Skip to main content

Assessing the role of criminality in neighbourhood safety feelings and self-reported health: results from a cross-sectional study in a Dutch municipality

Abstract

Background

Neighbourhood safety has repeatedly been shown to be associated with the health and well-being of the residents. Criminality is often seen as one of the key factors affecting neighbourhood safety. However, the relationship between crime, fear of crime and feelings of safety remains underexplored.

Methods

Data on socio-demographic, health and safety perceptions was extracted from the Maastricht municipality survey (the Netherlands) (n = 9656 adults) and merged with data on official neighbourhood crime rates from the Police Registry. Pearson correlation coefficients and multilevel logistic regression models were computed to assess the association between aspects of objective and perceived criminality, individuals’ feelings of safety and health.

Results

The correlation between the police recorded crime and residents’ perceptions of the neighbourhood crime rates was weak (0.14–0.38), with the exception of violent crime (0.59), which indicates that other factors contribute to the perceptions of safety. In turn, the perception of higher rates of violent crime and more nuisance (on the scale 0–10) but not other types of crime or nuisance was positively associated with feeling unsafe (OR 1.27 [1.22;1.32] and 1.39 [1.33;1.46], respectively). Lower general feelings of safety at both the individual and neighbourhood level were consistently associated with worse self-rated health. Among different indicators of safety, the general feelings of safety had the most pronounced association with health, while subjective or objective measures of crime showed limited to no direct relationship with health.

Conclusions

Public health policies targeting safety as a social determinant of health should consider prioritizing areas of violent crime and nuisance to improve general feelings of safety. Further research is needed to understand which factors aside from criminality are driving residents’ feelings of safety.

Peer Review reports

Background

Living in unsafe neighbourhoods has repeatedly been shown to be associated with poor mental and physical health and lower well-being of the residents. Crime, but also fear of crime and general feelings of safety have been associated with worse self-perceived health [1, 2], higher levels of stress, more depressive symptoms and worse mental health [3,4,5,6], increased risk of coronary heart disease [7], less physical activity [8] and even adverse birth outcomes. [9] A recent study from New Zealand showed that living in an unsafe environment resulted in high cortisol (stress) hormones in pregnant women as well as poor self-rated health, with a potential impact on maternal and child health. [10] Another study from the US suggested that unsafe feelings act as a mediator between low socio-economic status and worse self-rated health. [11] Understanding the complex phenomenon of neighbourhood safety is one of the promising ways to tackle urban health issues.

In research on neighbourhood characteristics and health outcomes, aspects of the neighbourhood environment can be categorized as either objective or subjective. [12] Objective aspects are independent of an individual’s own perception. Examples include quantity and proximity to places such as health and educational facilities, parks, bars, shops, hospitals, number of accidents and crimes in the neighbourhood, etc. This information can be derived from administrative databases, official registers, researchers’ observations, and geographic information systems. Subjective neighbourhood measures are individuals’ assessments, or perceptions, of the neighbourhood living environment and may include similar domains to the objective aspects, such as perceived reachability and access to facilities, perceived criminality and safety feelings, or domains that are almost exclusively measured by individuals’ perceptions, such as social cohesion. [3, 12, 13]

Studies of neighbourhood safety often focused exclusively on the relationship between either perceived (e.g. fear of crime or general feelings of safety) or objective safety (i.e. recorded crime and nuisance) and health. To our knowledge, relatively few studies have explored both measures in the same study population and reflected upon a relationship between them. Some of these studies reported the independent contribution of both perceived and objective safety to health, as well as the mediating role of perceived safety in a link between objective safety and health. [4, 14] Wilson-Genderson et al. demonstrated that both actual levels of neighbourhood violence and individual perceptions of neighbourhood safety had significant effects on the depressive symptoms experienced by community-dwelling older adults. [3] Another study explored inter-relationships between objective (census-based) and subjective (resident-reported) features of the residential environment, including safety, in African-American women. [15]

However, the link between objective safety (registered crime rates), fear of crime and feelings of safety is more complex than it may intuitively seem, partly because perceived safety may cover a broader perception of the social and physical environment than just issues specifically related to crime [6, 16, 17]. Furthermore, it is possible that some types of crime contribute more to feelings of unsafety than others. To our knowledge, this has never been explored in detail. Although some evidence suggests that measures of perceived and objective safety often do not follow similar patterns, still no clear picture exists of how the objective and perceived criminality and feelings of safety are related to each other [12].

