, 115, 17–22.CrossRef
go back to reference Kühberger, A., Fritz, A., & Scherndl, T. (2014). Publication bias in psychology: A diagnosis based on the correlation between effect size and sample size. PLoS One, 9, e105825.PubMedPubMedCentralCrossRef Kühberger, A., Fritz, A., & Scherndl, T. (2014). Publication bias in psychology: A diagnosis based on the correlation between effect size and sample size. PLoS One, 9, e105825.PubMedPubMedCentralCrossRef
go back to reference Lakens, D. (2013). Calculating and reporting effect sizes to facilitate cumulative science: A practical primer for t-tests and ANOVAs. Frontiers in Psychology, 4, 863.PubMedPubMedCentralCrossRef Lakens, D. (2013). Calculating and reporting effect sizes to facilitate cumulative science: A practical primer for t-tests and ANOVAs. Frontiers in Psychology, 4, 863.PubMedPubMedCentralCrossRef
go back to reference Lamy, D., & Egeth, H. E. (2003). Attentional capture in singleton-detection and feature-search modes. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 29, 1003–1020.PubMed Lamy, D., & Egeth, H. E. (2003). Attentional capture in singleton-detection and feature-search modes. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 29, 1003–1020.PubMed
go back to reference Lamy, D. F., & Kristjánsson, Á (2013). Is goal-directed attentional guidance just intertrial priming? A review. Journal of Vision, 13, 14.PubMedCrossRef Lamy, D. F., & Kristjánsson, Á (2013). Is goal-directed attentional guidance just intertrial priming? A review. Journal of Vision, 13, 14.PubMedCrossRef
go back to reference *Lamy, D., Leber, A., & Egeth, H. E. (2004). Effects of task relevance and stimulus-driven salience in feature-search mode. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 30, 1019–1031.PubMed *Lamy, D., Leber, A., & Egeth, H. E. (2004). Effects of task relevance and stimulus-driven salience in feature-search mode. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 30, 1019–1031.PubMed
go back to reference Leber, A. B., & Egeth, H. E. (2006). Attention on autopilot: Past experience and attentional set. Visual Cognition, 14, 565–583.CrossRef Leber, A. B., & Egeth, H. E. (2006). Attention on autopilot: Past experience and attentional set. Visual Cognition, 14, 565–583.CrossRef
go back to reference *Liao, H. I., & Yeh, S. L. (2011). Interaction between stimulus-driven orienting and top-down modulation in attentional capture. Acta Psychologica, 138, 52–59.PubMedCrossRef *Liao, H. I., & Yeh, S. L. (2011). Interaction between stimulus-driven orienting and top-down modulation in attentional capture. Acta Psychologica, 138, 52–59.PubMedCrossRef
go back to reference Liao, H.-I., & Yeh, S.-L. (2013). Capturing attention is not that simple: Different mechanisms for stimulus-driven and contingent capture. Attention, Perception, & Psychophysics, 75, 1703–1714.CrossRef Liao, H.-I., & Yeh, S.-L. (2013). Capturing attention is not that simple: Different mechanisms for stimulus-driven and contingent capture. Attention, Perception, & Psychophysics, 75, 1703–1714.CrossRef
go back to reference *Lien, M. C., Ruthruff, E., Goodin, Z., & Remington, R. W. (2008). Contingent attentional capture by top-down control settings: Converging evidence from event-related potentials. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 34, 509–530.PubMed *Lien, M. C., Ruthruff, E., Goodin, Z., & Remington, R. W. (2008). Contingent attentional capture by top-down control settings: Converging evidence from event-related potentials. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 34, 509–530.PubMed
go back to reference *Lien, M. C., Ruthruff, E., & Johnston, J. C. (2010). Attentional capture with rapidly changing attentional control settings. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 36, 1–16.PubMed *Lien, M. C., Ruthruff, E., & Johnston, J. C. (2010). Attentional capture with rapidly changing attentional control settings. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 36, 1–16.PubMed
go back to reference Liu, T., & Jigo, M. (2017). Limits in feature-based attention to multiple colors. Attention, Perception, & Psychophysics, 79, 2327–2337.CrossRef Liu, T., & Jigo, M. (2017). Limits in feature-based attention to multiple colors. Attention, Perception, & Psychophysics, 79, 2327–2337.CrossRef
go back to reference Maljkovic, V., & Martini, P. (2005). Implicit short-term memory and event frequency effects in visual search. Vision Research, 45, 2831–2846.PubMedCrossRef Maljkovic, V., & Martini, P. (2005). Implicit short-term memory and event frequency effects in visual search. Vision Research, 45, 2831–2846.PubMedCrossRef
