Discussion
To our knowledge, this is the first study that has explored grandparents’ perceived needs in relation to their young grandchild diagnosed with ASD in the context of the Swedish support system. Overall, results indicate that grandparents expressed most needs with in the subscale pertaining to information, followed by needs in topics related to childcare, thus, confirming our first hypothesis; the least expressed needs were for financial support. Before we discuss the results, we would like to point to some observations made on the demographic characteristics of the study sample. The participants consisted of 120 traditional maternal and paternal grandfathers as well as step-grandparents. One important observation is that in relation to grandchild with ASD, our sample did not differ much in a number of maternal and paternal grandmothers (n = 36 and n = 35, respectively), or in a number of maternal and paternal grandfathers (n = 21 and n = 18, respectively). Previous research with grandparents of children with ASD reported that their samples included more maternal grandparents than paternal grandparents (Glasberg and Harris
1997; Hillman et al.
2016,
2017; Margetts et al.
2006; Prendeville and Kinsella
2018), or had a fewer number of paternal grandmothers compared to maternal grandmothers (Sullivan et al.
2012). In addition, the majority of grandparents reported residing in large urban settings, which is not surprising: their grandchildren received support services at the Habilitation & Health, Stockholm County Council, located in the capital city of Sweden, Stockholm. Another observation is that our participants represent culturally and linguistically homogenous group: only 11 grandparents (9.9%) reported having their mother language other than Swedish (Table
1), which is somewhat lower than expected, given the fact that modern Sweden is a multicultural society with nearly 20% of its total population that consists of nationalities from almost 200 countries (Tavallali et al.
2014). The overwhelming majority of our sample (84.7%) perceived their health condition as good, thus, supporting previous findings on self-rated health among grandparents in Europe (DiGessa et al.
2016a). We did not inquire about grandparents’ income to ensure our participants’ integrity, but also because we thought that the items in the Financial support subscale of the Grandparents Needs survey would provide sufficient information in regard to participants’ financial situation.
The analyses of the Grandparent Needs survey demonstrate that grandparents’ strongest perceived needs were in the area of information, especially, concerning how to help grandchildren develop skills, how to handle problem behavior, and the effect of autism on grandchildren. This finding is similar to the results reported by Dougherty (
2009) where a need for information was the greatest among grandparents of children with disabilities in the US context. Our findings are consistent with previous studies (Hillman et al.
2017; Prendeville and Kinsella
2018). For instance, in their qualitative study, Hillman et al. (
2017) reported that two of the most important priorities for grandparents were needs for information about how to handle grandchild’s behavior, and for information about ASD. These needs were preceded by the need for more information about how to help the grandchild develop skills as well as learn to manage disruptive behavior and tantrums (Hillman et al.
2017). Notably, as in the present study, these findings reported by grandparents echo the results reported by parents of children with ASD in Derguy’s et al. (
2015) study, for whom information about ASD and knowledge about management of child behavior were key areas of perceived needs.
However, in the information topic area, there were several notable differences between participants’ responses in our study and those reported by Dougherty (
2009), mostly, on the level of individual items: while Dougherty (
2009) reported grandparents’ need for information about laws and rights regulating provision of services as the greatest, the grandparents in the present study ranked this need as least important. Similarly, unlike Dougherty’s (
2009) findings, the topic area of financial support was among the least prioritized needs for the grandparents in our study. Differences in the results could be attributed to several factors. Firstly, the present study included grandparents of preschool-aged children with a clinical diagnosis of ASD, while Dougherty’s (
2009) study included grandparents of children with various ages (0–21), with disabilities not limited to ASD only, but also other types, e.g. Down syndrome, visual impairment, spina bifida, cerebral palsy. Secondly, this finding could be explained by differences in provision of support services in two distinct cultural contexts of Sweden and of the USA. Socio-cultural differences could as well explain several other findings in the present study in relation to findings from previous research on grandparents’ needs. For instance, in our study the least expressed need was found in the topic area of
professional support, namely, for item
Getting help from a leader of religious faith, when 99.1% of all participants (n = 115) reported no need of support in comparison with only one participant (0.9%) who disclosed a definite need of support in this area. (Of interest, this participant self-reported as not being of Swedish origin based on language as proxy for ethnicity). This result is consistent with evidence from previous research on religion, mental health, and well-being conducted with older adults in Sweden, indicating very low rates of religious involvement among Swedes with only 1% of those who sought support in religion to cope with stress (Cederblad et al.