At the same time, it is of great importance for policy-makers to know how different aspects of safety relate to each other and, more importantly, to health, in addition to the notion that addressing safety has a high potential for public health. Better understanding of the underlying factors for negative safety perceptions is essential for targeted interventions to strengthen feelings of safety in the community and to improve health in the longer term.

The main objective of this study was to explore how objective and perceived neighbourhood criminality relate to each other and to feelings of safety and health, and the role of demographic and socio-economic factors in these relationships. The following research questions were set:

  1. (1)

    How do objective (police-recorded) and subjective (perceived) crime rates relate to feelings of safety?

  2. (2)

    How do objective (police-recorded) and subjective (perceived) crime rates relate to each other?

  3. (3)

    How do objective (police-recorded) and subjective (perceived) crime rates relate to self-rated health?

A recent study by Jackson et al. showed that individual demographic (gender in particular) and socio-economic characteristics are influential beyond the independent effect of observed social disorder cues. [18] Other research suggests that older people and persons with lower SES feel generally more vulnerable and less safe. [19, 20] Further, safety perceptions of different types of crime may differ substantially by gender, as females tend to report more unsafety feelings compared to males. [14, 21, 22] Therefore, we explored whether relationships between objective and subjective measures of safety differ by gender, age or socio-economic status.

Methods

Sources of data

We used data from two sources. The first source was cross-sectional survey data from the local authorities of Maastricht, the largest municipality in southern Limburg (122,488 inhabitants, 60.03 km2 [23]). This survey is conducted biannually by the municipal authorities among non-institutionalized inhabitants aged 18 years or over, and uses a probability sampling technique: a questionnaire is sent to a household, and the adult (≥18) person whose birthday comes first after the date on which the questionnaire was received is asked to complete it. The survey includes questions on perceived neighbourhood environment (including safety perceptions), demographic and socio-economic background, and health. We used the data from the 2010 survey (9656 respondents). The second source was the Police Registry of Crime from 2010. The registry contains the number of registered crimes (per calendar year) and the neighbourhood postal code where the crime took place.

Thirty-nine neighbourhoods (as defined by the “buurt code” by Statistics Netherlands [24]) were included in the analyses (150 to 6305 inhabitants per neighbourhood). Very small neighbourhoods with fewer than 100 residents (n = 3) were excluded from the sample in advance.

Outcome variables

Individual’s feelings of safety (referred to as perceived safety) and self-rated health were the main outcomes. Survey respondents were asked whether they perceive their neighbourhood as safe generally (no vs yes), and safe at night specifically (no vs yes). Self-rated health was measured by a question, “How would you rate your health in general?”, with five answer categories further dichotomized as poor (fair or poor) vs good (excellent, very good or good).

Independent variables

The municipality survey provided data on demographics (age and gender) and socio-economic status. Socio-economic status was assessed by level of educational achievement and income group. The six education categories mentioned in the questionnaire were classified as low education level (primary education, lower vocational education, pre-vocational secondary education), secondary education (secondary vocational education, senior general secondary education/pre-university education) or high education level (Bachelor’s degree and higher). Income group was self-reported by the respondents as ‘low’, ‘middle’ or ‘high’.

