Skip to main content
Top
Gepubliceerd in: TSG - Tijdschrift voor gezondheidswetenschappen 4/2022

Open Access 02-11-2022 | Wetenschappelijk artikel

Regional differences in health further explained

The contribution of lifestyle, loneliness and mastery

Auteurs: Rachelle Meisters, Polina Putrik, Daan Westra, Hans Bosma, Dirk Ruwaard, Maria Jansen

Gepubliceerd in: TSG - Tijdschrift voor gezondheidswetenschappen | Uitgave 4/2022

share
DELEN

Deel dit onderdeel of sectie (kopieer de link)

  • Optie A:
    Klik op de rechtermuisknop op de link en selecteer de optie “linkadres kopiëren”
  • Optie B:
    Deel de link per e-mail
insite
ZOEKEN

Abstract

Like in most Western countries, regional health inequalities are also present in the Netherlands. Explaining these inequalities is necessary for policymakers to target interventions to reduce them. Regional health inequalities are usually attributed to demographic and socio-economic factors, while lifestyle and psychosocial factors are increasingly shown to impact individuals’ health. Therefore, this study analyses the role of lifestyle, loneliness, and self-mastery in explaining regional inequalities, in addition to demographic factors and SES, for self-rated health, presence of chronic diseases, and psychological distress. Analyses are performed in the linked dataset from the Dutch Public Health Services, Statistics Netherlands, and the National Institute for Public Health and the Environment for the year 2016 (n = 334,721). The results show that lifestyle, loneliness and self-mastery contribute to the regional health inequalities in self-rated health and presence of chronic diseases. For psychological distress, both loneliness and self-mastery contribute to the regional health inequalities. Addressing lifestyle and psychosocial factors can offer policymakers additional pathways to bridge regional health inequalities. In this study, the region of Zuid-Limburg represents the reference region. Use compare regions for health and healthcare costs (Regiovergelijker gezondheid en zorgkosten [1]) in order to select all other Dutch regions as reference region.
Extras
Opmerkingen

Supplementary Information

The online version of this article (https://​doi.​org/​10.​1007/​s12508-022-00368-5) contains supplementary material, which is available to authorized users.
Original article: Meisters, R., Putrik, P., Westra, D., Bosma, H., Ruwaard, D., Jansen, M. Regionale verschillen in gezondheid nader verklaard. De bijdrage van leefstijl, eenzaamheid en zelfregie. TSG Tijdschrift Gezondheidswet. 2022;100:4–12. https://​doi.​org/​10.​1007/​s12508-021-00323-w
The Dutch version of this article can be found under https://​doi.​org/​10.​1007/​s12508-021-00323-w.

Introduction

Large socioeconomic health differences are prevalent in the Netherlands, and these differences have barely changed or have even increased in recent decades [2]. There are also regional differences. For example, 78.5% of all adults in the Dutch Public Health Services region of Central Holland (Hollands Midden) report a (very) good self-perceived health, compared with 69.5% of those living in South Limburg (Zuid-Limburg) [3]. The underlying determinants of health inequalities are very complex. They are often related to socioeconomic status (SES) [4], accumulate over different stages of life, and often lie outside the domain of the health sector [4]. Factors in the areas of housing, well-being, working and living environment, and education are related to health [5]. Moreover, the prevalence of the determinants of health differs greatly per region.
When explaining health differences between regions, the population is usually adjusted for differences in age, gender, income, education level, and migration background. According to studies based on the Health Monitor 2016, 11% of the variation in perceived health can be explained by these factors [6]. The authors suggested that in follow-up research, factors based on well-being and lifestyle should also be included to explain differences in perceived health. There is a large body of evidence for the negative health effects of lifestyle habits, such as smoking, alcohol consumption, and (inadequate) physical activity [7]. Lifestyle determinants alone, however, are not sufficient to explain health differences [4, 8].
In recent years, well-being and psychosocial factors related to health have gained increasing attention. For example, loneliness (the physical experience of a lack of connection with other people [9]) has been directly associated with increased mortality [10], morbidity, poorer mental health [11], and unhealthy habits such as smoking [11, 12], higher body mass index (BMI) [12], and less physical activity [12]. Moreover, in our current participatory society, citizens need better mastery skills to direct their own lives, even when they rely on support or care from others [13]. Although this is important for good (self-rated) health, it is not realistic for everyone [14]. Degrees of loneliness and mastery skills can vary per region and can thus contribute to regional health inequalities. All these factors together complicate efforts to diminish health inequalities. These inequalities represent a complex social issue, which requires a broader view [4]. In addition to demographic factors and SES, more research is needed to determine the contribution of lifestyle, loneliness, and mastery to explaining regional health inequalities.
The aim of this study was to further explain regional health inequalities in self-rated health, chronic disease, and psychological distress by using a more extensive set of lifestyle factors, loneliness, and mastery, in addition to demographic factors and SES. With the results of this research, we aimed to provide more insight into the factors associated with regional health inequalities in order to provide policymakers with more leads to help diminish health inequalities.