go back to reference Maljkovic, V., & Nakayama, K. (1994). Priming of pop-out: I. Role of features. Memory & Cognition, 22, 657–672.CrossRef Maljkovic, V., & Nakayama, K. (1994). Priming of pop-out: I. Role of features. Memory & Cognition, 22, 657–672.CrossRef
go back to reference *Mertes, C., Wascher, E., & Schneider, D. (2017). Compliance instead of flexibility? On age-related differences in cognitive control during visual search. Neurobiology of Aging, 53, 169–180.PubMedCrossRef *Mertes, C., Wascher, E., & Schneider, D. (2017). Compliance instead of flexibility? On age-related differences in cognitive control during visual search. Neurobiology of Aging, 53, 169–180.PubMedCrossRef
go back to reference Morris, S. B., & DeShon, R. P. (2002). Combining effect size estimates in meta-analysis with repeated measures and independent-groups designs. Psychological Methods, 7, 105–125.CrossRefPubMed Morris, S. B., & DeShon, R. P. (2002). Combining effect size estimates in meta-analysis with repeated measures and independent-groups designs. Psychological Methods, 7, 105–125.CrossRefPubMed
go back to reference Nothdurft, H.-C. (1993). The role of features in pre-attentive vision: Comparison of orientation, motion, and color cues. Vision Research, 33, 1937–1958.PubMedCrossRef Nothdurft, H.-C. (1993). The role of features in pre-attentive vision: Comparison of orientation, motion, and color cues. Vision Research, 33, 1937–1958.PubMedCrossRef
go back to reference Olivers, C. N., & Meeter, M. (2008). A boost and bounce theory of temporal attention. Psychological Review, 115, 836–863.PubMedCrossRef Olivers, C. N., & Meeter, M. (2008). A boost and bounce theory of temporal attention. Psychological Review, 115, 836–863.PubMedCrossRef
go back to reference Posner, M. I. (1980). Orienting of attention. Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology, 32A, 3–25.CrossRef Posner, M. I. (1980). Orienting of attention. Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology, 32A, 3–25.CrossRef
go back to reference *Prinzmetal, W., Taylor, J. A., Myers, L. B., & Nguyen-Espino, J. (2011). Contingent capture and inhibition of return: A comparison of mechanisms. Experimental Brain Research, 214, 47–60.PubMedCrossRef *Prinzmetal, W., Taylor, J. A., Myers, L. B., & Nguyen-Espino, J. (2011). Contingent capture and inhibition of return: A comparison of mechanisms. Experimental Brain Research, 214, 47–60.PubMedCrossRef
go back to reference Rauschenberger, R. (2003). Attentional capture by auto-and allo-cues. Psychonomic Bulletin & Review, 10, 814–842.CrossRef Rauschenberger, R. (2003). Attentional capture by auto-and allo-cues. Psychonomic Bulletin & Review, 10, 814–842.CrossRef
go back to reference Raymond, J. E., Shapiro, K. L., & Arnell, K. M. (1992). Temporary suppression of visual processing in an RSVP task: An attentional blink? Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 18, 849–860.PubMed Raymond, J. E., Shapiro, K. L., & Arnell, K. M. (1992). Temporary suppression of visual processing in an RSVP task: An attentional blink? Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 18, 849–860.PubMed
go back to reference Remington, R. W., Folk, C. L., & McLean, J. P. (2001). Contingent attentional capture or delayed allocation of attention? Attention, Perception, & Psychophysics, 63, 298–307.CrossRef Remington, R. W., Folk, C. L., & McLean, J. P. (2001). Contingent attentional capture or delayed allocation of attention? Attention, Perception, & Psychophysics, 63, 298–307.CrossRef
go back to reference *Roque, N. A., Wright, T. J., & Boot, W. R. (2016). Do different attention capture paradigms measure different types of capture? Attention, Perception, & Psychophysics, 78, 2014–2030.CrossRef *Roque, N. A., Wright, T. J., & Boot, W. R. (2016). Do different attention capture paradigms measure different types of capture? Attention, Perception, & Psychophysics, 78, 2014–2030.CrossRef
go back to reference Rosenthal, R. (1979). The “file drawer problem” and tolerance for null results. Psychological Bulletin, 86, 638–641.CrossRef Rosenthal, R. (1979). The “file drawer problem” and tolerance for null results. Psychological Bulletin, 86, 638–641.CrossRef
go back to reference RStudio Team. (2016). RStudio: Integrated development for R. Boston: Rstudio. RStudio Team. (2016). RStudio: Integrated development for R. Boston: Rstudio.