1995; Koenig and Larson
2001; Cohen and Koenig
2003). The lack of need for religious support expressed by the overwhelming majority of grandparents in the present study somewhat supports Dougherty’s (
2009) finding showing that 67.2% of grandparents residing in the US state of Kentucky reported no need in getting assistance to meet with a leader of their religious faith. Similarly, Hillman et al. (
2016) noted that in their sample, only 24% of grandparents who coped poorly with their grandchild’s ASD sought professional assistance from clergy. Interestingly, these results, including our study, contrast with the finding reported in a study conducted in the USA 30 years ago (Vadasy et al.
1986) when the majority of grandparents of children with disabilities described their involvement into religious groups as active with 71% participating in church activities at least once a month, and with 67% reporting that their religion helped them understand and accept their grandchild with disability. Dissimilar results revealed by this earlier study by Vadasy et al. (
1986) and by the most recent ones (e.g. Dougherty
2009; Hillman et al.
2016) including the present study, could be explained by possible cohort effects. It is possible that within these cultural contexts religiosity in general has become less strong over time, and that for grandparents of children with ASD other sources of support may have become more salient than seeking support from religious leaders. Indeed, as Hillman et al.’s (
2016) study showed, of those grandparents who did not cope well with their grandchild with ASD had sought professional help by turning to support groups (24%), psychologists (17%), social workers or other therapists (17%). In the present study, a need for
Learning how to communicate with teachers and other professionals regarding grandchild with ASD together with a need for
Accessing family counselling for parents and grandparents were the most prioritized needs reported by grandparents in the category Professional Support (see Table B, Online Supplementary Material).
Another finding in the present study that could be attributed to cross-cultural differences concerns the item
Locating an appropriate childcare facility for their grandchild with ASD as the least reported need in the Childcare category. Consistent with previous research on cultural variations on the prevalence of grandparental childcare across European countries (Di Gessa et al.
2016; Muller and Litwin
2011), this finding might suggests that, generally, in Sweden grandparents rely on formal childcare provision ensured by the state’s welfare policy, and therefore, they might not feel a need to look for other appropriate childcare facilities for their grandchildren. As Di Gessa at al. (
2016) demonstrated, availability of formal public childcare provision coupled with women’s access to paid work is associated with provision of intensive grandparental childcare (i.e. at least 15 h/week). For instance, the authors showed that in Sweden, a country characterized with high level of formal public childcare provision, only 3.6% of parents received help from child’s grandparents compared to other European countries with lower levels of formal childcare provision, where parents had to rely more on their own parents in looking after children.
The present study also aimed at investigating possible relationships between grandparents’ socio-demographic characteristics and their perceived needs. The findings did not confirm our second hypothesis: there were no significant results in perceived needs based on grandparent’s relation to grandchild (i.e. lineage), gender, employment status, or educational background, or frequency of meeting grandchild (see Table
3). There were also no associations found between perceptions of needs and grandparents’ geographic proximity to the child as well as grandparents’ self-rated health. Based on previous research, we expected that at least one of the above-mentioned demographic variable would be associated with grandparents’ perceptions of needs. Indeed, studies with grandparents of children with various types of disabilities showed that both grandparents’ better health and higher education level correlated positively with lower level of needs (Dougherty
2009); whereas in Vadasy et al.’s (
1986) study, the grandparents who had a university degree expressed high level of needs for information about availability of various support services for their grandchildren and families. Previous research has also shown the importance of including the gender variable into analyses when studying family needs, with results indicating that women tend to express more needs than men in relation to their child with disability. For instance, the Wang and Michaels’s (
2009) study on parental needs revealed that mothers had greater needs compared to fathers on the information subscale. Yet, the results of the present study demonstrated no observed relationships between the gender variable and perception of needs among grandparents, adding to inconclusiveness of research evidence on the influence of demographic factors on family needs. We suggest that the results of the present study could be explained by the potential effects of the Swedish social welfare system characterized by gender equality (Kolk
2014) and high level of formal public childcare provision (Di Gessa
2016), indicating that equal access to social services, health care and childcare provision ensured by the state could be accounted for insignificant role of grandparents’ socioeconomic status, gender, lineage, or other demographic factors in providing grandchild care in Sweden. Future research could consider including broader societal factors such as social norms and existing policies regulating services and support to both young and elderly as moderating variables in models of analysis to understand their possible influence on grandparents’ perceptions of needs when having a grandchild with ASD in various cultural contexts.