Methods

Data and sampling

This was a cross-sectional study based on data from the Dutch Health Survey (2016) and registry data from Statistics Netherlands. In accordance with the Dutch Public Health Law, Dutch municipalities are obliged to assess local public health issues. For this purpose, the Health Survey is held once every four years. This survey is a collaboration between the Dutch Public Health Services, the Dutch National Institute for Public Health and the Environment (RIVM), and Statistics Netherlands. The survey covers various topics regarding the respondent’s personal situation, such as lifestyle, mental and physical health, loneliness, household, and mastery. As a result of the sampling method used (complex sample method), weighting factors were calculated based on a number of individual and regional background characteristics to ensure the sample was representative of the entire Dutch population [15]. The sample (N= 457,150; response rate: ~ 40% [15]) included the non-institutionalized population aged 19 years and over.
The registry data provided by Statistics Netherlands were based on the Dutch Personal Records Database and data from the Dutch Tax and Customs Administration for the entire Dutch population. All data were linked in a secured environment, which is managed by Statistics Netherlands, and processed anonymously. After data linkage and exclusion of missing data, the sample included 334,721 respondents.

Dependent variables

For this study, three different dependent variables from the Health Survey were used. The variable “self-rated health” was assessed with the question “How would you rate your health in general?”, which is answered on a five-point Likert scale. The response categories were dichotomized into good self-rated health (“good” or “very good”) and poor self-rated health (“fair”, “poor”, or “very poor”). The variable “chronic disease” was derived from the question “Do you have one or more long-term diseases (expected duration six months or longer)?”, with the answer options “yes” and “no”. This question does not differentiate between physical and mental diseases.
Psychological distress was derived from the Kessler-10 questionnaire (K10) [16]. The ten questions in this questionnaire are answered on a five-point Likert scale, resulting in a total score ranging from 10 to 50. Scores of 30 and higher were considered as “high risk” (see Appendix Tables A1 and A2 for details).

Independent variables

The independent variables in this study included region, demographic factors, SES, lifestyle, loneliness, and mastery. The variable “region” was based on the 2016 division of the Dutch Public Health Services (resulting in 25 regions). The region of Zuid-Limburg was the reference group as this region scores worst on many health-related factors and outcome measures based on the information published by the RIVM [3]. Zuid-Limburg has the highest percentages of adults with a chronic disease (38.7% vs 33.9% nationwide), adults with (very) poor or fair self-rated health (30.5% vs 24.4%), lonely adults (47.8% vs 43.0%), and adults with insufficient mastery (12.5% vs 9.9%) [3].
Demographic factors included age, gender, migration background, and marital status. SES consisted of the highest attained level of education, the quartile of standardized household income, and income inadequacy. Household income was standardized for the number of persons in the household and then classified into quartiles based on income data of the entire Dutch population. The variable “income inadequacy” was examined with the question “In the past 12 months, have you had any concerns making ends meet with your household income?” Income inadequacy is considered to be a subjective measure of income as part of SES since it is also related to health, in addition to the objective income [17]. Lifestyle variables included BMI category [18] and sufficient physical activity (at least 2.5 hours of moderately intensive exercise or intense training twice a week, as defined by the Health Council [19]) as a proxy for eating and exercise behavior, smoking history, and alcohol consumption (“never”, “moderately”, or “excessively” (i.e., > 14 alcohol beverages a week for women and > 21 alcohol beverages a week for men)).
The variable “loneliness” was based on the total score on the eleven statements of the De Jong–Gierveld scale [9]. These statements refer to feelings of severe loneliness, problematic situations, companionship, and sociability (e.g., “I experience a general sense of emptiness” or “There are many people I can trust completely”). The answer options are “yes”, “more or less” and “no”. An individual is considered to be somewhat lonely if their score is in the range of 3 to 8, severely lonely with a score of 9 or 10, and very severely lonely with a score of 11 [20].
The variable “mastery” was assessed with the seven statements of the Pearlin and Schooler Mastery Scale [21]. These statements (e.g., “I have little control over the things that happen to me”) are answered on a five-point Likert scale (ranging from “strongly agree” to “strongly disagree”), with a total score of 7–35. The higher the total score, the more control one experiences over their life.

Statistical analyses

The relative risks for the negative health outcomes were modeled using a series of logistic and Poisson regressions. The outcomes “poor self-rated health” and “at least one chronic disease” were modelled with robust Poisson regressions because of the frequency of these outcomes (prevalence > 10%). Odds ratios (ORs) of logistic regressions can substantially overestimate the prevalence ratios (PRs) for this type of outcome variables [22]. For the outcome “psychological distress”, logistic regressions were used. For each outcome variable, six regression models were compared. Model 1 was only adjusted for region. This is a categorical variable with 24 dummies and the region of Zuid-Limburg as reference group. In Model 2, region, demographic factors, and SES were accounted for. In Model 3, the outcomes were corrected for region, demographic factors, SES, and a) lifestyle, b) loneliness, or c) mastery, respectively. Finally, in Model 4, all independent variables of this study were accounted for.
Given the number of missing data, all analyses were also performed with multiple imputed data (multivariate imputation by chained equations, 5 imputations, n= 452,664) [23]. These results and the original findings were comparable, making the findings robust. All analyses were performed using Stata 16 [24] and applied to the sampling design of the data.

