Skip to main content
Top
Gepubliceerd in: Psychological Research 3/2005

01-01-2005 | Original Article

Hierarchical number estimation

Auteurs: Jay Friedenberg, William Limratana

Gepubliceerd in: Psychological Research | Uitgave 3/2005

Log in om toegang te krijgen
share
DELEN

Deel dit onderdeel of sectie (kopieer de link)

  • Optie A:
    Klik op de rechtermuisknop op de link en selecteer de optie “linkadres kopiëren”
  • Optie B:
    Deel de link per e-mail

Abstract

We investigated number estimation using dot patterns grouped by proximity into larger clusters. Participants estimated the number of dots and clusters in separate trials. Estimation was most accurate when the numbers of elements on both scales were the same. When the number of elements on the unattended scale was higher, overestimation occurred. Conversely, when the number of elements on the unattended scale was lower, underestimation occurred. In Experiment 2, response cues were blocked to reduce any tendency toward attending the irrelevant level. The results were essentially unchanged, indicating response confusion alone cannot account for the effect. The data support the existence of an opposite scale effect in which the number of elements at the unattended level influence the processing of number.
Literatuur
go back to reference Allik, J., & Tuulmets, T. (1991). Occupancy model of perceived numerosity. Perception & Psychophysics, 49, 303–314. Allik, J., & Tuulmets, T. (1991). Occupancy model of perceived numerosity. Perception & Psychophysics, 49, 303–314.
go back to reference Atkinson, J., Francis, M. R., & Campbell, F. W. (1976). The dependence of the visual numerosity limit on orientation, colour, and grouping in the stimulus. Perception, 5, 335–342.PubMed Atkinson, J., Francis, M. R., & Campbell, F. W. (1976). The dependence of the visual numerosity limit on orientation, colour, and grouping in the stimulus. Perception, 5, 335–342.PubMed
go back to reference Briand, K. A. (1993). Efficient filtering of irrelevant global and local information when target level and location are random. Psychological Research, 55, 264–269. Briand, K. A. (1993). Efficient filtering of irrelevant global and local information when target level and location are random. Psychological Research, 55, 264–269.
go back to reference Brown, T. L., Gore, C. L., Carr, Thomas H. (2002). Visual attention and word recognition in Stroop color naming: Is word recognition “automatic?” Journal of Experimental Psychology: General, 131, 220–240.CrossRef Brown, T. L., Gore, C. L., Carr, Thomas H. (2002). Visual attention and word recognition in Stroop color naming: Is word recognition “automatic?” Journal of Experimental Psychology: General, 131, 220–240.CrossRef
go back to reference Dehaene, S., & Cohen, L. (1994). Dissociable mechanisms of subitizing and counting: Neuropsychological evidence from simultanagnosic patients. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 20, 958–975.CrossRefPubMed Dehaene, S., & Cohen, L. (1994). Dissociable mechanisms of subitizing and counting: Neuropsychological evidence from simultanagnosic patients. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 20, 958–975.CrossRefPubMed
go back to reference Durgin, F. H. (1995). Texture density adaptation and the perceived numerosity and distribution of texture. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 21, 149–169.CrossRef Durgin, F. H. (1995). Texture density adaptation and the perceived numerosity and distribution of texture. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 21, 149–169.CrossRef
go back to reference Eriksen, C. W., & St. James, J. D. (1986). Visual attention within and around the field of focal attention: A zoom lens model. Perception & Psychophysics, 40, 225–240. Eriksen, C. W., & St. James, J. D. (1986). Visual attention within and around the field of focal attention: A zoom lens model. Perception & Psychophysics, 40, 225–240.
go back to reference Gallistel, C. R., & Gelman, R. (1992). Preverbal and verbal counting and computation. Cognition, 44, 43–74.CrossRefPubMed Gallistel, C. R., & Gelman, R. (1992). Preverbal and verbal counting and computation. Cognition, 44, 43–74.CrossRefPubMed
go back to reference Ginsburg, N. (1991). Numerosity estimation as a function of stimulus organization. Perception, 20, 681–686.PubMed Ginsburg, N. (1991). Numerosity estimation as a function of stimulus organization. Perception, 20, 681–686.PubMed
go back to reference Ginsburg, N., & Nicholls, A. (1988). Perceived numerosity as a function of item size. Perceptual and Motor Skills, 67, 656–658.PubMed Ginsburg, N., & Nicholls, A. (1988). Perceived numerosity as a function of item size. Perceptual and Motor Skills, 67, 656–658.PubMed