The municipality survey included questions on perceived frequency of crime and nuisance (referred to as subjective crime perceptions). Three types of crime were recorded: thefts (bicycle thefts, thefts of outside parts of the car, damage and theft from the car, car thefts and pick-pocketing), burglaries, and violent crimes (threatening and actual violence). For nuisance, the following three types of variables were collected: traffic nuisance (frequent aggressive traffic behaviour, frequently exceeding speed limits, frequent noise from traffic, frequent smell from traffic), nuisance from neighbours (quarrels between neighbours, youth criminality, nuisance from groups of young people, nuisance from drunk people and bars and discos), and vandalism (damage to walls and buildings, damage to telephone booths). The answer categories included never, sometimes, and always. These items were grouped to match the categories of Police Registry of Crime and nuisance (number of claims (i.e. registered complaints not necessarily confirmed and prosecuted) per 100 neighbourhood inhabitants per year), namely, thefts, burglaries, violent crime, traffic nuisance, nuisance from neighbours, and vandalism (referred to as objective crime rates). Each individual item from the survey data was scored as 10 (always), 5 (sometimes) and 0 (never), and average of these items was assigned to each type of crime (Table 2).

Statistical analyses

First, Pearson correlation coefficients between actual and perceived crime rates for each of the six types of crime and nuisance were computed for the general sample and separately for sub-groups organized by gender, age (< 65 years old and ≥ 65 years old), education and income. Correlation coefficients < 0.50 were assumed to be weak, between 0.50 and 0.80 were moderate, and > 0.80 were strong. [25] Next, multilevel logistic regression models were used to take into account that characteristics of respondents (modelled as fixed effects) cluster in neighbourhoods’ random effects. Two multiple logistic regression models were computed with recorded crime rates (at the neighbourhood level) and perceived crime rates (aggregated at the neighbourhood level) as variables of interest, respectively. A manual forward selection modelling approach was used (cut-off p-value = 0.05). Feeling unsafe, feeling unsafe at night, and health were the outcomes (all dichotomized as unsafe/safe or poor/good in case of health), and all models were adjusted for potential confounders: individual age, gender, education and income. In models with perceived crime, due to high (> 0.8) correlations between nuisance and violent crime, each of the six types of perceived crime was first modelled in a separate regression model, and then two variations of multiple regression model were computed: one excluding perceived nuisance and the second excluding perceived violent crime.

As persons who tend to have more negative perceptions of life generally or who are less healthy may be reporting both a higher perceived frequency of crime and feeling unsafe in their neighbourhood, including only individual-level measures of perceived crime and feelings of safety (from the same source) can compromise the results (same source bias). In order to separate the impact of individual perception of frequency of a particular type of crime on feeling unsafe from the impact of neighbourhood safety as a feature of the neighbourhood social environment, we computed an aggregated perception of frequency of each type of crime (i.e. an averaged measure computed from the answers of all residents of a particular neighbourhood). This aggregated measure is less sensitive to individual perceptions and may therefore be considered as leading to more objective findings. At the same time, the contribution of individual perceptions of the environment to the individual health outcome was taken into account by including a second variable which was computed as the difference between the neighbourhood mean and the assessment given by an individual. Thus, each of the six variables measuring the perception of frequency of certain types of crime was included in the model using two variables: (1) the mean of scores given by respondents from the same neighbourhood and (2) the difference between neighbourhood mean and the individual score. To explore the relationship between feelings of safety and self-rated health, feelings of safety were added to the model first at the individual level, and then as a percentage of persons in the neighbourhood who reported feeling unsafe.

To explore whether the associations between crime, unsafety feelings and health depend on the individual’s demographic or socio-economic characteristics, interactions between objectively and subjectively measured crime rates and age, gender, education and income were tested (cut-off p-value for interaction term was 0.10). Analyses were performed on the complete cases available for each model. Statistical package STATA 12 was used. [26]

Results

Study population

In total, 9656 residents of Maastricht were included in the study (response rate 25%). On average, 248 (SD 117) persons per neighbourhood returned the questionnaire (Table 6 in online Appendix). The mean age of the respondents was 55 years, and 50% was male. Some 3192 (33%) respondents were educated to a low primary level, 2284 (24%) had completed secondary education, and almost 40% (n = 3817) had the highest level of educational achievement. More than half of the respondents (n = 4911; 51%) classified themselves in the middle income group. In total, one-third of the respondents reported feeling unsafe in the neighbourhood (n = 3297; 34%), and two-thirds felt unsafe in the neighbourhood at night (n = 6428; 67%) (Table 1). In total, 13,743 crime claims were registered by police in Maastricht in 2010, or on average 14.23 per 100 residents, ranging from 3.47 in the neighbourhoods with least crime to 74.03 in the neighbourhoods with highest reported crime. Of those, 5160 (38%) claims were about thefts, 3187 (23%) concerned nuisance, 1503 (11%) burglaries, 1434 (10%) traffic complaints, 1410 (10%) acts of vandalism, and 1049 (8%) violence.