Results

The sample included 334,721 persons, of whom more than half were women (52.4%), with a mean age of 59.2 years (standard deviation (SD): 16.9) (Tab. 1). The majority of the respondents was Dutch-born (88.0%), 8.4% had a Western migration background, and 3.6% had a non-Western migration background. In addition, 67.0% of the respondents were married or living together, 10.7% had never been married, 6.9% was widowed, and 9.4% was divorced. Furthermore, 74.0% of the respondents experienced a (very) good health, 60.7% had no chronic diseases, and 95.5% had no, a low, or a moderate risk of psychological distress. The mean loneliness score was 2.8 (SD: 3.1) (scores 0–3 indicated “not lonely”). For mastery, the mean score was 26.7 (SD: 5.2) (scale 7–35). The outcomes and factors were also weighted and are described per region (see Appendix Tables A4 and A5). An overview of the missing data can be found in Appendix Table A3.
Table 1
Sample characteristics (n = 334,721)
variable
category
n
%
gendera
Male
159,251
47.6%
migration backgrounda
Dutch-born
294,573
88.0%
Western migration background
 28,204
 8.4%
marital statusb
Married/living together
224,234
67.0%
Never married
 35,899
10.7%
Widowed
 23,052
 6.9%
highest attained level of educationb
Primary school
 19,061
 5.7%
Lower vocational education
102,886
30.7%
Middle vocational/secondary
106,341
31.8%
standardized household income quartilea
0–25%
 42,250
12.6%
26–50%
 84,243
25.2%
51–75%
 98,107
29.3%
self-perceived income inadequacyb
Inadequate, major concerns
  9,490
 2.8%
Inadequate, some concerns
 34,273
10.2%
Adequate, minor concerns
115,299
34.4%
physical activityb
Insufficient
 94,343
28.2%
body mass index, kg/m2b
Normal (18.5–25)
152,321
45.5%
Overweight (25–30)
128,977
38.5%
Obese (> 30)
 49,223
14.7%
alcohol consumptionb
Never
 32,663
 9.8%
Moderate
275,392
82.3%
smoking historyb
Never smoked
135,642
40.5%
Former smoker
144,994
43.3%
chronic diseaseb
None
203,330
60.7%
self-rated healthb
Very (good)
247,707
74.0%
psychological distressb
No, low or moderate risk
319,533
95.5%
 
Average
sd
age, yearsa
     59.2
16.9
loneliness scoreb
      2.8
 3.1
mastery scoreb
     26.7
 5.2
Based on unweighted, non-imputed data
SD standard deviation
aRegistry data
bSelf-reported data
Compared with residents of Zuid-Limburg, all residents of other Dutch regions had a lower risk of poor self-rated health in the uncorrected model, with PRs ranging from 0.72 to 0.93 (Tab. 2). In other words, if differences between regions remained uncorrected for any population characteristic, residents of Zuid-Limburg had a higher chance of poor self-rated health (8–39%) than residents of other regions. After adding demographic and SES factors, 23 regions remained significantly different. After adding lifestyle factors, 19 regions remained significantly different, and after adding loneliness, 21 regions remained significantly different. After addition of mastery, 20 regions remained significantly different from Zuid-Limburg (Fig. 1 and Appendix Table A6). When using the most extensive model, the regional differences in self-rated health remained significant in 17 of the 24 regions, with PRs ranging from 0.83 to 0.95 (Tab. 2).
Table 2
Regional differences in uncorrected and fully corrected models
 
