Skip to main content
Top
Gepubliceerd in: Psychological Research 3/2005

01-01-2005 | Original Article

Mechanisms underlying spatial coding in a multiple-item Simon task

Auteurs: Rob H. J. Van der Lubbe, Piotr Jaśkowski, Rolf Verleger

Gepubliceerd in: Psychological Research | Uitgave 3/2005

Log in om toegang te krijgen
share
DELEN

Deel dit onderdeel of sectie (kopieer de link)

  • Optie A:
    Klik op de rechtermuisknop op de link en selecteer de optie “linkadres kopiëren”
  • Optie B:
    Deel de link per e-mail

Abstract

Choice responses are faster when target position and response side correspond than when they do not, even if target position is response-irrelevant. This “Simon effect” has also been observed in case of multi-item arrays. Generally, it is assumed that an automatically generated spatial response code is responsible for the effect. The referential-coding account assumes that this code is directly related to the target, although the moment of production of the code is not fully clear. The attention-shift account assumes that the code is directly related to the direction of the most recent attentional shift. An experiment was performed in which left or right target locations were indicated by arrows occurring before (precue), simultaneous with (simcue), or after (postcue) six-element arrays. Overt responses and EEG potentials were recorded. The Simon effect was present in all conditions, and decreased when responses were slower. No relation was found between amplitude of posterior lateralized components and the magnitude of the Simon effect. A posterior contralateral negativity was also found after presenting the arrays in the precue condition, which might reflect the reorienting of attention toward the target position. The results are more favorable to the referential-coding account although this account becomes very similar to the attention-shift hypothesis as the moment of formation of the spatial response code is related to effective target onset rather than to stimulus onset.
Voetnoten
1
The term “spatial code” refers to the neuronal representation of a location in outer space. This code may be directly related to a stimulus or a response modality, but it may also be supramodal, referring to a description of space that is independent from but linked with different stimulus and also response modalities.
 
2
This criterion of 40 µV was determined in sessions which revealed that the amplitude of the hEOG in cases of a horizontal eye movement increases about 20 µV per degree of visual angle (see also Van der Lubbe & Woestenburg, 1999; Van der Lubbe & Keuss, 2001). So by approximation, eye movements larger than 2° were rejected from further analyses.
 
3
Hommel suggested that precues may not only speed up identification but also processing and possibly decay of the spatial response code along the automatic route, which could result in a Simon effect of equal magnitude in the three cue conditions.
 