Table 1 Socio-demographic characteristics of the sample and perceived safety (n = 9656)

Substantial differences were found in objective and perceived criminality between neighbourhoods. In the crime registry, the most frequent crime was burglary (5.19 per 100) of residents, followed by traffic accidents (Table 2).

Table 2 Neighbourhood perceived and objective criminality rates, in 2010

Relationship between objectively and subjectively measured neighbourhood criminality

Registered crime rates had only a weak correlation with the perceived frequency of crime, with the exception of violent crime, which showed a moderate correlation (Pearson correlation coefficient = 0.6). When the correlation analysis was repeated in subsamples of males and females separately, consistently stronger correlations for all six types of crime were observed in males, while in females five out of six correlation coefficients became weaker than in the general sample. The subgroup differences reached statistical significance for violent crime and thefts. Objective and subjective rates for violent crime had the highest correlation in both males and females compared to other types of crime, with a very strong correlation in the male subsample. No relevant correlation patterns were observed by age or socio-economic status, except for vandalism, where the correlation between actual and perceived nuisance was stronger among lower- and middle-educated respondents (Table 3).

Table 3 Pearson correlation coefficients between official crime rates and crime perception

Relationship between police-recorded crime rates and feelings of unsafety and self-rated health among residents

Among registered crime rates, only violent crime and nuisance caused by neighbours remained in the final multiple regression models with both general safety feelings and safety feelings at night as the outcome (separate model for each outcome) (Table 4). One additional violent crime per 100 residents of the neighbourhood was associated with 1.24 [95% CI1.02;1.51] higher odds of feeling unsafe (and 1.28 [95%CI 1.10;1.51] of feeling unsafe at night). Every additional complaint about nuisance from neighbours was associated with 1.18 [95%CI 1.08;1.30] higher OR of feeling unsafe. Significant interactions (p < 0.05) between violent crime and individual’s education level were detected. Stratification revealed that in highly educated persons, the association between registered violent crimes and safety feelings is not significant, while it is more pronounced in middle- and lower-educated groups (OR 1.28 [1.06;1.54], 1.38 [1.09;1.75] and 1.22 [0.99; 1.53] for lower, middle and highly educated respondents, respectively). The remaining interactions were not significant (p > 0.05). None of the indicators of the registered crime was associated with the self-rated health of the residents (p > 0.05; data not shown).

Table 4 Neighbourhood objective criminality rates and feelings of safety. Results from multiple multilevel logistic regression models

Relationship between perceived crime rates and feelings of unsafety and self-rated health among residents

The aggregated residents’ perceptions of frequency of all six types of crime studied were significantly associated with individual feelings of unsafety. Again, the perception of higher rates of violent crime and more nuisance from neighbours was associated with the largest odds of feeling unsafe (OR 1.27 [1.22;1.32] and 1.39 [1.33;1.46], respectively) compared to the other four types of crime and nuisance (Table 5). Significant interactions were observed between burglary and vandalism with gender: perceived frequency of burglaries and vandal acts was associated with unsafety feelings in females but not in males. ORs were 1.08 [1.00;1.17] and 0.98 [0.91;1.06] for burglaries in females and males, respectively, and 1.15 [1.04; 1.28] and 1.02 [0.91;1.13] for vandalism in females and males, respectively. The remaining interactions were not significant (p > 0.10).