go back to reference Schoeberl, T., Ditye, T., & Ansorge, U. (2018). Same-location costs in peripheral cueing: The role of cue awareness and feature changes. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 44, 433–451.PubMed Schoeberl, T., Ditye, T., & Ansorge, U. (2018). Same-location costs in peripheral cueing: The role of cue awareness and feature changes. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 44, 433–451.PubMed
go back to reference Schoeberl, T., Fuchs, I., Theeuwes, J., & Ansorge, U. (2015). Stimulus-driven attentional capture by subliminal onset cues. Attention, Perception, & Psychophysics, 77, 737–748.CrossRef Schoeberl, T., Fuchs, I., Theeuwes, J., & Ansorge, U. (2015). Stimulus-driven attentional capture by subliminal onset cues. Attention, Perception, & Psychophysics, 77, 737–748.CrossRef
go back to reference *Schoenhammer, J. G., & Kerzel, D. (2017). Detection costs and contingent attentional capture. Attention, Perception, & Psychophysics, 79, 429–437.CrossRef *Schoenhammer, J. G., & Kerzel, D. (2017). Detection costs and contingent attentional capture. Attention, Perception, & Psychophysics, 79, 429–437.CrossRef
go back to reference Sterling, T. D. (1959). Publication decisions and their possible effects on inferences drawn from tests of significance—or vice versa. Journal of the American Statistical Association, 54, 30–34. Sterling, T. D. (1959). Publication decisions and their possible effects on inferences drawn from tests of significance—or vice versa. Journal of the American Statistical Association, 54, 30–34.
go back to reference Sterne, J. A., & Egger, M. (2005). Regression methods to detect publication and other bias in meta-analysis. In H. R. Rothstein, A. J. Sutton & M. Borenstein (Eds.), Publication bias in meta-analysis: Prevention, assessment and adjustments (pp. 99–110). Chichester: Wiley. Sterne, J. A., & Egger, M. (2005). Regression methods to detect publication and other bias in meta-analysis. In H. R. Rothstein, A. J. Sutton & M. Borenstein (Eds.), Publication bias in meta-analysis: Prevention, assessment and adjustments (pp. 99–110). Chichester: Wiley.
go back to reference Sterne, J. A., Sutton, A. J., Ioannidis, J. P., Terrin, N., Jones, D. R., Lau, J., & Tetzlaff, J. (2011). Recommendations for examining and interpreting funnel plot asymmetry in meta-analyses of randomised controlled trials. British Medical Journal, 343, d4002.PubMedCrossRef Sterne, J. A., Sutton, A. J., Ioannidis, J. P., Terrin, N., Jones, D. R., Lau, J., & Tetzlaff, J. (2011). Recommendations for examining and interpreting funnel plot asymmetry in meta-analyses of randomised controlled trials. British Medical Journal, 343, d4002.PubMedCrossRef
go back to reference Theeuwes, J. (1991). Exogenous and endogenous control of attention: The effect of visual onsets and offsets. Attention, Perception, & Psychophysics, 49, 83–90.CrossRef Theeuwes, J. (1991). Exogenous and endogenous control of attention: The effect of visual onsets and offsets. Attention, Perception, & Psychophysics, 49, 83–90.CrossRef
go back to reference Theeuwes, J. (1992). Perceptual selectivity for color and form. Attention, Perception, & Psychophysics, 51, 599–606.CrossRef Theeuwes, J. (1992). Perceptual selectivity for color and form. Attention, Perception, & Psychophysics, 51, 599–606.CrossRef
go back to reference Theeuwes, J. (2010). Top-down and bottom-up control of visual selection. Acta Psychologica, 135, 77–99.PubMedCrossRef Theeuwes, J. (2010). Top-down and bottom-up control of visual selection. Acta Psychologica, 135, 77–99.PubMedCrossRef
go back to reference Theeuwes, J. (2013). Feature-based attention: It is all bottom-up priming. Philosophical Transactions of the Royal Society of London B: Biological Sciences, 368, 20130055.PubMedCrossRefPubMedCentral Theeuwes, J. (2013). Feature-based attention: It is all bottom-up priming. Philosophical Transactions of the Royal Society of London B: Biological Sciences, 368, 20130055.PubMedCrossRefPubMedCentral
go back to reference Theeuwes, J., Atchley, P., & Kramer, A. F. (2000). On the time course of top-down and bottom-up control of visual attention. In S. Monsell & J. Driver (Eds.), Control of cognitive processes: Attention and performance XVIII (pp. 105–125). Cambridge: MIT Press. Theeuwes, J., Atchley, P., & Kramer, A. F. (2000). On the time course of top-down and bottom-up control of visual attention. In S. Monsell & J. Driver (Eds.), Control of cognitive processes: Attention and performance XVIII (pp. 105–125). Cambridge: MIT Press.