self-rated health (very) poor or fair
PR (95% CI)
at least one chronic disease
PR (95% CI)
high risk for psychological distress
OR (95% CI)
region
Model 1: region
Model 4: totala
Model 1: region
Model 4: totala
Model 1: region
Model 4: totala
Zuid-Limburg
1.00 (ref)
1.00 (ref)
1.00 (ref)
1.00 (ref)
1.00 (ref)
1.00 (ref)
Zuid-Holland-Zuid
0.75
(0.70–0.80)
0.83
(0.79–0.88)
0.83
(0.78–0.87)
0.88
(0.84–0.92)
0.66
(0.54–0.79)
0.76
(0.60–0.96)
Zeeland
0.91
(0.85–0.96)
1.00
(0.95–1.05)
0.87
(0.83–0.91)
0.89
(0.85–0.93)
0.70
(0.58–0.84)
0.80
(0.64–0.99)
Zaanstreek-Waterland
0.82
(0.78–0.86)
0.94
(0.90–0.98)
0.93
(0.90–0.97)
1.01
(0.97–1.05)
0.82
(0.72–0.94)
1.09
(0.92–1.30)
West-Brabant
0.88
(0.84–0.93)
1.00
(0.95–1.05)
0.81
(0.77–0.84)
0.85
(0.82–0.89)
0.75
(0.65–0.88)
0.96
(0.79–1.16)
Utrecht
0.73
(0.69–0.76)
0.94
(0.90–0.98)
0.82
(0.79–0.85)
0.95
(0.93–0.98)
0.69
(0.61–0.77)
0.94
(0.81–1.09)
Twente
0.75
(0.71–0.80)
0.88
(0.83–0.93)
0.89
(0.85–0.94)
0.98
(0.94–1.02)
0.96
(0.81–1.14)
1.49
(1.20–1.84)
Rotterdam
0.93
(0.89–0.97)
0.93
(0.89–0.96)
0.94
(0.91–0.97)
0.98
(0.95–1.01)
1.07
(0.96–1.19)
0.99
(0.86–1.15)
Noord- en Oost-Gelderland
0.73
(0.69–0.77)
0.89
(0.85–0.93)
0.95
(0.92–0.99)
1.03
(0.99–1.07)
0.71
(0.62–0.81)
1.05
(0.89–1.25)
Limburg-Noord
0.86
(0.82–0.90)
0.97
(0.93–1.01)
0.93
(0.89–0.96)
0.97
(0.94–1.01)
0.67
(0.59–0.76)
0.84
(0.71–0.99)
Kennemerland
0.72
(0.68–0.76)
0.90
(0.85–0.94)
0.84
(0.80–0.88)
0.94
(0.90–0.97)
0.68
(0.58–0.79)
1.01
(0.84–1.22)
IJsselland
0.72
(0.67–0.77)
0.91
(0.85–0.97)
0.91
(0.87–0.96)
1.03
(0.98–1.08)
0.70
(0.58–0.84)
0.99
(0.79–1.25)
Hollands Noorden
0.78
(0.74–0.82)
0.95
(0.91–0.99)
0.87
(0.83–0.91)
0.96
(0.92–0.99)
0.71
(0.61–0.82)
0.95
(0.79–1.14)
Hollands Midden
0.72
(0.68–0.75)
0.86
(0.82–0.90)
0.93
(0.90–0.97)
1.02
(0.98–1.05)
0.82
(0.72–0.92)
1.11
(0.94–1.30)
Hart voor Brabant
0.84
(0.80–0.88)
1.00
(0.96–1.05)
0.84
(0.80–0.87)
0.91
(0.88–0.94)
0.76
(0.67–0.86)
1.05
(0.89–1.24)
Haaglanden
0.88
(0.84–0.93)
0.92
(0.88–0.96)
0.97
(0.93–1.01)
1.04
(1.00–1.08)
1.27
(1.12–1.44)
1.22
(1.03–1.44)
Groningen
0.79
(0.74–0.83)
0.96
(0.92–1.01)
0.95
(0.91–0.99)
1.06
(1.02–1.11)
0.66
(0.57–0.78)
0.84
(0.69–1.03)
Gooi en Vechtstreek
0.76
(0.70–0.81)
0.94
(0.88–0.99)
0.85
(0.81–0.90)
0.93
(0.89–0.98)
0.71
(0.58–0.87)
1.01
(0.78–1.29)
Gelderland-Zuid
0.75
(0.71–0.79)
0.90
(0.86–0.95)
0.86
(0.82–0.90)
0.95
(0.91–0.99)
0.86
(0.75–0.98)
1.19
(0.99–1.42)
Gelderland-Midden
0.76
(0.71–0.81)
0.89
(0.84–0.94)
0.93
(0.88–0.98)
1.01
(0.96–1.06)
0.95
(0.78–1.14)
1.19
(0.97–1.46)
Friesland
0.73
(0.69–0.77)
0.86
(0.82–0.91)
0.89
(0.86–0.93)
0.95
(0.92–0.99)
0.66
(0.57–0.77)
0.95
(0.79–1.14)
Flevoland
0.81
(0.71–0.92)
0.96
(0.87–1.07)
0.93
(0.85–1.02)
1.05
(0.96–1.14)
1.01
(0.74–1.37)
1.27
(0.90–1.79)
Drenthe
0.76
(0.70–0.82)
0.88
(0.82–0.94)
0.90
(0.84–0.95)
0.95
(0.90–1.01)
0.65
(0.52–0.80)
0.84
(0.63–1.13)
Brabant-Zuidoost
0.88
(0.84–0.92)
1.04
