Skip to main content
Top
Gepubliceerd in: Psychological Research 3/2005

01-01-2005 | Original Article

Right-left prevalence with task-irrelevant spatial codes

Auteurs: Sandro Rubichi, Roberto Nicoletti, Carlo Umiltà

Gepubliceerd in: Psychological Research | Uitgave 3/2005

Log in om toegang te krijgen
share
DELEN

Deel dit onderdeel of sectie (kopieer de link)

  • Optie A:
    Klik op de rechtermuisknop op de link en selecteer de optie “linkadres kopiëren”
  • Optie B:
    Deel de link per e-mail

Abstract

The present work investigated the right-left prevalence effect caused by the automatic activation of horizontal and vertical spatial codes in a task (Simon task) in which spatial information is task-irrelevant. Experiment 1 showed a horizontal Simon effect and a vertical Simon effect with a two-dimensional stimulus-response set. In Experiments 2 and 3, the right-left prevalence was obtained in two-dimensional Simon tasks with two contralateral effectors and four effectors respectively. Experiment 4 showed that horizontal coding is based on multiple spatial codes, whereas only one spatial code was formed for vertical coding. On the whole, these results support the notion that the right-left prevalence effect is a general phenomenon affecting spatial coding, and suggest that the horizontal dimension is prevalent because it is based on multiple spatial codes.
Voetnoten
1
Hereafter, we will also use the terms compatible and incompatible trials for the Simon task.
 
2
When the present paper was about to be submitted, the paper by Proctor et al. (2003) came to our attention as soon as it was submitted. In it the authors investigated right-left prevalence by the use of the two-dimensional Simon task. Simon effects were evident for both the horizontal and vertical dimensions, and they were of similar magnitude. When salience of one dimension was manipulated, the Simon effect was greater for the more salient dimension. These results of course were in accordance with the relative salience explanation of the right-left prevalence effect and allow this explanation to be extended to conditions in which the spatial codes are formed automatically.
 
3
Part of the data of the present work were collected when Sandro Rubichi was at the University of Urbino.
 
4
If we consider that using contralateral hand and foot responses is a way of enhancing the environmental salience of the horizontal dimension, a test of the right-left prevalence in Simon tasks would be provided by an experiment along the lines of Vu and Proctor’s (2001) Experiment 2 with stimulus location as the irrelevant stimulus dimension. This test has been conducted by Proctor et al. (2003). More precisely, the authors evaluated HE and VE with contralateral and ipsilateral hand-foot responses when the display salience was biased towards the horizontal or the vertical dimensions. Results showed that when both the display and the response salience favored one spatial dimension, the Simon effect was larger for that dimension. However, when the display and response salience did not match, the overall Simon effect favored the horizontal dimension. This asymmetry reflects the fact that display and response salience only influenced the Simon effect for the vertical dimension, whereas the horizontal Simon effect was not. Of course, this pattern of result can be interpreted as indicating right-left prevalence effect that is partially independent of environmental salience.
 
5
When the present paper was about to be submitted, the paper by Proctor et al. (2003) came to our attention as soon as it was submitted. In it the authors investigated right-left prevalence by the use of the two-dimensional Simon task. Simon effects were evident for both the horizontal and vertical dimensions, and they were of similar magnitude. When salience of one dimension was manipulated, the Simon effect was greater for the more salient dimension. These results of course were in accordance with the relative salience explanation of the right-left prevalence effect and allow this explanation to be extended to conditions in which the spatial codes are formed automatically.
 