4
We thank Hartmut Leuthold for this suggestion.
 
Literatuur
go back to reference Corbetta, M., Kincade, J. M., & Shulman, G.L. (2002) Neural systems for visual orienting and their relationships to spatial working memory. Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience, 14, 508–523. Corbetta, M., Kincade, J. M., & Shulman, G.L. (2002) Neural systems for visual orienting and their relationships to spatial working memory. Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience, 14, 508–523.
go back to reference De Jong, R., Liang, C., & Lauber E. (1994). Conditional and unconditional automaticity: A dual-process model of effects of spatial stimulus-response correspondence. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 20, 731–750.PubMed De Jong, R., Liang, C., & Lauber E. (1994). Conditional and unconditional automaticity: A dual-process model of effects of spatial stimulus-response correspondence. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 20, 731–750.PubMed
go back to reference Deubel, H., & Schneider, W. X. (1996). Saccade target selection and object recognition: Evidence for a common attentional mechanism. Vision Research, 36, 1827–1837. Deubel, H., & Schneider, W. X. (1996). Saccade target selection and object recognition: Evidence for a common attentional mechanism. Vision Research, 36, 1827–1837.
go back to reference Deubel, H., Schneider, W. X., & Paprotta A (1998). Selective dorsal and ventral processing: Evidence for a common attentional mechanism in reaching and perception. Visual Cognition, 5, 81–107. Deubel, H., Schneider, W. X., & Paprotta A (1998). Selective dorsal and ventral processing: Evidence for a common attentional mechanism in reaching and perception. Visual Cognition, 5, 81–107.
go back to reference Eimer, M. (1995). Stimulus-response compatibility and automatic response activation: Evidence from psychophysiological studies. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 21, 837–854.CrossRefPubMed Eimer, M. (1995). Stimulus-response compatibility and automatic response activation: Evidence from psychophysiological studies. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 21, 837–854.CrossRefPubMed
go back to reference Eimer, M. (1996). The N2pc component as an indicator of attentional selectivity. Electroencephalography and Clinical Neurophysiology, 99, 225–234.CrossRefPubMed Eimer, M. (1996). The N2pc component as an indicator of attentional selectivity. Electroencephalography and Clinical Neurophysiology, 99, 225–234.CrossRefPubMed
go back to reference Eimer, M., Hommel, B., & Prinz, W. (1995). S-R compatibility and response selection. Acta Psychologica, 90, 301–313.CrossRef Eimer, M., Hommel, B., & Prinz, W. (1995). S-R compatibility and response selection. Acta Psychologica, 90, 301–313.CrossRef
go back to reference Harter, M. R., Miller, S. L., Price, N. J., LaLonde, M. E., & Keyes, A. L. (1989). Neural processes involved in directing attention. Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience, 1, 223–237. Harter, M. R., Miller, S. L., Price, N. J., LaLonde, M. E., & Keyes, A. L. (1989). Neural processes involved in directing attention. Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience, 1, 223–237.
go back to reference Hommel, B. (1993a). The role of attention for the Simon effect. Psychological Research, 55, 208–222. Hommel, B. (1993a). The role of attention for the Simon effect. Psychological Research, 55, 208–222.
go back to reference Hommel, B. (1993b). The relationship between stimulus processing and response selection in the Simon task: Evidence for a temporal overlap. Psychological Research, 55, 280–290. Hommel, B. (1993b). The relationship between stimulus processing and response selection in the Simon task: Evidence for a temporal overlap. Psychological Research, 55, 280–290.
go back to reference Hommel, B. (1994). Spontaneous decay of response-code activation. Psychological Research, 56, 261–268.PubMed Hommel, B. (1994). Spontaneous decay of response-code activation. Psychological Research, 56, 261–268.PubMed