(0.99–1.08)
0.83
(0.80–0.87)
0.90
(0.86–0.93)
0.83
(0.73–0.94)
1.04
(0.87–1.24)
Amsterdam
0.79
(0.74–0.84)
0.88
(0.84–0.93)
0.83
(0.79–0.87)
0.93
(0.89–0.98)
0.96
(0.84–1.11)
1.03
(0.86–1.23)
Results of robust Poisson and logistic regressions (n= 334,721) based on weighted data. PRs and ORs in italics are significant (P < 0.05)
PR prevalence ratio, OR odds ratio, CI confidence interval
aModel 4 was corrected for region, demographic factors, SES, lifestyle, loneliness, and mastery
The risk of having at least one chronic disease differed significantly between 22 regions and Zuid-Limburg in the uncorrected model; the PRs ranged from 0.81 to 0.95 (Tab. 2). The regional differences for chronic disease could be partly explained by demographic factors and SES (17 significantly different regions), lifestyle (15 significantly different regions), loneliness (15 significantly different regions), and mastery (14 significantly different regions) (Fig. 2 and Appendix Table A7). In the most extensive model, residents of Zuid-Limburg had a higher risk (4–18%) of having at least one chronic disease compared with the residents of 12 other regions (significant PRs ranged from 0.85 to 0.96). Residents of the region of Groningen had a higher risk of having at least one chronic disease compared with Zuid-Limburg residents in the most extensive model (PR: 1.06; 95% confidence interval (CI): 1.02–1.11).
The risk of psychological distress differed significantly between the 18 other regions and Zuid-Limburg in the uncorrected model; the ORs ranged from 0.65 to 0.86. The regional differences for the risk of psychological distress could be largely explained by demographic factors, SES, loneliness, and mastery (Fig. 3 and Appendix Table A8). Of the 19 significantly different regions in Model 1, 12 remained significantly different when demographic factors and SES were added, 10 remained significantly different with the addition of lifestyle, 6 remained significantly different with the addition of loneliness, and 5 remained significantly different with the addition of mastery. In the most comprehensive model, residents of three other regions had a lower risk of psychological distress compared with residents of Zuid-Limburg. These three regions were South Holland–South (Zuid-Holland-Zuid) (OR: 0.76; 95% CI: 0.60–0.96), Zeeland (OR: 0.80; 95% CI: 0.64–0.99), and Northern Limburg (Limburg-Noord) (OR: 0.84; 95% CI: 0.71–0.99). The residents of Twente (OR: 1.49; 95% CI: 1.20–1.84) and Haaglanden (OR: 1.22; 95% CI: 1.03–1.44) had a higher risk of psychological distress compared with those living in Zuid-Limburg. After adding demographic factors, SES, lifestyle, loneliness, and mastery, there were no significant differences between the other 19 regions and Zuid-Limburg. The results of the multiple imputed data set were comparable with the results based on the sample of 334,721 respondents.