Table 5 Associations between perceived criminality (by type of crime) at neighbourhood level, feeling unsafe (at night) and self-rated health

When self-rated health was the outcome, only two out of six indicators of aggregated perceived crime showed significant associations, namely, perceived frequency of violent crime (OR 1.14 [1.04; 1.26]) and perceived frequency of traffic nuisance (OR 1.15 [1.06; 1.24]) (Table 5). Residents who reported feeling unsafe in their neighbourhood also reported worse health (OR 1.73 [1.54;1.94] and OR 1.43 [1.25;1.63] for general feelings of safety and specifically at night, respectively). This effect was still significant when safety was considered at the aggregated neighbourhood level, as the percentage of persons who reported feeling unsafe: residents from neighbourhoods where more people reported feeling unsafe were more likely to report worse health (OR 1.01 [1.00;1.01] for both general safety and at night, per 1% increase in respondents in the neighbourhood reporting not feeling safe). This means in a neighbourhood where 10% more residents report feeling unsafe, there are 1.06 [95% CI 1.02;1.10] higher odds that a resident will report poor health. It was striking that the percentage of residents in the neighbourhood who reported feeling unsafe ranged from 12 to 63%. Sensitivity analyses yielded similar results to the main analyses (Tables 9, 10 and 11 in the online Appendix).

Discussion

The objective of this study was to explore how crime and fear of crime are associated with each other and with an individual’s feelings of safety and health. With regard to our first research question (How do objective and subjective crime rates relate to feelings of safety?), we found that among objectively (police) registered crime, only violent crime and nuisance from neighbours were associated with higher odds of feeling unsafe, while police records of thefts, burglary, traffic nuisance and vandalism were not significantly related to residents’ feelings of unsafety. At the same time, individuals’ perceptions of frequency of any of the six types of crime (e.g. fear of crime) were always significantly related to feelings of safety, with violent crime and nuisance having the strongest association with unsafety feelings.

The answer to our second research question (How do objective and subjective crime rates relate to each other?) is that police-registered frequency and perceived frequency of crime showed only a moderate correlation for violent crime, and no or very weak correlations for other types of crime. Interestingly, the correlations were more pronounced in men, which might indicate that men have a more realistic perception of the actual crime situation in the neighbourhood. Lower- and middle-educated people were more aware of vandalism crimes in their neighbourhood and their awareness correlated well with the official registry, while those who were highly educated seemed not to be triggered by graffiti or limited damage to their physical environment. Vandalism is an indirect indicator of crime and disorder, which might become a trigger when people are confronted with a range of other social and safety issues that are known to cluster as the socio-economic status decreases.

With regard to our third research question (How do objective and subjective crime rates relate to self-rated health?), we found no direct association between objective crime rates and health, a limited relationship between fear of crime and health (only in case of violent crime and traffic nuisance), while worse general feelings of safety showed a consistent association with poorer health at the individual and neighbourhood levels. Of note, no substantially different findings were found in any of the analyses when feelings of unsafety specifically at night was an outcome.

Our findings that official crime rates make a modest contribution to explaining safety feelings are consistent with previous studies. [6, 27] Studies in the UK and Canada demonstrated that people consider factors like visibility and outside lighting, presence of groups of young people and ethnic minorities on the street, media reporting of crime, etc. in their judgement about their feelings of safety. [17, 28] In the Netherlands, data from other sources also indirectly support our findings, indicating that whereas actual crime rates are falling, feelings of unsafety remain stable. [29] Apparently, factors aside from crime ultimately define how neighbourhood residents perceive safety.

Furthermore, we observed a clear gender difference in the relation between objective and perceived crime frequency. Females reported higher rates of all other types of crime, and these correlated poorly with the objective numbers, in particular for violent crime and thefts. Furthermore, similar perceptions of the frequency of burglaries and acts of vandalism translated into stronger unsafety feelings in females, but not in males. A recent study by Lovasi et al. also observed that “dishonesty crime” (i.e. crime involving destruction of property) has been associated with unsafety feelings only among females. Unlike our results, Lovasi et al. didn’t find such a pattern in case of burglaries, possibly because they were analyzed together with thefts (‘property crime’), while we separated them in our study. [14] Females feel more vulnerable and are apparently more sensitive to indirect indicators of crime such as vandalism and are more anxious about burglaries. These findings suggest a strong gender component in safety feelings that should be considered by local policy-makers when designing and implementing initiatives to address issues around safety. Female perspectives on safety should not be neglected and overlooked in more generalist approaches to neighbourhood policies.