go back to reference Hommel, B. (1995). Stimulus-response compatibility and the Simon effect: Toward an empirical clarification. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 21, 764–775.CrossRef Hommel, B. (1995). Stimulus-response compatibility and the Simon effect: Toward an empirical clarification. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 21, 764–775.CrossRef
go back to reference Hopf, J.-M., & Mangun, G. R. (2000). Shifting visual attention in space: An electrophysiological analysis using high spatial resolution mapping. Clinical Neurophysiology, 11, 1241–1257.CrossRef Hopf, J.-M., & Mangun, G. R. (2000). Shifting visual attention in space: An electrophysiological analysis using high spatial resolution mapping. Clinical Neurophysiology, 11, 1241–1257.CrossRef
go back to reference Ivanoff, J. (2003). On spatial response code activation in a Simon task. Acta Psychologica, 112, 157–179.CrossRefPubMed Ivanoff, J. (2003). On spatial response code activation in a Simon task. Acta Psychologica, 112, 157–179.CrossRefPubMed
go back to reference Ivanoff, J., & Peters, M. (2000). A shift of attention may be necessary, but it is not sufficient, for the generation of the Simon effect. Psychological Research, 64, 117–135.CrossRefPubMed Ivanoff, J., & Peters, M. (2000). A shift of attention may be necessary, but it is not sufficient, for the generation of the Simon effect. Psychological Research, 64, 117–135.CrossRefPubMed
go back to reference Kornblum, S., Hasbroucq, T., & Osman, A. (1990). Dimensional overlap: Cognitive basis for stimulus-response compatibility. A model and taxonomy. Psychological Review, 97, 253–270.PubMed Kornblum, S., Hasbroucq, T., & Osman, A. (1990). Dimensional overlap: Cognitive basis for stimulus-response compatibility. A model and taxonomy. Psychological Review, 97, 253–270.PubMed
go back to reference Lu, C.-H., & Proctor, R. W. (1995). The influence of irrelevant local information on performance: A review of the Simon effect and spatial Stroop effects. Psychonomic Bulletin & Review, 2, 174–207. Lu, C.-H., & Proctor, R. W. (1995). The influence of irrelevant local information on performance: A review of the Simon effect and spatial Stroop effects. Psychonomic Bulletin & Review, 2, 174–207.
go back to reference Luck, S. J., & Hillyard, S. A. (1994). Spatial filtering during visual search: Evidence from human electrophysiology. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 20, 1000–1014.CrossRefPubMed Luck, S. J., & Hillyard, S. A. (1994). Spatial filtering during visual search: Evidence from human electrophysiology. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 20, 1000–1014.CrossRefPubMed
go back to reference Nicoletti, R., & Umiltà, C. (1989). Splitting visual space with attention. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 15, 164–169.CrossRefPubMed Nicoletti, R., & Umiltà, C. (1989). Splitting visual space with attention. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 15, 164–169.CrossRefPubMed
go back to reference Nicoletti, R., & Umiltà, C. (1994). Attention shift produces spatial stimulus codes. Psychological Research, 56, 144–150.PubMed Nicoletti, R., & Umiltà, C. (1994). Attention shift produces spatial stimulus codes. Psychological Research, 56, 144–150.PubMed
go back to reference Notebaert, W., Soetens, E., & Melis, A. (2001). Sequential analysis of a Simon task: Evidence for an attention shift account. Psychological Research, 65, 170–184.CrossRefPubMed Notebaert, W., Soetens, E., & Melis, A. (2001). Sequential analysis of a Simon task: Evidence for an attention shift account. Psychological Research, 65, 170–184.CrossRefPubMed
go back to reference Pivik, R. T., Broughton, R. J., Coppola, R., Davidson, R. J., Fox, N, & Nuwer, M. R. (1993). Guidelines for the recording and quantitative analysis of electroencephalographic activity in research contexts. Psychophysiology, 30, 547–558. Pivik, R. T., Broughton, R. J., Coppola, R., Davidson, R. J., Fox, N, & Nuwer, M. R. (1993). Guidelines for the recording and quantitative analysis of electroencephalographic activity in research contexts. Psychophysiology, 30, 547–558.
