Skip to main content
Top
Gepubliceerd in: Psychological Research 3/2020

07-08-2018 | Original Article

When flow is not enough: evidence from a lane changing task

Auteurs: Xin Xu, Guy Wallis

Gepubliceerd in: Psychological Research | Uitgave 3/2020

Log in om toegang te krijgen
share
DELEN

Deel dit onderdeel of sectie (kopieer de link)

  • Optie A:
    Klik op de rechtermuisknop op de link en selecteer de optie “linkadres kopiëren”
  • Optie B:
    Deel de link per e-mail

Abstract

Humans are able to estimate their heading on the basis of optic flow information and it has been argued that we use flow in this way to guide navigation. Consistent with this idea, several studies have reported good navigation performance in flow fields. However, one criticism of these studies is that they have generally focused on the task of walking or steering towards a target, offering an additional, salient directional cue. Hence, it remains a matter of debate as to whether humans are truly able to control steering in the presence of optic flow alone. In this study, we report a set of maneuvers carried out in flow fields in the absence of a physical target. To do this, we studied the everyday task of lane changing, a commonplace multiphase steering maneuver which can be conceptualized without the need for a target. What is more (and here is the crucial quirk), previous literature has found that in the absence of visual feedback, drivers show a systematic, asymmetric steering response, resulting in a systematic final heading error. If optic flow is sufficient for controlling navigation through our environment, we would expect this asymmetry to disappear whenever optic flow is provided. However, our results show that this asymmetry persisted, even in the presence of a flow field, implying that drivers are unable to use flow to guide normal steering responses in this task.
Bijlagen
Alleen toegankelijk voor geautoriseerde gebruikers
Literatuur
go back to reference Banks, M. S., Ehrlich, S. M., Backus, B. T., & Crowell, J. A. (1996). Estimating heading during real and simulated eye movements. Vision Research, 36(3), 431–443.PubMedCrossRef Banks, M. S., Ehrlich, S. M., Backus, B. T., & Crowell, J. A. (1996). Estimating heading during real and simulated eye movements. Vision Research, 36(3), 431–443.PubMedCrossRef
go back to reference Bruggeman, H., Zosh, W., & William, H., & Warren (2007). Optic flow drives human visuo-locomotor adaptation. Current Biology, 17(23), 2035–2040.PubMedCrossRef Bruggeman, H., Zosh, W., & William, H., & Warren (2007). Optic flow drives human visuo-locomotor adaptation. Current Biology, 17(23), 2035–2040.PubMedCrossRef
go back to reference Cloete, S., & Wallis, G. (2009). Limitations of feedforward control in multiple-phase steering movements. Experimental Brain Research, 195(3), 481–487.PubMedCrossRef Cloete, S., & Wallis, G. (2009). Limitations of feedforward control in multiple-phase steering movements. Experimental Brain Research, 195(3), 481–487.PubMedCrossRef
go back to reference Cloete, S., & Wallis, G. (2011). Visuomotor control of steering: The artefact of the matter. Exp Brain Res, 208(4), 475–489.PubMedCrossRef Cloete, S., & Wallis, G. (2011). Visuomotor control of steering: The artefact of the matter. Exp Brain Res, 208(4), 475–489.PubMedCrossRef
go back to reference Cutting, J. E. (1986). Perception with an eye for motion (pp. 151–168). Cambridge, Massachusetts: The MIT Press. Cutting, J. E. (1986). Perception with an eye for motion (pp. 151–168). Cambridge, Massachusetts: The MIT Press.