Literatuur
go back to reference De Jong, R., Liang, C.-C., & Lauber, E. (1994). Conditional and unconditional automaticity: A dual-process model of effects of spatial stimulus-response correspondence. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 20, 731–750.PubMed De Jong, R., Liang, C.-C., & Lauber, E. (1994). Conditional and unconditional automaticity: A dual-process model of effects of spatial stimulus-response correspondence. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 20, 731–750.PubMed
go back to reference Fitts, P. M., & Seeger, C. M. (1953). S-R compatibility: Spatial characteristics of stimulus and response codes. Journal of Experimental Psychology, 46, 199–210.PubMed Fitts, P. M., & Seeger, C. M. (1953). S-R compatibility: Spatial characteristics of stimulus and response codes. Journal of Experimental Psychology, 46, 199–210.PubMed
go back to reference Hedge, A., & Marsh, N. W. A. (1975). The effect of irrelevant spatial correspondences on two-choice response-time. Acta Psychologica, 39, 427–439.PubMed Hedge, A., & Marsh, N. W. A. (1975). The effect of irrelevant spatial correspondences on two-choice response-time. Acta Psychologica, 39, 427–439.PubMed
go back to reference Heuer H. (1990). Rapid responses with the left or right hand: Response-response compatibility effects due to intermanual interactions. In R. W. Proctor & T. G. Reeve (Eds.), Stimulus–response compatibility. An integrated perspective (pp. 311–342). Amsterdam: North-Holland Heuer H. (1990). Rapid responses with the left or right hand: Response-response compatibility effects due to intermanual interactions. In R. W. Proctor & T. G. Reeve (Eds.), Stimulus–response compatibility. An integrated perspective (pp. 311–342). Amsterdam: North-Holland
go back to reference Heuer, H. (1993). Structural constraints on bimanual movements. Psychological Research, 55, 83–98.PubMed Heuer, H. (1993). Structural constraints on bimanual movements. Psychological Research, 55, 83–98.PubMed
go back to reference Hommel, B. (1994). Spontaneous decay of response-code activation. Psychological Research, 56, 261–268.PubMed Hommel, B. (1994). Spontaneous decay of response-code activation. Psychological Research, 56, 261–268.PubMed
go back to reference Hommel, B. (1996). No prevalence of right-left over top-bottom spatial codes. Perception & Psychophysics, 58, 102–110.PubMed Hommel, B. (1996). No prevalence of right-left over top-bottom spatial codes. Perception & Psychophysics, 58, 102–110.PubMed
go back to reference Hommel, B., & Prinz, W. (Eds.), (1997). Theoretical issues in stimulus-response compatibility. Amsterdam: North-Holland. Hommel, B., & Prinz, W. (Eds.), (1997). Theoretical issues in stimulus-response compatibility. Amsterdam: North-Holland.
go back to reference Kornblum, S., Hasbroucq, T., & Osman, A. (1990). Dimensional overlap: Cognitive basis for stimulus-response compatibility—A model and taxonomy. Psychological Review, 97, 253–270.PubMed Kornblum, S., Hasbroucq, T., & Osman, A. (1990). Dimensional overlap: Cognitive basis for stimulus-response compatibility—A model and taxonomy. Psychological Review, 97, 253–270.PubMed
go back to reference Lamberts, K., Tavernier, G., & d’Ydewalle, G. (1992). Effects of multiple reference points in spatial stimulus-response compatibility. Acta Psychologica, 79, 115–130.CrossRefPubMed Lamberts, K., Tavernier, G., & d’Ydewalle, G. (1992). Effects of multiple reference points in spatial stimulus-response compatibility. Acta Psychologica, 79, 115–130.CrossRefPubMed
go back to reference Lu, C.-H., & Proctor, R. W. (1995). The influence of irrelevant location information on performance: A review of the Simon and spatial Stroop effects. Psychonomic Bulletin & Review, 2, 174–207. Lu, C.-H., & Proctor, R. W. (1995). The influence of irrelevant location information on performance: A review of the Simon and spatial Stroop effects. Psychonomic Bulletin & Review, 2, 174–207.
go back to reference Nicoletti, R., & Umiltà, C. (1984). Right-left prevalence in spatial compatibility. Perception & Psychophysics, 35, 333–343. Nicoletti, R., & Umiltà, C. (1984). Right-left prevalence in spatial compatibility. Perception & Psychophysics, 35, 333–343.
go back to reference Nicoletti, R., & Umiltà, C. (1985). Responding with hand and foot: The right-left prevalence in spatial compatibility is still present. Perception & Psychophysics, 38, 211–216. Nicoletti, R., & Umiltà, C. (1985). Responding with hand and foot: The right-left prevalence in spatial compatibility is still present. Perception & Psychophysics, 38, 211–216.