go back to reference Posner, M. I. (1980). Orienting of attention. Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology, 32, 3–25.PubMed Posner, M. I. (1980). Orienting of attention. Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology, 32, 3–25.PubMed
go back to reference Praamstra, P., & Plat, F. M. (2001). Failed suppression of direct visuomotor activation in Parkinson’s disease. Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience, 13, 31–43.CrossRefPubMed Praamstra, P., & Plat, F. M. (2001). Failed suppression of direct visuomotor activation in Parkinson’s disease. Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience, 13, 31–43.CrossRefPubMed
go back to reference Proctor, R. W., & Lu, C.-H. (1994). Referential coding and attention-shifting account of the Simon effect. Psychological Research, 56, 185–195.PubMed Proctor, R. W., & Lu, C.-H. (1994). Referential coding and attention-shifting account of the Simon effect. Psychological Research, 56, 185–195.PubMed
go back to reference Rizzolatti, G., Riggio, L., Dascola I, & Umiltà, C. (1987). Reorienting attention across the horizontal and vertical meridians: Evidence in favor of a premotor theory of attention. Neuropsychologia, 25, 31–46.PubMed Rizzolatti, G., Riggio, L., Dascola I, & Umiltà, C. (1987). Reorienting attention across the horizontal and vertical meridians: Evidence in favor of a premotor theory of attention. Neuropsychologia, 25, 31–46.PubMed
go back to reference Rubichi, S., Nicoletti, R., Iani, C., & Umiltà, C. (1997). The Simon effect occurs relative to the direction of an attention shift. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 23, 1353–1364.CrossRefPubMed Rubichi, S., Nicoletti, R., Iani, C., & Umiltà, C. (1997). The Simon effect occurs relative to the direction of an attention shift. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 23, 1353–1364.CrossRefPubMed
go back to reference Simon, J. R. (1990). The effects of an irrelevant directional cue on human information processing. In: Proctor, R. W. and Reeve, T. G. (Eds.) Stimulus-response compatibility: An integrated perspective (pp. 31–86). Amsterdam: North Holland. Simon, J. R. (1990). The effects of an irrelevant directional cue on human information processing. In: Proctor, R. W. and Reeve, T. G. (Eds.) Stimulus-response compatibility: An integrated perspective (pp. 31–86). Amsterdam: North Holland.
go back to reference Simon, J. R., & Rudell, A. P. (1967). Auditory S-R compatibility: The effect of an irrelevant cue on information processing. Journal of Experimental Psychology, 51, 300–304. Simon, J. R., & Rudell, A. P. (1967). Auditory S-R compatibility: The effect of an irrelevant cue on information processing. Journal of Experimental Psychology, 51, 300–304.
go back to reference Stoffer, T. H. (1991). Attentional focussing and spatial stimulus-response compatibility. Psychological Research, 53, 127–135.PubMed Stoffer, T. H. (1991). Attentional focussing and spatial stimulus-response compatibility. Psychological Research, 53, 127–135.PubMed
go back to reference Stoffer, T. H., & Umiltà, C. (1997). Spatial stimulus coding and the focus of attention in S-R compatibility and the Simon effect. In B. Hommel & W. Prinz (Eds.), Theoretical issues in stimulus-response compatibility (pp. 181–208). Amsterdam: Elsevier. Stoffer, T. H., & Umiltà, C. (1997). Spatial stimulus coding and the focus of attention in S-R compatibility and the Simon effect. In B. Hommel & W. Prinz (Eds.), Theoretical issues in stimulus-response compatibility (pp. 181–208). Amsterdam: Elsevier.
go back to reference Stoffer, T. H., & Yakin, A. R. (1994). The functional role of attention for spatial coding in the Simon effect. Psychological Research, 56, 151–162.PubMed Stoffer, T. H., & Yakin, A. R. (1994). The functional role of attention for spatial coding in the Simon effect. Psychological Research, 56, 151–162.PubMed
go back to reference Umiltà, C., & Liotti, M. (1987). Egocentric and relative spatial codes in S-R compatibility. Psychological Research, 49, 81–90. Umiltà, C., & Liotti, M. (1987). Egocentric and relative spatial codes in S-R compatibility. Psychological Research, 49, 81–90.