go back to reference Treisman, A. M., & Gelade, G. (1980). A feature-integration theory of attention. Cognitive Psychology, 12, 97–136.PubMedCrossRef Treisman, A. M., & Gelade, G. (1980). A feature-integration theory of attention. Cognitive Psychology, 12, 97–136.PubMedCrossRef
go back to reference Viechtbauer, W. (2010). Conducting meta-analyses in R with the metafor package. Journal of Statistical Software, 36, 1–48.CrossRef Viechtbauer, W. (2010). Conducting meta-analyses in R with the metafor package. Journal of Statistical Software, 36, 1–48.CrossRef
go back to reference Weichselbaum, H., & Ansorge, U. (2018). Bottom-up attention capture with distractor and target singletons defined in the same (color) dimension is not a matter of feature uncertainty. Attention, Perception, & Psychophysics, 80, 1350–1361.CrossRef Weichselbaum, H., & Ansorge, U. (2018). Bottom-up attention capture with distractor and target singletons defined in the same (color) dimension is not a matter of feature uncertainty. Attention, Perception, & Psychophysics, 80, 1350–1361.CrossRef
go back to reference Wolfe, J. M. (1994). Guided search 2.0 a revised model of visual search. Psychonomic Bulletin & Review, 1, 202–238.CrossRef Wolfe, J. M. (1994). Guided search 2.0 a revised model of visual search. Psychonomic Bulletin & Review, 1, 202–238.CrossRef
go back to reference Wolfe, J. M., Butcher, S. J., Lee, C., & Hyle, M. (2003). Changing your mind: On the contributions of top-down and bottom-up guidance in visual search for feature singletons. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 29, 483–502.PubMed Wolfe, J. M., Butcher, S. J., Lee, C., & Hyle, M. (2003). Changing your mind: On the contributions of top-down and bottom-up guidance in visual search for feature singletons. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 29, 483–502.PubMed
go back to reference *Worschech, F., & Ansorge, U. (2012). Top-down search for color prevents voluntary directing of attention to informative singleton cues. Experimental Psychology, 59, 153–162.PubMedCrossRef *Worschech, F., & Ansorge, U. (2012). Top-down search for color prevents voluntary directing of attention to informative singleton cues. Experimental Psychology, 59, 153–162.PubMedCrossRef
go back to reference Yantis, S., & Jonides, J. (1984). Abrupt visual onsets and selective attention: Evidence from visual search. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 10, 601–621.PubMed Yantis, S., & Jonides, J. (1984). Abrupt visual onsets and selective attention: Evidence from visual search. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 10, 601–621.PubMed
go back to reference *Yeh, S. L., & Liao, H. I. (2008). On the generality of the contingent orienting hypothesis. Acta Psychologica, 129, 157–165.PubMedCrossRef *Yeh, S. L., & Liao, H. I. (2008). On the generality of the contingent orienting hypothesis. Acta Psychologica, 129, 157–165.PubMedCrossRef
Metagegevens
Titel
A meta-analysis of contingent-capture effects
Auteurs
Christian Büsel
Martin Voracek
Ulrich Ansorge
Publicatiedatum
31-08-2018
Uitgeverij
Springer Berlin Heidelberg
Gepubliceerd in
Psychological Research / Uitgave 3/2020
Print ISSN: 0340-0727
Elektronisch ISSN: 1430-2772
DOI
https://doi.org/10.1007/s00426-018-1087-3

Andere artikelen Uitgave 3/2020

Psychological Research 3/2020 Naar de uitgave