Another explanation to the finding could be potential selection bias (Šimundić
2013) suggesting that grandparents who participated in our study might not be fully representative of grandparents of young children with ASD in Sweden. Indeed, as we have mentioned earlier, the participants in our study were not representative of grandparents with immigrant background. Besides, our sample represented those families whose children and their parents resided and obtained support services in the greater Stockholm area; moreover, these grandparents were invited to participate in the seminar as part of the service package specifically aimed to address grandparents’ informational needs related to ASD. Given that the overwhelming majority of the grandparents in our sample (84.7%) described their health as good, it is possible that needs of those grandparents who could not attend the seminars due to poor health would differ from those reported in the present study. Indeed, according to the data from the Swedish national public health survey (Public Health Agency of Sweden
2016), 62% of older people aged 65–84 years reported their health as “good” or “very good”. Furthermore, based on previous research (D’Astous et al.
2013), one could argue that it is possible that grandparents in our study chose to participate in the seminars because they had closer and more committed relationships with their adult children and grandchildren with ASD and, therefore, were more involved into grandchild care and wanted to gain more knowledge about how to support their families in the most optimal way. Further research involving more heterogeneous and representative samples selected by using random methods is required to examine possible relations between grandparents’ needs and socio-demographic characteristics in diverse subgroups of grandparents of young children with ASD, including those with ethnic and cultural background other than Swedish and those residing in geographic areas other than the greater Stockholm region. Qualitative research designs could shed light on how patterns of relationships between grandparents and their adult children might affect grandparents’ level of involvement with grandchild with autism, which in turn could help understand further the unique needs of these grandparents.
The results of our study revealed no observed associations between grandparents’ perceived needs and their perceptions of grandchildren’s difficulties or impact these difficulties might interfere with their grandchild’s social life (total impact score) as measured by the SDQ Impact supplement scale. The most plausible explanation to this finding could be grandparents’ difficulty to describe their grandchildren’s functioning in everyday life. In this study, all but one participating grandparents reported not living together with their grandchildren in the same household. It is not surprising, given the fact that in Sweden family support seldom involves multi-generational co-residence (Muller and Litwin
2011). Another possible explanation to this finding could be the instrument chosen for data collection. The SDQ Impact supplement scale was primarily designed to be completed by immediate family members, i.e. parents, and by teachers (Goodman
1999) who meet and interact with children on a daily basis, and who can make reliable observations of child’s behavior throughout the day, suggesting that the instrument might not be entirely suitable for use with non-custodial grandparents, as they do not always meet their grandchildren on a daily basis and, therefore, lacked information on grandchild’s social functioning. Future researchers may want to develop and use more sensitive measures tailored for this population of grandparents, as well as to use instruments designed specifically for children with ASD in order to assess their functioning abilities and disabilities in various contexts. For instance, Bölte et al. (
2018) recently published the Brief ICF Core Sets for ASD based on the International Classification of Functioning, Disability and Health (ICF; World Health Organization
2001). The ICF Core Sets for ASD include categories emphasizing activities of daily living and functioning in various environmental contexts for persons with ASD encompassing both strengths and challenges associated with ASD, and their impact on participation in everyday activities. The ICF Core Sets for ASD can therefore serve as a foundation for development of more standardized and more personalized assessment tools that could help improve service provision (Bölte et al.
2018). Other well-known measures, although not ASD-specific, could also be appropriate, e.g. the ABILITIES Index (Simeonsson and Bailey
1991).
