Elsevier

Cognitive Brain Research

Volume 3, Issue 1, December 1995, Pages 41-49
Cognitive Brain Research

Research report
Electrophysiologic correlates of age effects on visuospatial attention shift

https://doi.org/10.1016/0926-6410(95)00017-8Get rights and content

Abstract

The effects of age on the neural processes underlying shifts in visuospatial attention were studied by non-invasive recording of brain electrical activity. Event-related evoked potentials (ERPs) were recorded in young (age 28–36 years old) and elderly (59–74 years old) subjects during a target detection task. Targets were preceded by central or peripheral informative cues, which are hypothesized to provoke voluntary and reflexive shifts of spatial attention, respectively. There were no age-related differences in the effects of cue validity on reaction times. Elderly subjects had longer reaction times to both the valid and invalid targets. The ERP recordings demonstrated that attention shift-related negativities (ARNs) were generated over the hemisphere contralateral to the central cue direction with the same amplitude in young and elderly subjects. However, the onset latency was delayed for the elderly at anterior scalp sites. Peripheral cues enhanced the N1 component (140–200 ms post-cue) over the contralateral hemisphere comparably in the two age groups. The ARNs following N1 enhancement demonstrated delayed onset and restricted scalp distributions in the elderly group compared with the young group. These results suggest that resistance of cue validity effects to aging is consistent with existence of the N1 enhancement and ARNs, even in elderly subjects. Different age effects on ERPs generated by central and peripheral cues support the notion that voluntary and reflexive mechanisms of attention shift involve distinct neural systems with different vulnerability to aging.

References (41)

  • M.L. Cheal et al.

    Central and peripheral precuing of forced-choice discrimination

    Quart. J. Exp. Psychol.

    (1991)
  • M. Corbetta et al.

    A PET study of visuospatial attention

    J. Neurosci.

    (1993)
  • F.I.M. Craik et al.

    Aging and cognitive deficits: the role of attentional resources

  • C.W. Ericksen et al.

    Temporal and spatial characteristics of selective encoding from visual displays

    Percept. Psychophys.

    (1972)
  • C.W. Eriksen et al.

    Movement of attentional focus across the visual field: a critical look at the evidence

    Percept. Psychophys.

    (1987)
  • C.W. Eriksen et al.

    Allocation of attention in the visual field

    J. Exp. Psychol. Hum. Percept. Perform.

    (1985)
  • C.L. Folk et al.

    Aging and shifts of visual spatial attention

    Psychol. Aging

    (1992)
  • P. Goldman-Rakic

    Topography of cognition: parallel distributed networks in primate association cortex

    Anna. Rev. Neurosci.

    (1988)
  • M.R. Harter et al.

    Neural processes involved in directing attention

    J. Cogn. Neurosci.

    (1989)
  • A.A. Hartley et al.

    Age differences and similarities in the effects of cues and prompts

    J. Exp. Psychol. Hum. Percept. Perform.

    (1990)
  • Cited by (52)

    • An open-label, single-arm pilot study of tocotrienols supplementation on improving memory and attention in healthy young adults

      2022, Journal of Functional Foods
      Citation Excerpt :

      With these findings, the relationship of alpha activity to performance on a sustained attention task has been established. Aging leads to the degeneration of the sensory and motor cortex, resulting in the decline in cognitive functions such as alertness and attention (Müller-Oehring et al., 2013), stimulus perception (Anstey et al., 2001; Glass, 2007), discrimination (Madden & Allen, 1995; Schroeder et al., 1995; Yamaguchi et al., 1995) as well as response selection (Rabbitt et al., 2007; van der Lubbe & Verleger, 2002). As one age, there is an accumulation of impaired and aggregated proteins resulting from oxidative stress (OS) in the cytoplasm and mitochondria of the neuronal cells (Castelli et al., 2019).

    • How microsaccades relate to lateralized ERP components of spatial attention: A co-registration study

      2017, Neuropsychologia
      Citation Excerpt :

      EDAN and LDAP were both measured at a bilateral occipital electrode pair (PO9/PO10) and quantified as the mean voltage difference between the ipsilateral and the contralateral electrode in the interval from 240 to 350 ms (for EDAN) and 500–700 ms (for LDAP) after the cue. Because ADAN was previously observed over frontal and central sites (e.g., Van Velzen et al., 2006; Yamaguchi et al., 1995), this component was calculated as the mean voltage difference between contralateral and ipsilateral sites at a frontal ROI (F3/F4, F7/F8, and FC5/FC6) and at a central ROI (C3/C4 and T7/T8) in the interval 340–410 ms after cue onset. Lateralized cue-ERPs were submitted to LMMs and tested for the effect of cued attention shifts (electrode site contralateral vs. ipsilateral to cue direction).

    View all citing articles on Scopus
    View full text