Discussion

The aim of this study was to explain the regional health differences in self-rated health, chronic disease, and psychological distress based on an extensive set of lifestyle factors, loneliness, and mastery, in addition to demographic factors and SES. When we corrected for all explanatory factors, the number of regions that differed from the region of Zuid-Limburg with respect to self-rated health decreased from 24 to 17. Lifestyle, loneliness, and mastery partly contributed to this difference, with 19, 21, and 20 significant regions, respectively (out of 23 after adjusting for demographic factors and SES). With regard to chronic disease, residents of 12 regions were less likely to have a chronic disease than residents of Zuid-Limburg after accounting for all explanatory factors. Lifestyle and loneliness (number of significantly different regions reduced from 17 to 15 for both) and mastery (from 17 to 14 significant regions) contributed to the differences between regions in self-perceived health and presence of chronic diseases. Of the 19 regions with significant differences in the risk of psychological distress, 14 were no longer significantly different when correcting for the explanatory factors. This could be largely explained by loneliness (from 12 to 6 significant regions) and mastery (from 12 to 5 significant regions), in addition to the corrections for demographic characteristics and SES (from 19 to 12 significant regions) and lifestyle (from 12 to 10 significant regions). This study presented regional differences compared with the reference region Zuid-Limburg. In the tool Compare Regions for Health and Healthcare Costs (Regiovergelijker gezondheid en zorgkosten [1]), users can select any reference region and display the results of the uncorrected model (Model 1) and the fully corrected model (Model 6) [1].
The correction for demographic factors and SES partly contributed to the explanation of regional differences in self-rated health. This finding is similar to the results of previous research in the Netherlands, in which municipal differences in self-rated health were partly explained by age, migration background, income, and education level [6]. Similarly, international studies have shown that regional health inequalities can be partially explained by personal characteristics. In the United States, 30% of mortality differences for women are explained by individual characteristics (demographic factors and SES), whereas 53% is attributed to contextual features per state (social cohesion, economic, and socio-political structure) [25]. In England, the north-south divide in cardiovascular diseases can also be partly explained by demographic factors and SES, in this case by smoking behavior, BMI, and blood pressure [26].
After adding all explanatory factors to the model, there were still some regional health inequalities. Which factors can further explain these differences? It is well known that neighborhood characteristics such as social cohesion, facilities, perceived safety, and less nuisance are associated with better health [27, 28]. Statistics Netherlands has published national and regional data on this topic in its Safety Monitor. These data showed that residents of the regions of Amsterdam, Rotterdam, parts of The Hague region, Utrecht city, and Zuid-Limburg feel less safe, experience more nuisance, and are less satisfied with the quality of life in neighborhoods [29]. In addition, the Dutch Opportunity Map (KansenKaart), which makes regional differences in upward mobility (i.e., the ability to improve one’s social status relative to that of another social group) insightful, has shown that upward mobility in income is less common in people who grew up in the northern provinces of the Netherlands [30]. Upward mobility in education level (higher vocational training or university), on the other hand, is less common in people who grew up in the Dutch Bible Belt and the northern regions. These regional differences could be further investigated, in combination with lifestyle and psychosocial factors.
When explaining poorer health outcomes, cultural and historical aspects are often taken into consideration. For Zuid-Limburg, for example, its mining history is often considered [31]. When the mines closed, not only did many jobs disappear, but the social structures of mine, church, and state also collapsed. These structures provided education, healthcare, social cohesion, and housing in Zuid-Limburg. A possible consequence is that residents of Zuid-Limburg have a more dependent attitude and therefore experience a lower sense of mastery [31]. The disappearance of these structures, and the absence of adequate alternatives [32], has also led to a greater feeling of loneliness and less mastery. Nevertheless, even when we corrected for mastery and loneliness, regional differences remained (5–20% higher chance of poorer health and 4–18% higher chance of having a chronic disease). This may indicate the presence of other determinants we did not include or may imply that the used scales insufficiently measure loneliness and mastery. Although these are validated scales, feelings of loneliness and lower sense of mastery may not have been fully covered by the questions, as people feel politicians and government have abandoned them [32]. Perhaps future (qualitative and) longitudinal research can unravel the mechanisms behind these factors.
Our results offer leads for policymakers to reduce regional health inequalities by tackling unhealthier lifestyles and loneliness and strengthening mastery in the population. For some factors, interventions can be fairly simple by using straightforward programs, such as lifestyle campaigns. To combat loneliness, group interventions are recommended that focus on educational or social activities for specific target groups and not only on home visits or learning how to make friends [33]. In addition, as lifestyle habits, loneliness, and mastery are formed in the broad context of the individual’s living environment, the causes of the causes need to be assessed as well [5]. This requires consideration of a broad range of domains beyond healthcare, such as labor, housing, education, and living environment [4, 34]. More qualitative and regional research is needed to determine exactly which problems are prevalent and which interventions are best suited for these problems.
Based on this study, no recommendations on specific interventions can be made, but we have shown that investments in these interventions can reduce regional health inequalities. Prioritization of vulnerable regions and populations is recommended [34], which is also in the interest of a regional approach of making healthcare more sustainable and looking at a broader perspective of prosperity [35, 36]. Regions are taking on a greater role in realizing local partnerships at the border of medical and social care and in bringing coherence in health, income, and well-being [35]. Aside from the possible leads this research offers based on the corrected differences, the uncorrected differences visualize the actual situation in vulnerable regions and indicate that extra attention and investments are needed.
A possible limitation of the study is related to the composition of the sample. It is known that certain groups are less inclined to participate in surveys, for example people with a lower SES and/or worse health, which can lead to selection bias [37]. In addition, people in the lowest income quartile were underrepresented in this dataset (12.6% of the respondents). This was taken into account when sampling and analyzing the data by adding weighting factors. Nevertheless, the weighted data showed that 18.7% of the respondents belonged to the lowest quartile. In addition, institutionalized residents are not included in the sample of the Health Survey. The underrepresentation of this group of citizens may have underestimated the actual regional health inequalities.
A second limitation is the use of cross-sectional data. This made it impossible to draw causal conclusions, and we could only analyze possible associations. For example, regions with more lonely residents can, as a result, become unhealthier. At the same time, citizens become lonelier as a result of their poorer health.
In conclusion, lifestyle factors, loneliness, and mastery are possible starting points for the explanation of regional health inequalities in the Netherlands. Lifestyle factors contribute to the explanation of regional differences in self-rated health and presence of chronic disease. In addition, loneliness and mastery contribute to explaining regional differences in self-perceived health, chronic disease, and psychological distress.
Open Access This article is licensed under a Creative Commons Attribution 4.0 International License, which permits use, sharing, adaptation, distribution and reproduction in any medium or format, as long as you give appropriate credit to the original author(s) and the source, provide a link to the Creative Commons licence, and indicate if changes were made. The images or other third party material in this article are included in the article’s Creative Commons licence, unless indicated otherwise in a credit line to the material. If material is not included in the article’s Creative Commons licence and your intended use is not permitted by statutory regulation or exceeds the permitted use, you will need to obtain permission directly from the copyright holder. To view a copy of this licence, visit http://​creativecommons.​org/​licenses/​by/​4.​0/​.
share
DELEN

Deel dit onderdeel of sectie (kopieer de link)

  • Optie A:
    Klik op de rechtermuisknop op de link en selecteer de optie “linkadres kopiëren”
  • Optie B:
    Deel de link per e-mail

Onze productaanbevelingen

Tijdschrift voor gezondheidswetenschappen

TSG, het Tijdschrift voor Gezondheidswetenschappen, is het enige Nederlandstalige tijdschrift dat multidisciplinaire informatie bevat op het gebied van de volksgezondheid en gezondheidszorg. Naast de multidisciplinaire oriëntatie is de combinatie van wetenschap, beleid en praktijk uniek.