go back to reference Cutting, J. E., Vishton, P. M., Fluckiger, M., Baumberger, B., & Gerndt, J. D. (1997). Heading and path information from retinal flow in naturalistic environments. Perception & Psychophysics, 59(3), 426–441.CrossRef Cutting, J. E., Vishton, P. M., Fluckiger, M., Baumberger, B., & Gerndt, J. D. (1997). Heading and path information from retinal flow in naturalistic environments. Perception & Psychophysics, 59(3), 426–441.CrossRef
go back to reference Donges, E. (1978). A two-level model of driver steering behavior. Human Factors: The Journal of the Human Factors and Ergonomics Society, 20(6), 691–707.CrossRef Donges, E. (1978). A two-level model of driver steering behavior. Human Factors: The Journal of the Human Factors and Ergonomics Society, 20(6), 691–707.CrossRef
go back to reference Ehrlich, S. M., Beck, D. M., Crowell, J. A., Freeman, T. C. A., & Banks, M. S. (1998). Depth information and perceived self-motion during simulated gaze rotations. Vision Research, 38(20), 3129–3145.PubMedCrossRef Ehrlich, S. M., Beck, D. M., Crowell, J. A., Freeman, T. C. A., & Banks, M. S. (1998). Depth information and perceived self-motion during simulated gaze rotations. Vision Research, 38(20), 3129–3145.PubMedCrossRef
go back to reference Fajen, B., & Warren, W. (2007). Behavioral dynamics of intercepting a moving target. Experimental Brain Research, 180(2), 303–319.PubMedCrossRef Fajen, B., & Warren, W. (2007). Behavioral dynamics of intercepting a moving target. Experimental Brain Research, 180(2), 303–319.PubMedCrossRef
go back to reference Fajen, B., Warren, W., Temizer, S., & Kaelbling, L. (2003). A dynamical model of visually-guided steering, obstacle avoidance, and route selection. International Journal of Computer Vision, 54(1–3), 13–34.CrossRef Fajen, B., Warren, W., Temizer, S., & Kaelbling, L. (2003). A dynamical model of visually-guided steering, obstacle avoidance, and route selection. International Journal of Computer Vision, 54(1–3), 13–34.CrossRef
go back to reference Fajen, B. R. (2013). Guiding locomotion in complex, dynamic environments. Frontiers in Behavioral Neuroscience, 7, 15.CrossRef Fajen, B. R. (2013). Guiding locomotion in complex, dynamic environments. Frontiers in Behavioral Neuroscience, 7, 15.CrossRef
go back to reference Fajen, B. R., & Warren, W. H. (2003). Behavioral dynamics of steering, obstacle avoidance, and route selection. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 29(2), 343–362.PubMed Fajen, B. R., & Warren, W. H. (2003). Behavioral dynamics of steering, obstacle avoidance, and route selection. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 29(2), 343–362.PubMed
go back to reference Foulkes, A. J., Rushton, S. K., & Warren, P. A. (2013). Heading recovery from optic flow: Comparing performance of humans and computational models. Frontiers in Behavioral Neuroscience, 7, 53.PubMedPubMedCentral Foulkes, A. J., Rushton, S. K., & Warren, P. A. (2013). Heading recovery from optic flow: Comparing performance of humans and computational models. Frontiers in Behavioral Neuroscience, 7, 53.PubMedPubMedCentral
go back to reference Frissen, I., & Mars, F. (2014). The effect of visual degradation on anticipatory and compensatory steering control. Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology, 67(3), 499–507.CrossRef Frissen, I., & Mars, F. (2014). The effect of visual degradation on anticipatory and compensatory steering control. Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology, 67(3), 499–507.CrossRef
go back to reference Gibson, J. J. (1950). The perception of the visual world (pp. 119–129). Oxford: Houghton Mifflin. Gibson, J. J. (1950). The perception of the visual world (pp. 119–129). Oxford: Houghton Mifflin.