We have further demonstrated that objective crime rates have no direct relationship to self-rated health, and perceived crime has only a limited relationship to health, while more general feelings of safety have a strong association with self-rated health, and this finding is consistent with previous publications in the field. [1, 3, 30] Objective crime is a statistic which essentially covers only some of the safety and crime issues in the neighbourhood. As such, it is not surprising that people are more sensitive to what they think is happening as opposed to what has been on the police radar. This suggests that policies purely targeted at amending criminality are not necessarily expected to achieve noticeable improvements in community health outcomes, while targeting safety feelings has a higher potential for public health. In this context, it is worthwhile mentioning recent work by Browning et al. focused on ecological (“eco-”) networks – networks linking households within neighbourhoods through shared activity locations. They observed that the intensity of such networks is inversely correlated with neighbourhood crime and thus offers promising opportunities for neighbourhood policies. [31] Future research is warranted to unravel factors aside from criminality in order to improve safety feelings. A mix of qualitative and quantitative methods will likely be required to go beyond the observed association and obtain insights into the ways that an individual’s perception around neighbourhood safety is formed.

The strength and novelty of our study consist in exploring the relationships between objective and perceived safety and health in greater detail, separating the specific types of crime and nuisance. While an earlier study claimed that addressing objectively measured criminality may not have a strong effect on improving safety perceptions and health [6], our study reveals that registered criminality is positively related to feelings of safety and unsafety only for certain types of crime (violent crime and nuisance), and thus interventions to lower these specific crime rates may have a higher potential impact on health via feelings of safety. At the same time, other types of objectively recorded crime were shown to be unrelated to the population’s safety feelings. While addressing them in public health policies may not bring the desired effect, they remain important for other spheres of social and community life.

Our study also has several notable limitations. First of all, the low response to the survey (25%) may have affected the findings, which is a common limitation to population survey data. Our sample was comparable to the general Maastricht population in terms of age, gender, and education, although the number of highly educated respondents was slightly higher (Table 12 in the online Appendix). Second, while there was a risk of confounding by unmeasured individual variables (e.g. ethnicity or cultural), we did adjust our models for important individual demographic and socio-economic variables, namely age, gender, education and income, as well as population density. Last but not least, issues related to defining the neighbourhood boundaries and the fact that respondents may not refer to the same area in their responses continuously hinder the efforts to measure the area-level effects on health. [1, 32,33,34,35]

In conclusion, our results suggest that individual perceptions of crime correlate poorly with officially recorded crime, and only violent crime and nuisance affect safety feelings. Among different indicators of safety, only general feelings of safety were positively related to health, while objective or subjective measures of crime showed little or no direct relationship to health. Public health policies targeting safety as a social determinant of health should consider prioritizing areas of violent crime and nuisance to improve general feelings of safety.

Availability of data and materials

Datasets may be shared after the approval of the data owners.