go back to reference Umiltà, C., & Nicoletti, R. (1992). An integrated model of the Simon effect. In J. Alegria, D. Holender, J. Junca de Morais, & M. Radeau (Eds.), Analytic approaches to human cognition (pp. 331–350). Amsterdam: North-Holland. Umiltà, C., & Nicoletti, R. (1992). An integrated model of the Simon effect. In J. Alegria, D. Holender, J. Junca de Morais, & M. Radeau (Eds.), Analytic approaches to human cognition (pp. 331–350). Amsterdam: North-Holland.
go back to reference Van der Lubbe, R. H. J., & Keuss, P. J. G. (2001). Focused attention reduces the effect of lateral interference in multi-element arrays. Psychological Research, 65, 107–118.CrossRefPubMed Van der Lubbe, R. H. J., & Keuss, P. J. G. (2001). Focused attention reduces the effect of lateral interference in multi-element arrays. Psychological Research, 65, 107–118.CrossRefPubMed
go back to reference Van der Lubbe, R. H. J., & Verleger, R. (2002). Aging and the Simon task. Psychophysiology, 39, 100–110.CrossRefPubMed Van der Lubbe, R. H. J., & Verleger, R. (2002). Aging and the Simon task. Psychophysiology, 39, 100–110.CrossRefPubMed
go back to reference Van der Lubbe, R. H. J., & Woestenburg, J. C. (1999). The influence of peripheral cues on the tendency to react towards a lateral relevant stimulus with multiple-item arrays. Biological Psychology, 51, 1–21.CrossRefPubMed Van der Lubbe, R. H. J., & Woestenburg, J. C. (1999). The influence of peripheral cues on the tendency to react towards a lateral relevant stimulus with multiple-item arrays. Biological Psychology, 51, 1–21.CrossRefPubMed
go back to reference Van der Lubbe, R. H. J., Keuss, P. J. G., & Stoffels, E. (1996). Threefold effect of peripheral precues: Alertness, orienting, and response tendencies. Acta Psychologica, 94, 319–337.CrossRefPubMed Van der Lubbe, R. H. J., Keuss, P. J. G., & Stoffels, E. (1996). Threefold effect of peripheral precues: Alertness, orienting, and response tendencies. Acta Psychologica, 94, 319–337.CrossRefPubMed
go back to reference Van der Lubbe, R. H. J., Wauschkuhn, B., Wascher, E., Niehoff, T., Kömpf D, & Verleger, R. (2000). Lateralized EEG components with direction information for the preparation of saccades and finger movements. Experimental Brain Research, 132, 163–178.CrossRef Van der Lubbe, R. H. J., Wauschkuhn, B., Wascher, E., Niehoff, T., Kömpf D, & Verleger, R. (2000). Lateralized EEG components with direction information for the preparation of saccades and finger movements. Experimental Brain Research, 132, 163–178.CrossRef
go back to reference Van der Lubbe, R.H.J., Jaśkowski, P., Wauschkuhn, B., & Verleger, R. (2001). Influence of time pressure in a simple response task, a choice-by-location task, and the Simon task. Journal of Psychophysiology, 15, 241–255.CrossRef Van der Lubbe, R.H.J., Jaśkowski, P., Wauschkuhn, B., & Verleger, R. (2001). Influence of time pressure in a simple response task, a choice-by-location task, and the Simon task. Journal of Psychophysiology, 15, 241–255.CrossRef
go back to reference Van Velzen, J., & Eimer, M. (2003). Early posterior ERP components do not reflect the control of attentional shifts towards expected peripheral events. Psychophysiology, 40, 827–831.CrossRefPubMed Van Velzen, J., & Eimer, M. (2003). Early posterior ERP components do not reflect the control of attentional shifts towards expected peripheral events. Psychophysiology, 40, 827–831.CrossRefPubMed
go back to reference Verleger, R., Gasser, T., & Möcks, J. (1982). Correction for EOG artifacts in event related potentials of the EEG: Aspects of reliability and validity. Psychophysiology, 19, 472–480.PubMed Verleger, R., Gasser, T., & Möcks, J. (1982). Correction for EOG artifacts in event related potentials of the EEG: Aspects of reliability and validity. Psychophysiology, 19, 472–480.PubMed
go back to reference Verleger, R., Vollmer, C., Wauschkuhn, B., Van der Lubbe, R. H. J., & Wascher, E. (2000). Dimensional overlap between arrows as cueing stimuli and responses? Evidence from contra-ipsilateral differences in EEG potentials. Cognitive Brain Research, 10, 99–109.CrossRefPubMed Verleger, R., Vollmer, C., Wauschkuhn, B., Van der Lubbe, R. H. J., & Wascher, E. (2000). Dimensional overlap between arrows as cueing stimuli and responses? Evidence from contra-ipsilateral differences in EEG potentials. Cognitive Brain Research, 10, 99–109.CrossRefPubMed