go back to reference Gibson, J. J. (1958). Visually controlled locomotion and visual orientation in animals. British Journal of Psychology, 49(3), 182–194.PubMedCrossRef Gibson, J. J. (1958). Visually controlled locomotion and visual orientation in animals. British Journal of Psychology, 49(3), 182–194.PubMedCrossRef
go back to reference Godthelp, J. (1985). Precognitive control: Open- and closed-loop steering in a lane-change manoeuvre. Ergonomics, 28(10), 1419–1438.PubMedCrossRef Godthelp, J. (1985). Precognitive control: Open- and closed-loop steering in a lane-change manoeuvre. Ergonomics, 28(10), 1419–1438.PubMedCrossRef
go back to reference Harris, M. G., & Carre, G. (2001). Is optic flow used to guide walking while wearing a displacing prism? Perception, 30(7), 811–818.PubMedCrossRef Harris, M. G., & Carre, G. (2001). Is optic flow used to guide walking while wearing a displacing prism? Perception, 30(7), 811–818.PubMedCrossRef
go back to reference Hildreth, E. C., Beusmans, J. M., Boer, E. R., & Royden, C. S. (2000). From vision to action: Experiments and models of steering control during driving. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 26(3), 1106–1132.PubMed Hildreth, E. C., Beusmans, J. M., Boer, E. R., & Royden, C. S. (2000). From vision to action: Experiments and models of steering control during driving. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 26(3), 1106–1132.PubMed
go back to reference Kawato, M. (1999). Internal models for motor control and trajectory planning. Current Opinion in Neurobiology, 9(6), 718–727.PubMedCrossRef Kawato, M. (1999). Internal models for motor control and trajectory planning. Current Opinion in Neurobiology, 9(6), 718–727.PubMedCrossRef
go back to reference Kountouriotis, G. K., Floyd, R. C., Gardner, P. H., Merat, N., & Wilkie, R. M. (2012). The role of gaze and road edge information during high-speed locomotion. Journal of Experimental Psychology-Human Perception and Performance, 38(3), 687–702.PubMedCrossRef Kountouriotis, G. K., Floyd, R. C., Gardner, P. H., Merat, N., & Wilkie, R. M. (2012). The role of gaze and road edge information during high-speed locomotion. Journal of Experimental Psychology-Human Perception and Performance, 38(3), 687–702.PubMedCrossRef
go back to reference Kountouriotis, G. K., Shire, K. A., Mole, C. D., Gardner, P. H., Merat, N., & Wilkie, R. M. (2013). Optic flow asymmetries bias high-speed steering along roads. Journal of Vision, 13(10), 9.CrossRef Kountouriotis, G. K., Shire, K. A., Mole, C. D., Gardner, P. H., Merat, N., & Wilkie, R. M. (2013). Optic flow asymmetries bias high-speed steering along roads. Journal of Vision, 13(10), 9.CrossRef
go back to reference Kountouriotis, G. K., & Wilkie, R. M. (2013). Displaying optic flow to simulate locomotion: Comparing heading and steering. Perception, 4(5), 333–346.CrossRef Kountouriotis, G. K., & Wilkie, R. M. (2013). Displaying optic flow to simulate locomotion: Comparing heading and steering. Perception, 4(5), 333–346.CrossRef
go back to reference Land, M., & Horwood, J. (1995). Which parts of the road guide steering? Nature, 377(6547), 339–340.PubMedCrossRef Land, M., & Horwood, J. (1995). Which parts of the road guide steering? Nature, 377(6547), 339–340.PubMedCrossRef
go back to reference Lappe, M., Bremmer, F., & Van den Berg, A. V. (1999). Perception of self-motion from visual flow. Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 3(9), 329–336.PubMedCrossRef Lappe, M., Bremmer, F., & Van den Berg, A. V. (1999). Perception of self-motion from visual flow. Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 3(9), 329–336.PubMedCrossRef
go back to reference Lappi, O. (2014). Future path and tangent point models in the visual control of locomotion in curve driving. Journal of Vision, 14(12), 22.CrossRef Lappi, O. (2014). Future path and tangent point models in the visual control of locomotion in curve driving. Journal of Vision, 14(12), 22.CrossRef
go back to reference Lee, D. N., & Lishman, R. (1977). Visual control of locomotion. Scandinavian Journal of Psychology, 18(3), 224–230.PubMedCrossRef Lee, D. N., & Lishman, R. (1977). Visual control of locomotion. Scandinavian Journal of Psychology, 18(3), 224–230.PubMedCrossRef
go back to reference Lehtonen, E., Lappi, O., Koirikivi, I., & Summala, H. (2014). Effect of driving experience on anticipatory look-ahead fixations in real curve driving. Accident Analysis & Prevention, 70(0), 195–208.CrossRef Lehtonen, E., Lappi, O., Koirikivi, I., & Summala, H. (2014). Effect of driving experience on anticipatory look-ahead fixations in real curve driving. Accident Analysis & Prevention, 70(0), 195–208.CrossRef
go back to reference Li, L., & Chen, J. (2010). Relative contributions of optic flow, bearing, and splay angle information to lane keeping. Journal of Vision, 10(11), 14.CrossRef Li, L., & Chen, J. (2010). Relative contributions of optic flow, bearing, and splay angle information to lane keeping. Journal of Vision, 10(11), 14.CrossRef
go back to reference Li, L., Chen, J., & Peng, X. Z. (2009). Influence of visual path information on human heading perception during rotation. Journal of Vision, 9(3), 14.CrossRef Li, L., Chen, J., & Peng, X. Z. (2009). Influence of visual path information on human heading perception during rotation. Journal of Vision, 9(3), 14.CrossRef
go back to reference Li, L., & Cheng, J. C. (2011a). Heading but not path or the tau-equalization strategy is used in the visual control of steering toward a goal. Journal of Vision, 11(12). Li, L., & Cheng, J. C. (2011a). Heading but not path or the tau-equalization strategy is used in the visual control of steering toward a goal. Journal of Vision, 11(12).