References

  1. Putrik P, de Vries NK, Mujakovic S, van Amelsvoort L, Kant I, Kunst AE, van Oers H, Jansen M. Living environment matters: relationships between neighborhood characteristics and health of the residents in a Dutch municipality. J Community Health. 2015;40(1):47–56.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  2. Green G, Gilbertson JM, Grimsley MF. Fear of crime and health in residential tower blocks. A case study in Liverpool, UK. Eur J Pub Health. 2002;12(1):10–5.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  3. Wilson-Genderson M, Pruchno R. Effects of neighborhood violence and perceptions of neighborhood safety on depressive symptoms of older adults. Soc Sci Med. 2013;85:43–9.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  4. Curry A, Latkin C, Davey-Rothwell M. Pathways to depression: the impact of neighborhood violent crime on inner-city residents in Baltimore, Maryland, USA. Soc Sci Med. 2008;67(1):23–30.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  5. Kruger DJ, Reischl TM, Gee GC. Neighborhood social conditions mediate the association between physical deterioration and mental health. Am J Community Psychol. 2007;40(3–4):261–71.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  6. Lorenc T, et al. Crime, fear of crime, environment, and mental health and wellbeing: mapping review of theories and causal pathways. Health Place. 2012;18(4):757–65.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  7. Sundquist K, et al. Neighborhood violent crime and unemployment increase the risk of coronary heart disease: a multilevel study in an urban setting. Soc Sci Med. 2006;62(8):2061–71.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  8. Rees-Punia E, Hathaway ED, Gay JL. Crime, perceived safety, and physical activity: a meta-analysis. Prev Med. 2018;111:307–13.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  9. Messer LC, et al. Violent crime exposure classification and adverse birth outcomes: a geographically-defined cohort study. Int J Health Geogr. 2006;5:22.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  10. Thayer ZM. Dark shadow of the long white cloud: neighborhood safety is associated with self-rated health and cortisol during pregnancy in Auckland, Aotearoa/New Zealand. SSM Popul Health. 2017;3:75–80.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  11. Meyer OL, Castro-Schilo L, Aguilar-Gaxiola S. Determinants of mental health and self-rated health: a model of socioeconomic status, neighborhood safety, and physical activity. Am J Public Health. 2014;104(9):1734–41.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  12. Weden MM, Carpiano RM, Robert SA. Subjective and objective neighborhood characteristics and adult health. Soc Sci Med. 2008;66(6):1256–70.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  13. Wen M, Hawkley LC, Cacioppo JT. Objective and perceived neighborhood environment, individual SES and psychosocial factors, and self-rated health: an analysis of older adults in Cook County, Illinois. Soc Sci Med. 2006;63(10):2575–90.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  14. Lovasi GS, et al. The independent associations of recorded crime and perceived safety with physical health in a nationally representative cross-sectional survey of men and women in New Zealand. BMJ Open. 2014;4(3):e004058.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  15. Sealy-Jefferson S, et al. Inter-relationships between objective and subjective measures of the residential environment among urban African American women. Ann Epidemiol. 2017;27(3):164–8.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  16. Farrall S, et al. Questioning the measurement of the “fear of crime”: findings from a major methodological study. Br J Criminol. 1997;37:658–79.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  17. Lorenc T, et al. Crime, fear of crime and mental health: synthesis of theory and systematic reviews of interventions and qualitative evidence. Public Health Research. Southampton: NIHR Journals Library; 2014.

    Google Scholar 

  18. Jackson AL, Soller B, Browning CR. The influence of Women's neighborhood resources on perceptions of social disorder. City Community. 2017;16(2):189–208.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  19. Covington JM, Taylor RB. Fear of crime in urban residential neighborhoods: implications of between and within-neighborhood sources for current models. Sociol Q. 1991;32:231.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  20. Greve W. Fear of crime among the elderly: foresight, not fright. Int Rev Victimol. 1998;5:277–309.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  21. Reid LW, Konrad M. The gender gap in fear: assessing the interactive effects of gender and perceived risk on fear of crime. Sociol Spectr. 2004;24:399–425.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  22. Logan TK, Walker R. The gender safety gap: examining the impact of victimization history, perceived risk, and personal control. J Interpers Violence. 2017:886260517729405. https://journals.sagepub.com/doi/full/10.1177/0886260517729405?url_ver=Z39.88-2003&rfr_id=ori%3Arid%3Acrossref.org&rfr_dat=cr_pub%3Dpubmed.

  23. CBS, Kerncijfers wijken en buurten” (Key figures for neighbourhoods). CBS Statline (Statistics Netherlands). Accessed 28-07-2014. 2014.

    Google Scholar 

  24. CBS, Central Bureau voor de Statistiek. http://statline.cbs.nl/statweb/.

  25. Zou KH, Tuncali K, Silverman SG. Correlation and simple linear regression. Radiology. 2003;227(3):617–22.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  26. StataCorp. Stata Statistical software: release 12. College Station: StataCorp LP; 2011.