go back to reference Nicoletti, R., Umiltà, C., Tressoldi, E. P., & Marzi, C. A. (1988). Why are left-right spatial codes easier to form than above-below ones? Perception & Psychophysics, 43, 287–292. Nicoletti, R., Umiltà, C., Tressoldi, E. P., & Marzi, C. A. (1988). Why are left-right spatial codes easier to form than above-below ones? Perception & Psychophysics, 43, 287–292.
go back to reference Proctor, R. W., & Reeve, T. G. (1985). Compatibility effects in the assignments of symbolic stimuli to discrete finger response. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 11, 623–639.CrossRef Proctor, R. W., & Reeve, T. G. (1985). Compatibility effects in the assignments of symbolic stimuli to discrete finger response. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 11, 623–639.CrossRef
go back to reference Proctor, R. W., & Reeve, T. G. (Eds.), (1990). Stimulus-response compatibility: An integrated perspective. Amsterdam: North-Holland. Proctor, R. W., & Reeve, T. G. (Eds.), (1990). Stimulus-response compatibility: An integrated perspective. Amsterdam: North-Holland.
go back to reference Proctor, R. W., Vu, K. P., & Nicoletti, R. (2003). The Simon effect for two-dimensional spatial stimulus-response sets. Perception & Psychophysics, 65, 1318–1329. Proctor, R. W., Vu, K. P., & Nicoletti, R. (2003). The Simon effect for two-dimensional spatial stimulus-response sets. Perception & Psychophysics, 65, 1318–1329.
go back to reference Roswarski, T. E., & Proctor, R. W. (1996). Multiple spatial codes and temporal overlap in choice-reaction tasks. Psychological Research, 59, 196–211. Roswarski, T. E., & Proctor, R. W. (1996). Multiple spatial codes and temporal overlap in choice-reaction tasks. Psychological Research, 59, 196–211.
go back to reference Rubichi, S., Pelosi, A., Nicoletti, R., & Umiltà, C. (in press). Right-left prevalence effect with horizontal and vertical effectors. Perception & Psychophysics Rubichi, S., Pelosi, A., Nicoletti, R., & Umiltà, C. (in press). Right-left prevalence effect with horizontal and vertical effectors. Perception & Psychophysics
go back to reference Simon, J. R, & Rudell, A. P. (1967). Auditory S-R compatibility: The effect of an irrelevant cue on information processing. Journal of Applied Psychology, 51, 300–304.PubMed Simon, J. R, & Rudell, A. P. (1967). Auditory S-R compatibility: The effect of an irrelevant cue on information processing. Journal of Applied Psychology, 51, 300–304.PubMed
go back to reference Tagliabue, M., Zorzi, M., Umiltà, C., & Bassignani, F. (2000). The role of long-term-memory and short-term-memory links in the Simon effect. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception & Performance, 26, 648–670.CrossRefPubMed Tagliabue, M., Zorzi, M., Umiltà, C., & Bassignani, F. (2000). The role of long-term-memory and short-term-memory links in the Simon effect. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception & Performance, 26, 648–670.CrossRefPubMed
go back to reference Umiltà, C., & Liotti, M. (1987). Egocentric and relative spatial codes in S-R compatibility. Psychological Research, 49, 81–90. Umiltà, C., & Liotti, M. (1987). Egocentric and relative spatial codes in S-R compatibility. Psychological Research, 49, 81–90.
go back to reference Vu, K. P., & Proctor, R. W. (2001). Determinants of the right-left and top-bottom prevalence for two-dimensional spatial compatibility. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 27, 813–828.CrossRefPubMed Vu, K. P., & Proctor, R. W. (2001). Determinants of the right-left and top-bottom prevalence for two-dimensional spatial compatibility. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 27, 813–828.CrossRefPubMed
go back to reference Vu, K. P., & Proctor, R. W. (2002). The prevalence effect in two-dimensional stimulus-response compatibility is a function of the relative salience of dimensions. Perception & Psychophysics, 64, 815–828. Vu, K. P., & Proctor, R. W. (2002). The prevalence effect in two-dimensional stimulus-response compatibility is a function of the relative salience of dimensions. Perception & Psychophysics, 64, 815–828.
go back to reference Vu, K. P., Proctor, R. W., & Pick, D. F. (2000). Vertical versus horizontal spatial compatibility: right-left prevalence with bimanual responses. Psychological Research, 64, 25–40.CrossRefPubMed Vu, K. P., Proctor, R. W., & Pick, D. F. (2000). Vertical versus horizontal spatial compatibility: right-left prevalence with bimanual responses. Psychological Research, 64, 25–40.CrossRefPubMed
Metagegevens
Titel
Right-left prevalence with task-irrelevant spatial codes
Auteurs
Sandro Rubichi
Roberto Nicoletti
Carlo Umiltà
Publicatiedatum
01-01-2005
Uitgeverij
Springer-Verlag
Gepubliceerd in
Psychological Research / Uitgave 3/2005
Print ISSN: 0340-0727
Elektronisch ISSN: 1430-2772
DOI
https://doi.org/10.1007/s00426-003-0168-z

Andere artikelen Uitgave 3/2005

Psychological Research 3/2005 Naar de uitgave