go back to reference Li, L., & Cheng, J. C. K. (2011b). Perceiving path from optic flow. Journal of Vision, 11(1), 15.CrossRef Li, L., & Cheng, J. C. K. (2011b). Perceiving path from optic flow. Journal of Vision, 11(1), 15.CrossRef
go back to reference Li, L., & Cheng, J. C. K. (2013). Visual strategies for the control of steering toward a goal. Displays, 34(2), 97–104.CrossRef Li, L., & Cheng, J. C. K. (2013). Visual strategies for the control of steering toward a goal. Displays, 34(2), 97–104.CrossRef
go back to reference Li, L., & Niehorster, D. C. (2014). Influence of optic flow on the control of heading and target egocentric direction during steering toward a goal. Journal of Neurophysiology, 112(4), 766–777.PubMedCrossRef Li, L., & Niehorster, D. C. (2014). Influence of optic flow on the control of heading and target egocentric direction during steering toward a goal. Journal of Neurophysiology, 112(4), 766–777.PubMedCrossRef
go back to reference Li, L., Stone, L. S., & Chen, J. (2011). Influence of optic-flow information beyond the velocity field on the active control of heading. Journal of Vision, 11(4). Li, L., Stone, L. S., & Chen, J. (2011). Influence of optic-flow information beyond the velocity field on the active control of heading. Journal of Vision, 11(4).
go back to reference Li, L., Sweet, B. T., & Stone, L. S. (2006). Humans can perceive heading without visual path information. Journal of Vision, 6(9), 874–881.PubMedCrossRef Li, L., Sweet, B. T., & Stone, L. S. (2006). Humans can perceive heading without visual path information. Journal of Vision, 6(9), 874–881.PubMedCrossRef
go back to reference Li, L., & Warren, W. H. (2000). Perception of heading during rotation: Sufficiency of dense motion parallax and reference objects. Vision Research, 40(28), 3873–3894.PubMedCrossRef Li, L., & Warren, W. H. (2000). Perception of heading during rotation: Sufficiency of dense motion parallax and reference objects. Vision Research, 40(28), 3873–3894.PubMedCrossRef
go back to reference Li, L., & Warren, W. H. (2002). Retinal flow is sufficient for steering during observer rotation. Psychological Science, 13(5), 485–491.PubMedCrossRef Li, L., & Warren, W. H. (2002). Retinal flow is sufficient for steering during observer rotation. Psychological Science, 13(5), 485–491.PubMedCrossRef
go back to reference Li, L., & Warren, W. H. Jr. (2004). Path perception during rotation: Influence of instructions, depth range, and dot density. Vision Res, 44(16), 1879–1889.PubMedCrossRef Li, L., & Warren, W. H. Jr. (2004). Path perception during rotation: Influence of instructions, depth range, and dot density. Vision Res, 44(16), 1879–1889.PubMedCrossRef
go back to reference Llewellyn, K. R. (1971). Visual guidance of locomotion. Journal of Experimental Psychology, 91(2), 245–245&.PubMedCrossRef Llewellyn, K. R. (1971). Visual guidance of locomotion. Journal of Experimental Psychology, 91(2), 245–245&.PubMedCrossRef
go back to reference Macuga, K. L., Beall, A. C., Kelly, J. W., Smith, R. S., & Loomis, J. M. (2007). Changing lanes: Inertial cues and explicit path information facilitate steering performance when visual feedback is removed. Experimental Brain Research, 178(2), 141–150.PubMedCrossRef Macuga, K. L., Beall, A. C., Kelly, J. W., Smith, R. S., & Loomis, J. M. (2007). Changing lanes: Inertial cues and explicit path information facilitate steering performance when visual feedback is removed. Experimental Brain Research, 178(2), 141–150.PubMedCrossRef