One of the subscales introduced by Goodman (
1999) into the extended version of the SDQ was the one-item burden scale. The concept of burden was defined by Platt (
1985) as “the presence of problems, difficulties or adverse events which affect the life (lives) of the psychiatric patients’ significant other(s), e.g. members of the household and/or the family” (p. 385). As part of our study, we looked at possible relationships between grandparents’ needs and grandparents’ perceived burden of grandchild’s difficulties on them or their families. Regression analyses revealed a positive association between felt burden imposed by their grandchild’s ASD and their perception of needs. This finding could be partly explained by impact that severity of grandchild’s ASD might have on family’s everyday functioning as previous empirical research on parental needs has suggested that perceptions of family needs may vary as a function of type and severity of disability (Epley et al.
2011). The finding is also congruent with earlier research demonstrating that child’s symptomatology (diagnosis combined with impairment) significantly predicted perceived burden in parents (Angold et al.
1998). However, this explanation would contradict our results from descriptive statistical analyses, demonstrating that overwhelming majority of the grandparents (91.1%) did not actually perceive their grandchild’s difficulties as a burden on them or family as a whole, whereas only nine respondents (8.9%) did so. These findings point to a complex picture of relationships between experiences of burden in family members and their perceptions of difficulties caused by child’s ASD, and therefore, requires further investigation. As Angold et al. (
1998) noted, “It would also be interesting to know what criteria parents use in deciding to attribute a difficulty of their own to a child’s behavior, and why some parents report no burden despite having a severely disturbed child” (p. 79). In this connection, most recent findings drawn from the area of stress research with families of children with ASD could be informative. For instance, a study by Sim et al. (
2018) showed no direct associations between severe family stress and child’s characteristics, such as ASD diagnosis. The authors recommended that in order to understand patterns of stress management in families of children with autism, one should study the influence of resilience factors, such as informal social support and respite care, in the context of the child’s whole family system nested in the broader social environment. Indeed, as evidence suggests, presence of resources such as self-differentiation and social support can be associated with reduced stress in grandparents irrespective of their grandchild’s disability status (Findler
2014). Among other protective factors that can contribute to resilience and personal growth in grandparents of children with disabilities are intergenerational connections and the grandparenting role with the accompanying feelings of unconditional love, joy, and gratitude (Findler
2014; Hillman et al.
2017). Grandparents’ positive feelings and satisfaction with their role are pivotal in the family system where a child with ASD is present: grandparents can be invaluable resource to parents and siblings by providing practical and emotional support (Pit-ten Cate et al.
2007; Prendeville and Kinsella
2018).
Previous research with grandparents of children with autism has reported on grandmothers’ greater involvement in grandchild care compared to grandfathers (Glasberg and Harris
1997; Hillman et al.
2016). Several authors argued that grandchild care is a gendered experience entailing roles, expectations and activities different for grandmothers and grandfathers (Di Gessa et al.
2016b). As Hoffman and Mitchell (
1998) noted, women and caregiving task are still seen as synonymous notions as historically many women have devoted their lives to this role. Although the results of the present study showed similar amount of needs for both grandmothers and grandfathers, we inquired about the influence of the gender variable on the observed relationship between grandparents’ perceived burden discussed earlier; we predicted that women would feel more burden compared to men. However, the results of the Kruskal–Wallis test showed no differences in perceptions of burden between grandmothers and grandfathers. This finding might suggest that grandparents’ roles in families with a child with autism may have changed over time and can vary in different cultural contexts. For instance, in Ireland, grandfathers’ role has been described as having the calming influence on family, especially in those families where a grandchild with ASD had significant behavioral problems (Prendeville and Kinsella
2018). In Sweden, a country characterized by a gender equality (Kolk
2014), gender differences in relation to childcare are not vivid: nowadays both mothers and fathers are equally engaged in their children’s lives (Kridahl
2017). As Kridahl (
2017) suggested, perceptions and expectations of grandmothers’ and grandfathers’ roles in the Swedish family may have disappeared over time and, therefore, may be similar to grandchild’s parents, especially given the fact that Swedish grandparents are not primary caregivers for their grandchildren. However, due to the lack of research on grandparenting a child with ASD in Sweden, unfortunately, it is not possible to draw any firm conclusions at the moment; more research is needed to obtain first-hand perspectives of both grandparents and their adult children on perceptions of roles of grandmothers and grandmothers when caring after a young child with ASD. Longitudinal research designs could help elucidate possible changes in these roles over time in Sweden as well as in other cultural contexts.