    Google Scholar 

  27. Foster S, Giles-Corti B. The built environment, neighborhood crime and constrained physical activity: an exploration of inconsistent findings. Prev Med. 2008;47(3):241–51.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  28. Cote-Lussier C, et al. A child's view: social and physical environmental features differentially predict parent and child perceived neighborhood safety. J Urban Health. 2015;92(1):10–23.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  29. CBS. Data on feelings of safety ((on)veiligheidsbeleving) and registered crime (Geregistreerde criminaliteit; soort misdrijf en regio): Den Haag/Heerlen; 2015.

  30. Chandola T. The fear of crime and area differences in health. Health Place. 2001;7(2):105–16.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  31. Browning CR, et al. Ecological networks and urban crime: the structure of shared routine activity locations and neighborhood-level informal control capacity. Criminology. 2017;55(4):754–78.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  32. Stafford M, et al. Pathways to obesity: identifying local, modifiable determinants of physical activity and diet. Soc Sci Med. 2007;65(9):1882–97.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  33. Diez Roux AV. Neighborhoods and health: where are we and were do we go from here? Rev Epidemiol Sante Publique. 2007;55(1):13–21.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  34. Diez Roux AV. Investigating neighborhood and area effects on health. Am J Public Health. 2001;91(11):1783–9.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  35. Diez Roux AV, Mair C. Neighborhoods and health. Ann N Y Acad Sci. 2010;1186:125–45.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

Download references

Acknowledgements

Authors would like to thank Maastricht Municipality for providing the survey data.

Funding

This study did not receive specific funding and was performed as part of the research activities at Maastricht University.

Author information

Authors and Affiliations

Authors

Contributions

PP, MWJ and NDV have conceived the idea, PP performed the analyses and drafted the manuscript, LvA assisted with statistical analyses, all authors have contributed to interpretation of the findings, editing the manuscript and approved the final version.

Corresponding author

Correspondence to Polina Putrik.

Ethics declarations

Ethics approval and consent to participate

Ethical approval and consent were obtained when survey data was collected by Maastricht Municipality. This secondary analyses of the anonymised data did not require a separate ethical approval.

Consent for publication

All authors have read the final version and consented for publication.

Competing interests

The authors declare that they have no competing interests.

Additional information

Publisher’s Note

Springer Nature remains neutral with regard to jurisdictional claims in published maps and institutional affiliations.

Appendix

Appendix

Table 6 Number of respondents per neighborhood
Table 7 Associations between objective crime measures and self-perceived health. Results from multiple logistic regression models, each crime measure in a separate model
Table 8 Pearson correlation coefficients between perceptions of different types of crime
Table 9 Sensitivity analyses 1: Associations between perceived criminality (by type of crime) at neighborhood level, feeling unsafe (at night) and self-rated health. Models without correction for individual difference from the aggregated score on crime perception
Table 10 Sensitivity analyses 2: Associations between perceived criminality (by type of crime) at neighborhood level, feeling unsafe (at night) and self-rated health. Models ran on complete cases only for all the covariates
Table 11 Sensitivity analyses 3: Associations between perceived criminality (by type of crime) at neighborhood level, feeling unsafe (at night) and self-rated health. Models additionally adjusted for population density
Table 12 Dutch general population composition for age, gender and education, 2010

Rights and permissions

Open Access This article is distributed under the terms of the Creative Commons Attribution 4.0 International License (http://creativecommons.org/licenses/by/4.0/), which permits unrestricted use, distribution, and reproduction in any medium, provided you give appropriate credit to the original author(s) and the source, provide a link to the Creative Commons license, and indicate if changes were made. The Creative Commons Public Domain Dedication waiver (http://creativecommons.org/publicdomain/zero/1.0/) applies to the data made available in this article, unless otherwise stated.

Reprints and permissions

About this article

Check for updates. Verify currency and authenticity via CrossMark

Cite this article

Putrik, P., van Amelsvoort, L., Mujakovic, S. et al. Assessing the role of criminality in neighbourhood safety feelings and self-reported health: results from a cross-sectional study in a Dutch municipality. BMC Public Health 19, 920 (2019). https://doi.org/10.1186/s12889-019-7197-z

Download citation

  • Received:

  • Accepted:

  • Published:

  • DOI: https://doi.org/10.1186/s12889-019-7197-z

Keywords