go back to reference Perrone, J. A. (1992). Model for the computation of self-motion in biological-systems. Journal of the Optical Society of America A-Optics Image Science and Vision, 9(2), 177–194.CrossRef Perrone, J. A. (1992). Model for the computation of self-motion in biological-systems. Journal of the Optical Society of America A-Optics Image Science and Vision, 9(2), 177–194.CrossRef
go back to reference Robertshaw, K. D., & Wilkie, R. M. (2008). Does gaze influence steering around a bend? Journal of Vision, 8(4). Robertshaw, K. D., & Wilkie, R. M. (2008). Does gaze influence steering around a bend? Journal of Vision, 8(4).
go back to reference Royden, C. S., Banks, M. S., & Crowell, J. A. (1992). The perception of heading during eye-movements. Nature, 360(6404), 583–587.PubMedCrossRef Royden, C. S., Banks, M. S., & Crowell, J. A. (1992). The perception of heading during eye-movements. Nature, 360(6404), 583–587.PubMedCrossRef
go back to reference Rushton, S. K. (2008). Perceptually guided action: A step in the right direction. Current Biology, 18(1), R36–R37.PubMedCrossRef Rushton, S. K. (2008). Perceptually guided action: A step in the right direction. Current Biology, 18(1), R36–R37.PubMedCrossRef
go back to reference Rushton, S. K., Harris, J. M., Lloyd, M. R., & Wann, J. P. (1998). Guidance of locomotion on foot uses perceived target location rather than optic flow. Current Biology, 8(21), 1191–1194.PubMedCrossRef Rushton, S. K., Harris, J. M., Lloyd, M. R., & Wann, J. P. (1998). Guidance of locomotion on foot uses perceived target location rather than optic flow. Current Biology, 8(21), 1191–1194.PubMedCrossRef
go back to reference Rushton, S. K., & Salvucci, D. D. (2001). An egocentric account of the visual guidance of locomotion. Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 5(1), 6–7.CrossRef Rushton, S. K., & Salvucci, D. D. (2001). An egocentric account of the visual guidance of locomotion. Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 5(1), 6–7.CrossRef
go back to reference Salvucci, D. D., & Gray, R. (2004). A two-point visual control model of steering. Perception, 33(10), 1233–1248.PubMedCrossRef Salvucci, D. D., & Gray, R. (2004). A two-point visual control model of steering. Perception, 33(10), 1233–1248.PubMedCrossRef
go back to reference Salvucci, D. D., & Liu, A. (2002). The time course of a lane change: Driver control and eye-movement behavior. Transportation Research Part F: Traffic Psychology and Behaviour, 5(2), 123–132.CrossRef Salvucci, D. D., & Liu, A. (2002). The time course of a lane change: Driver control and eye-movement behavior. Transportation Research Part F: Traffic Psychology and Behaviour, 5(2), 123–132.CrossRef
go back to reference Saunders, J. A. (2014). Reliability and relative weighting of visual and nonvisual information for perceiving direction of self-motion during walking. Journal of Vision, 14(3), 24–24.PubMedPubMedCentralCrossRef Saunders, J. A. (2014). Reliability and relative weighting of visual and nonvisual information for perceiving direction of self-motion during walking. Journal of Vision, 14(3), 24–24.PubMedPubMedCentralCrossRef
go back to reference Shadmehr, R., Smith, M. A., & Krakauer, J. W. (2010). Error correction, sensory prediction, and adaptation in motor control. Annual Review of Neuroscience, 33:89–108. (S. E. Hyman. Palo Alto, Annual Reviews).PubMedCrossRef Shadmehr, R., Smith, M. A., & Krakauer, J. W. (2010). Error correction, sensory prediction, and adaptation in motor control. Annual Review of Neuroscience, 33:89–108. (S. E. Hyman. Palo Alto, Annual Reviews).PubMedCrossRef
go back to reference Vandenberg, A. V. (1993). Perception of heading. Nature, 365(6446), 497–498.CrossRef Vandenberg, A. V. (1993). Perception of heading. Nature, 365(6446), 497–498.CrossRef