BSL Academy SW-TP Saxion Parttime School jaarlicentie

BSL Academy SW-TP Saxion Parttime School half jaarlicentie

Toon meer producten
Bijlagen

Supplementary Information

Literatuur
4.
go back to reference Bussemaker J, ’S Jongers T, Vonk R. Gezondheidsverschillen voorbij. Tijdschr Gezondheidswet. 2021;99:36–9.CrossRef Bussemaker J, ’S Jongers T, Vonk R. Gezondheidsverschillen voorbij. Tijdschr Gezondheidswet. 2021;99:36–9.CrossRef
5.
go back to reference Marmot M, Allen J, Bell R, Bloomer E, Goldblatt P. WHO European review of social determinants of health and the health divide. Lancet. 2012;380:1011–29.CrossRef Marmot M, Allen J, Bell R, Bloomer E, Goldblatt P. WHO European review of social determinants of health and the health divide. Lancet. 2012;380:1011–29.CrossRef
7.
go back to reference Lantz PM, Lynch JW, House JS, Lepkowski JM, Mero RP, Musick MA, Williams DR. Socioeconomic disparities in health change in a longitudinal study of US adults: the role of health-risk behaviors. Soc Sci Med. 2001;53(1):29–40.CrossRef Lantz PM, Lynch JW, House JS, Lepkowski JM, Mero RP, Musick MA, Williams DR. Socioeconomic disparities in health change in a longitudinal study of US adults: the role of health-risk behaviors. Soc Sci Med. 2001;53(1):29–40.CrossRef
8.
go back to reference Frank J, Abel T, Campostrini S, Cook S, Lin VK, McQueen DV. The social determinants of health: time to re-think? Int J Environ Res Public Health. 2020;17(5856):1–8. Frank J, Abel T, Campostrini S, Cook S, Lin VK, McQueen DV. The social determinants of health: time to re-think? Int J Environ Res Public Health. 2020;17(5856):1–8.
9.
go back to reference de Jong-Gierveld J, Kamphuls F. The development of a Rasch-type loneliness scale. Appl Psychol Meas. 1985;9(3):289–99.CrossRef de Jong-Gierveld J, Kamphuls F. The development of a Rasch-type loneliness scale. Appl Psychol Meas. 1985;9(3):289–99.CrossRef
10.
go back to reference Holt-Lunstad J, Smith TB, Baker M, Harris T, Stephenson D. Loneliness and social isolation as risk factors for mortality: a meta-analytic review. Perspect Psychol Sci. 2015;10(2):227–37.CrossRef Holt-Lunstad J, Smith TB, Baker M, Harris T, Stephenson D. Loneliness and social isolation as risk factors for mortality: a meta-analytic review. Perspect Psychol Sci. 2015;10(2):227–37.CrossRef
11.
go back to reference Beutel ME, Klein EM, Brähler E, Reiner I, Jünger C, Michal M, et al. Loneliness in the general population: prevalence, determinants and relations to mental health. BMC Psychiatry. 2017;17(1):97.CrossRef Beutel ME, Klein EM, Brähler E, Reiner I, Jünger C, Michal M, et al. Loneliness in the general population: prevalence, determinants and relations to mental health. BMC Psychiatry. 2017;17(1):97.CrossRef
12.
go back to reference Lauder W, Mummery K, Jones M, Caperchione C. A comparison of health behaviours in lonely and non-lonely populations. Psychol Health Med. 2006;11(2):233–45.CrossRef Lauder W, Mummery K, Jones M, Caperchione C. A comparison of health behaviours in lonely and non-lonely populations. Psychol Health Med. 2006;11(2):233–45.CrossRef
16.
go back to reference Kessler RC, Andrews G, Colpe LJ, Hiripi E, Mroczek DK, Normand S‑L, et al. Short screening scales to monitor population prevalences and trends in non-specific psychological distress. Psychol Med. 2002;32(6):959–76.CrossRef Kessler RC, Andrews G, Colpe LJ, Hiripi E, Mroczek DK, Normand S‑L, et al. Short screening scales to monitor population prevalences and trends in non-specific psychological distress. Psychol Med. 2002;32(6):959–76.CrossRef
17.
go back to reference Arber S, Fenn K, Meadows R. Subjective financial well-being, income and health inequalities in mid and later life in Britain. Soc Sci Med. 2014;100:12–20.CrossRef Arber S, Fenn K, Meadows R. Subjective financial well-being, income and health inequalities in mid and later life in Britain. Soc Sci Med. 2014;100:12–20.CrossRef
20.
go back to reference van Tilburg T, de Jong-Gierveld J. Cesuurbepaling van de eenzaamheidsschaal. Tijdschr Gerontol Geriatr. 1999;30:158–63. van Tilburg T, de Jong-Gierveld J. Cesuurbepaling van de eenzaamheidsschaal. Tijdschr Gerontol Geriatr. 1999;30:158–63.
21.
go back to reference Pearlin LI, Schooler C. The structure of coping. J Health Soc Behav. 1978;19:2–21.CrossRef Pearlin LI, Schooler C. The structure of coping. J Health Soc Behav. 1978;19:2–21.CrossRef