Implications for Practice
The results of our study have important implications for professionals involved into provision of early intervention programs for children with ASD and their family members. The findings can inform practitioners about grandparents’ most and least prioritized needs in various topic areas, which may help practitioners improve already existing formal support programs offered to grandparents, parents, and preschool teachers. For instance, meeting grandparents’ need of information about how to help develop grandchild’s skills can potentially assist grandparents to become more active in supporting of their grandchild. This is especially salient if the child’s parents have full-time jobs. Also, as the results of the present study suggest, grandparents expressed a need for meeting their grandchild’s teachers. It would be desirable if practitioners at disability services could inform preschool teachers about those needs and encourage them to view grandparents as potential partners when providing educational interventions. As Findler’s (
2007) study showed, teachers and special educators rarely met grandparents of children with disabilities in their practice; moreover, they expressed no interest in receiving special training on how to meet the needs of grandparents. Existing pre-service general and special educational programs could include this component in their curricula in order to raise educators’ awareness of the beneficial role of grandparents in supporting the child with ASD, his/her siblings and parents, as well as other family members in a broader family system network.
Provision of publicly funded psychoeducational workshops by disability services in Stockholm specifically designed for grandparents as part of family-centered early intervention programs for children with ASD can serve as an example of a support model tailored to the needs of extended family members. This model adds to the list of available workshop models for grandparents of children with disabilities described in the literature (e.g. Vadasy et al.
1986). However, future research needs to evaluate the effectiveness of these models (Kahana et al.
2015; Seligman and Darling
1997).
Limitations of the Study
The findings of this study should be seen in the light of its limitations. Firstly, the sample in our study was not representative in terms of socio-demographic characteristics: the majority of the grandparents held university academic degree and spoke Swedish as their mother language. Obtaining a more representative sample of grandparents from various educational, socio-economic and cultural backgrounds is important for research and practice (Hillman et al.
2017; Zakirova Engstrand et al.
2018). Involving ethnically and socio-economically diverse participants ensures external validity of research findings (Kistner and Robbins
1986; Pierce et al.
2014; West et al.
2016) and help develop culturally competent services for families of children with ASD (Stahmer et al.
2011; Zakirova Engstrand et al.
2018). Moreover, our findings may not be generalizable to other cultural settings due to differences in provision of formal supports across different countries. Other important limitations are related to methodological aspects, particularly in relation to the extended version of the SDQ, (Goodman
1999). Firstly, the SDQ was developed primarily for screening for possible mental/psychiatric disorders to get services or for prevention in a community sample (Goodman
1997,
1999). In our study, the children had already obtained the formal diagnosis of ASD and were already receiving support services, thus constituting a clinical group. Secondly, as we mentioned earlier, the parent version of the SDQ Impact supplement scale was used in the study, which resulted in a rather large number of missing data for the items eliciting answers on child’s impairment in social life. In fact, during the data collection phase, some of the grandparents told to the first author that they could not answer the survey questions as they did not actually live with their grandchild in the same house. Thirdly, the burden item included into the SDQ Impact supplement scale did not clearly delineate between perceptions of burden for each individual grandparent, and for family as a whole. For future research, it is recommended using more objective instruments to investigate the longer-term effects of grandchild disability on grandparental functioning (Hastings
1997).
Despite these limitations, the study does provide valuable information on how to improve already existing support programs for children with ASD and their family members, and facilitate better planning of future support services based on identified needs. Furthermore, as research on needs of non-custodial grandparents of children with ASD is scant, the present study contributes to literature by providing additional evidence to understand the multi-dimensional nature of issues related to grandchild care in the context of childhood disability.