go back to reference vandenBerg, A. V. (1996). Judgements of heading. Vision Research, 36(15), 2337–2350.CrossRef vandenBerg, A. V. (1996). Judgements of heading. Vision Research, 36(15), 2337–2350.CrossRef
go back to reference Wallis, G., Chatziastros, A., & Bülthoff, H. (2002). An unexpected role for visual feedback in vehicle steering control. Current biology, 12(4), 295–299.PubMedCrossRef Wallis, G., Chatziastros, A., & Bülthoff, H. (2002). An unexpected role for visual feedback in vehicle steering control. Current biology, 12(4), 295–299.PubMedCrossRef
go back to reference Wallis, G., Chatziastros, A., Tresilian, J., & Tomasevic, N. (2007). The role of visual and nonvisual feedback in a vehicle steering task. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 33(5), 1127–1144.PubMed Wallis, G., Chatziastros, A., Tresilian, J., & Tomasevic, N. (2007). The role of visual and nonvisual feedback in a vehicle steering task. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 33(5), 1127–1144.PubMed
go back to reference Wann, J., & Land, M. (2000). Steering with or without the flow: Is the retrieval of heading necessary?. Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 4(8), 319–324.PubMedCrossRef Wann, J., & Land, M. (2000). Steering with or without the flow: Is the retrieval of heading necessary?. Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 4(8), 319–324.PubMedCrossRef
go back to reference Wann, J. P., Swapp, D., & Rushton, S. K. (2000). Heading perception and the allocation of attention. Vision Research, 40(18), 2533–2543.PubMedCrossRef Wann, J. P., Swapp, D., & Rushton, S. K. (2000). Heading perception and the allocation of attention. Vision Research, 40(18), 2533–2543.PubMedCrossRef
go back to reference Wann, J. P., & Swapp, D. K. (2000). Why you should look where you are going. Nature Neuroscience, 3(7), 647–648.PubMedCrossRef Wann, J. P., & Swapp, D. K. (2000). Why you should look where you are going. Nature Neuroscience, 3(7), 647–648.PubMedCrossRef
go back to reference Warren, W. H., & Hannon, D. J. (1988). Direction of self-motion is perceived from optical flow. Nature, 336(6195), 162–163.CrossRef Warren, W. H., & Hannon, D. J. (1988). Direction of self-motion is perceived from optical flow. Nature, 336(6195), 162–163.CrossRef
go back to reference Warren, W. H., & Hannon, D. J. (1990). Eye-movements and optical-flow. Journal of the Optical Society of America A-Optics Image Science and Vision, 7(1), 160–169.CrossRef Warren, W. H., & Hannon, D. J. (1990). Eye-movements and optical-flow. Journal of the Optical Society of America A-Optics Image Science and Vision, 7(1), 160–169.CrossRef
go back to reference Warren, W. H., Kay, B. A., Zosh, W. D., Duchon, A. P., & Sahuc, S. (2001). Optic flow is used to control human walking. Nature Neuroscience, 4(2), 213–216.PubMedCrossRef Warren, W. H., Kay, B. A., Zosh, W. D., Duchon, A. P., & Sahuc, S. (2001). Optic flow is used to control human walking. Nature Neuroscience, 4(2), 213–216.PubMedCrossRef
go back to reference Warren, W. H., Morris, M. W., & Kalish, M. (1988). Perception of translational heading from optical-flow. Journal of Experimental Psychology-Human Perception and Performance, 14(4), 646–660.PubMedCrossRef Warren, W. H., Morris, M. W., & Kalish, M. (1988). Perception of translational heading from optical-flow. Journal of Experimental Psychology-Human Perception and Performance, 14(4), 646–660.PubMedCrossRef
go back to reference Wilkie, R., & Wann, J. (2003). Controlling steering and judging heading: Retinal flow, visual direction, and extraretinal information. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 29(2), 363–378.PubMed Wilkie, R., & Wann, J. (2003). Controlling steering and judging heading: Retinal flow, visual direction, and extraretinal information. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 29(2), 363–378.PubMed