go back to reference Helson, H. (1964). Adaptation-level theory. An experimental and systematic approach to behavior. New York: Harper & Row. Helson, H. (1964). Adaptation-level theory. An experimental and systematic approach to behavior. New York: Harper & Row.
go back to reference Intriligator, J., & Cavanagh, P. (2001). The spatial resolution of visual attention. Cognitive Psychology, 43, 171–216.CrossRefPubMed Intriligator, J., & Cavanagh, P. (2001). The spatial resolution of visual attention. Cognitive Psychology, 43, 171–216.CrossRefPubMed
go back to reference Kaufman, E., Lord, M., Reese, T., & Volkmann, J. (1949). The discrimination of visual number. American Journal of Psychology, 62, 498–525. Kaufman, E., Lord, M., Reese, T., & Volkmann, J. (1949). The discrimination of visual number. American Journal of Psychology, 62, 498–525.
go back to reference Kimchi, R. (1992). Primacy of wholistic processing and global/local paradigm: A critical review. Psychological Bulletin, 112, 24–38.CrossRefPubMed Kimchi, R. (1992). Primacy of wholistic processing and global/local paradigm: A critical review. Psychological Bulletin, 112, 24–38.CrossRefPubMed
go back to reference Navon, D. (1977). Forest before trees: The precedence of global features in visual perception. Cognitive Psychology, 9, 353–383. Navon, D. (1977). Forest before trees: The precedence of global features in visual perception. Cognitive Psychology, 9, 353–383.
go back to reference Oyama, T. (1986). The effect of stimulus organization on numerosity discrimination. Japanese Psychological Research, 28, 77–86. Oyama, T. (1986). The effect of stimulus organization on numerosity discrimination. Japanese Psychological Research, 28, 77–86.
go back to reference Paquet, L., & Merikle, P. M. (1988). Global precedence in attended and unattended objects. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 14, 89–100.CrossRefPubMed Paquet, L., & Merikle, P. M. (1988). Global precedence in attended and unattended objects. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 14, 89–100.CrossRefPubMed
go back to reference Paquet, P. (1992). Global and local processing in unattended objects: A failure to induce local processing dominance. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 18, 512–529.CrossRefPubMed Paquet, P. (1992). Global and local processing in unattended objects: A failure to induce local processing dominance. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 18, 512–529.CrossRefPubMed
go back to reference Sawyer, T. F., & Wesensten, N. J. (1994). Anchoring effects on judgement, estimation, and discrimination of numerosity. Perceptual and Motor Skills, 78, 91–98.PubMed Sawyer, T. F., & Wesensten, N. J. (1994). Anchoring effects on judgement, estimation, and discrimination of numerosity. Perceptual and Motor Skills, 78, 91–98.PubMed
go back to reference Stoffer, T. H. (1993). The time course of attentional zooming: A comparison of voluntary and involuntary allocation of attention to the levels of compound stimuli. Psychological Research, 56, 14–25.PubMed Stoffer, T. H. (1993). The time course of attentional zooming: A comparison of voluntary and involuntary allocation of attention to the levels of compound stimuli. Psychological Research, 56, 14–25.PubMed
go back to reference Trick, L. M., & Enns, J. T. (1997). Clusters precede shapes in perceptual organization. Psychological Science, 8, 124–129. Trick, L. M., & Enns, J. T. (1997). Clusters precede shapes in perceptual organization. Psychological Science, 8, 124–129.
go back to reference Trick, L. M., & Pylyshyn, Z. W. (1993). What enumeration studies can show us about spatial attention: Evidence for limited capacity preattentive processing. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 19, 331–351.CrossRefPubMed Trick, L. M., & Pylyshyn, Z. W. (1993). What enumeration studies can show us about spatial attention: Evidence for limited capacity preattentive processing. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 19, 331–351.CrossRefPubMed
go back to reference Van Oeffelen, M. P., & Vos, P. G. (1982). Configural effects on the enumeration of dots: Counting by groups. Memory and Cognition, 10, 396–404. Van Oeffelen, M. P., & Vos, P. G. (1982). Configural effects on the enumeration of dots: Counting by groups. Memory and Cognition, 10, 396–404.
go back to reference Wandell, B. A. (1995). Foundations of vision. Sunderland, MA: Sinauer. Wandell, B. A. (1995). Foundations of vision. Sunderland, MA: Sinauer.
Metagegevens
Titel
Hierarchical number estimation
Auteurs
Jay Friedenberg
William Limratana
Publicatiedatum
01-01-2005
Uitgeverij
Springer-Verlag
Gepubliceerd in
Psychological Research / Uitgave 3/2005
Print ISSN: 0340-0727
Elektronisch ISSN: 1430-2772
DOI
https://doi.org/10.1007/s00426-003-0169-y

Andere artikelen Uitgave 3/2005

Psychological Research 3/2005 Naar de uitgave