go back to reference Wallace, R. J. (1971). S-R compatibility and the idea of a response code. Journal of Experimental Psychology, 88, 354–360.PubMed Wallace, R. J. (1971). S-R compatibility and the idea of a response code. Journal of Experimental Psychology, 88, 354–360.PubMed
go back to reference Wascher, E., & Wauschkuhn, B. (1996). The interaction of stimulus- and response-related processing measured by event-related lateralizations of the EEG. Electroencephalography and Clinical Neurophysiology, 99, 149–162.CrossRefPubMed Wascher, E., & Wauschkuhn, B. (1996). The interaction of stimulus- and response-related processing measured by event-related lateralizations of the EEG. Electroencephalography and Clinical Neurophysiology, 99, 149–162.CrossRefPubMed
go back to reference Wascher, E., & Wolber, M. (2004). Attentional and intentional cueing in a Simon task: An EEG-based approach. Psychological Research, 68, 18–30 CrossRefPubMed Wascher, E., & Wolber, M. (2004). Attentional and intentional cueing in a Simon task: An EEG-based approach. Psychological Research, 68, 18–30 CrossRefPubMed
go back to reference Wascher, E., Schatz, U., Kuder, T., & Verleger, R. (2001). Validity and boundary conditions of automatic response activation in the Simon task. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 27, 731–751.PubMed Wascher, E., Schatz, U., Kuder, T., & Verleger, R. (2001). Validity and boundary conditions of automatic response activation in the Simon task. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 27, 731–751.PubMed
go back to reference Wauschkuhn, B., Wascher, E., & Verleger, R. (1997). Lateralised cortical activity due to preparation of saccades and finger movements: a comparative study. Electroencephalography and Clinical Neurophysiology, 102, 114–124.CrossRefPubMed Wauschkuhn, B., Wascher, E., & Verleger, R. (1997). Lateralised cortical activity due to preparation of saccades and finger movements: a comparative study. Electroencephalography and Clinical Neurophysiology, 102, 114–124.CrossRefPubMed
go back to reference Yamaguchi, S., Tsuchiya, H., & Kobayashi, S. (1994). Electroencephalographic activity associated with shifts of visuospatial attention. Brain, 117, 553–562.PubMed Yamaguchi, S., Tsuchiya, H., & Kobayashi, S. (1994). Electroencephalographic activity associated with shifts of visuospatial attention. Brain, 117, 553–562.PubMed
go back to reference Yamaguchi, S., Tsuchiya, H., & Kobayashi, S. (1995). Electrophysiologic correlates of age effects on visuospatial attention shift. Cognitive Brain Research, 3, 41–49.CrossRefPubMed Yamaguchi, S., Tsuchiya, H., & Kobayashi, S. (1995). Electrophysiologic correlates of age effects on visuospatial attention shift. Cognitive Brain Research, 3, 41–49.CrossRefPubMed
go back to reference Zhang, J., & Kornblum, S. (1997). Distributional analysis and De Jong, Liang, and Lauber’s (1994) dual-process model of the Simon effect. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 23, 1543–1551.CrossRef Zhang, J., & Kornblum, S. (1997). Distributional analysis and De Jong, Liang, and Lauber’s (1994) dual-process model of the Simon effect. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 23, 1543–1551.CrossRef
go back to reference Zimba, L., & Brito, C. F. (1995). Attention precueing and the Simon effect: A test of the attention-coding account of the Simon effect. Psychological Research, 58, 102–118. Zimba, L., & Brito, C. F. (1995). Attention precueing and the Simon effect: A test of the attention-coding account of the Simon effect. Psychological Research, 58, 102–118.
Metagegevens
Titel
Mechanisms underlying spatial coding in a multiple-item Simon task
Auteurs
Rob H. J. Van der Lubbe
Piotr Jaśkowski
Rolf Verleger
Publicatiedatum
01-01-2005
Uitgeverij
Springer-Verlag
Gepubliceerd in
Psychological Research / Uitgave 3/2005
Print ISSN: 0340-0727
Elektronisch ISSN: 1430-2772
DOI
https://doi.org/10.1007/s00426-004-0176-7

Andere artikelen Uitgave 3/2005

Psychological Research 3/2005 Naar de uitgave