22.
go back to reference Chen W, Qian L, Shi J, Franklin M. Comparing performance between log-binomial and robust Poisson regression models for estimating risk ratios under model misspecification. BMC Med Res Methodol. 2018;18(1):63.CrossRef Chen W, Qian L, Shi J, Franklin M. Comparing performance between log-binomial and robust Poisson regression models for estimating risk ratios under model misspecification. BMC Med Res Methodol. 2018;18(1):63.CrossRef
23.
go back to reference van Buuren S. Flexible imputation of missing data. Boca Raton: CRC press; 2018.CrossRef van Buuren S. Flexible imputation of missing data. Boca Raton: CRC press; 2018.CrossRef
24.
go back to reference StataCorp. Stata statistical software: release 16. College Station: StataCorp LLC; 2019. StataCorp. Stata statistical software: release 16. College Station: StataCorp LLC; 2019.
25.
go back to reference Montez JK, Zajacova A, Hayward MD. Explaining inequalities in women’s mortality between U.S. States. SSM Popul Health. 2016;2:561–71.CrossRef Montez JK, Zajacova A, Hayward MD. Explaining inequalities in women’s mortality between U.S. States. SSM Popul Health. 2016;2:561–71.CrossRef
26.
go back to reference Kumar R, Dalton AR. The English North-South divide: risk factors for cardiovascular disease accounting for cross-sectional socio-economic position. Perspect Public Health. 2014;134(6):339–45.CrossRef Kumar R, Dalton AR. The English North-South divide: risk factors for cardiovascular disease accounting for cross-sectional socio-economic position. Perspect Public Health. 2014;134(6):339–45.CrossRef
27.
go back to reference Putrik P, van Amelsvoort L, Mujakovic S, Kunst AE, van Oers H, Kant I, et al. Assessing the role of criminality in neighbourhood safety feelings and self-reported health: results from a cross-sectional study in a Dutch municipality. BMC Public Health. 2019;19(1):920.CrossRef Putrik P, van Amelsvoort L, Mujakovic S, Kunst AE, van Oers H, Kant I, et al. Assessing the role of criminality in neighbourhood safety feelings and self-reported health: results from a cross-sectional study in a Dutch municipality. BMC Public Health. 2019;19(1):920.CrossRef
28.
go back to reference Putrik P, de Vries NK, Mujakovic S, van Amelsvoort L, Kant I, Kunst AE, et al. Living environment matters: relationships between neighborhood characteristics and health of the residents in a Dutch municipality. J Community Health. 2015;40(1):47–56.CrossRef Putrik P, de Vries NK, Mujakovic S, van Amelsvoort L, Kant I, Kunst AE, et al. Living environment matters: relationships between neighborhood characteristics and health of the residents in a Dutch municipality. J Community Health. 2015;40(1):47–56.CrossRef
33.
go back to reference Cattan M, White M, Bond J, Learmouth A. Preventing social isolation and loneliness among older people: a systematic review of health promotion interventions. Ageing Soc. 2005;25(1):41–67.CrossRef Cattan M, White M, Bond J, Learmouth A. Preventing social isolation and loneliness among older people: a systematic review of health promotion interventions. Ageing Soc. 2005;25(1):41–67.CrossRef
36.
go back to reference van Bavel B, Hardeman S, Rijpma A. Vervolgstappen voor integrale welvaartsmeting. Econ Stat Ber. 2019;104(4772s):22–5. van Bavel B, Hardeman S, Rijpma A. Vervolgstappen voor integrale welvaartsmeting. Econ Stat Ber. 2019;104(4772s):22–5.
37.
go back to reference Lorant V, Demarest S, Miermans P‑J, van Oyen H. Survey error in measuring socio-economic risk factors of health status: a comparison of a survey and a census. Int J Epidemiol. 2007;36(6):1292–9.CrossRef Lorant V, Demarest S, Miermans P‑J, van Oyen H. Survey error in measuring socio-economic risk factors of health status: a comparison of a survey and a census. Int J Epidemiol. 2007;36(6):1292–9.CrossRef
Metagegevens
Titel
Regional differences in health further explained
The contribution of lifestyle, loneliness and mastery
Auteurs
Rachelle Meisters
Polina Putrik
Daan Westra
Hans Bosma
Dirk Ruwaard
Maria Jansen
Publicatiedatum
02-11-2022
Uitgeverij
Bohn Stafleu van Loghum
Gepubliceerd in
TSG - Tijdschrift voor gezondheidswetenschappen / Uitgave 4/2022
Print ISSN: 1388-7491
Elektronisch ISSN: 1876-8776
DOI
https://doi.org/10.1007/s12508-022-00368-5

Andere artikelen Uitgave 4/2022

TSG - Tijdschrift voor gezondheidswetenschappen 4/2022 Naar de uitgave