go back to reference Wilkie, R. M., Kountouriotis, G. K., Merat, N., & Wann, J. P. (2010). Using vision to control locomotion: Looking where you want to go. Experimental Brain Research, 204(4), 539–547.PubMedCrossRef Wilkie, R. M., Kountouriotis, G. K., Merat, N., & Wann, J. P. (2010). Using vision to control locomotion: Looking where you want to go. Experimental Brain Research, 204(4), 539–547.PubMedCrossRef
go back to reference Wilkie, R. M., & Wann, J. P. (2002). Driving as night falls: The contribution of retinal flow and visual direction to the control of steering. Current Biology, 12(23), 2014–2017.PubMedCrossRef Wilkie, R. M., & Wann, J. P. (2002). Driving as night falls: The contribution of retinal flow and visual direction to the control of steering. Current Biology, 12(23), 2014–2017.PubMedCrossRef
go back to reference Wilkie, R. M., & Wann, J. P. (2005). The role of visual and nonvisual information in the control of locomotion. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 31(5), 901–911.PubMed Wilkie, R. M., & Wann, J. P. (2005). The role of visual and nonvisual information in the control of locomotion. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 31(5), 901–911.PubMed
go back to reference Wilkie, R. M., & Wann, J. P. (2006). Judgments of path, not heading, guide locomotion. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 32(1), 88–96.PubMed Wilkie, R. M., & Wann, J. P. (2006). Judgments of path, not heading, guide locomotion. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 32(1), 88–96.PubMed
go back to reference Wilkie, R. M., Wann, J. P., & Allison, R. S. (2008). Active gaze, visual look-ahead, and locomotor control. Journal of Experimental Psychology-Human Perception and Performance, 34(5), 1150–1164.PubMedCrossRef Wilkie, R. M., Wann, J. P., & Allison, R. S. (2008). Active gaze, visual look-ahead, and locomotor control. Journal of Experimental Psychology-Human Perception and Performance, 34(5), 1150–1164.PubMedCrossRef
go back to reference Wood, R. M., Harvey, M. A., Young, C. E., Beedie, A., & Wilson, T. (2000). Weighting to go with the flow? Current Biology, 10(15), R545–R546.PubMedCrossRef Wood, R. M., Harvey, M. A., Young, C. E., Beedie, A., & Wilson, T. (2000). Weighting to go with the flow? Current Biology, 10(15), R545–R546.PubMedCrossRef
go back to reference Xu, X., Wallis, G., & Cloete, S. (2014). Naïve physics in vehicle steering control. HCI International 2014—Posters’ Extended Abstracts. C. Stephanidis. Springer International Publishing, 434, 384–389. Xu, X., Wallis, G., & Cloete, S. (2014). Naïve physics in vehicle steering control. HCI International 2014—Posters’ Extended Abstracts. C. Stephanidis. Springer International Publishing, 434, 384–389.
go back to reference Zhao, H., & Warren, W. H. (2015). On-line and model-based approaches to the visual control of action. Vision Research, 110(Part B): 190–202.PubMedCrossRef Zhao, H., & Warren, W. H. (2015). On-line and model-based approaches to the visual control of action. Vision Research, 110(Part B): 190–202.PubMedCrossRef
Metagegevens
Titel
When flow is not enough: evidence from a lane changing task
Auteurs
Xin Xu
Guy Wallis
Publicatiedatum
07-08-2018
Uitgeverij
Springer Berlin Heidelberg
Gepubliceerd in
Psychological Research / Uitgave 3/2020
Print ISSN: 0340-0727
Elektronisch ISSN: 1430-2772
DOI
https://doi.org/10.1007/s00426-018-1070-z

Andere artikelen Uitgave 3/2020

Psychological Research 3/2020 Naar de uitgave