Skip to main content
Top
Gepubliceerd in: Psychological Research 3/2005

01-01-2005 | Original Article

Selection and consolidation of objects and actions

Auteurs: Bernhard Hommel, Christian F. Doeller

Gepubliceerd in: Psychological Research | Uitgave 3/2005

Log in om toegang te krijgen
share
DELEN

Deel dit onderdeel of sectie (kopieer de link)

  • Optie A:
    Klik op de rechtermuisknop op de link en selecteer de optie “linkadres kopiëren”
  • Optie B:
    Deel de link per e-mail

Abstract

Response selection bottleneck models attribute performance costs under dual-task conditions to the human inability to select more than one response at a time. Consistent with this claim Pashler (1991) found that carrying out a speeded manual choice reaction time (RT) task does not impair the unspeeded report of a cued visual target from a masked display. In contrast, Jolicoeur and Dell’Acqua (1999, Experiment 2) observed pronounced interference between a speeded manual choice RT task and the unspeeded report of a small number of visually presented letters, a finding they attributed to resource sharing between response selection and stimulus consolidation. We demonstrate that comparable costs are obtained with the same task combination used by Pashler (1991) if only task order is reversed—a manipulation that is likely to increase the necessity of consolidating the target stimulus into working memory. We also found that these costs are not diminished if the location of the target to be reported is cued in advance (reducing demands on spatial focusing) and that they do not vary with the number of target features to be reported. These findings support a consolidation account of costs in dual-task performance.
Literatuur
go back to reference Akyürek, E. G., & Hommel, B. (2004). Short-term memory and the Attentional Blink: Capacity versus content. Manuscript submitted for publication. Akyürek, E. G., & Hommel, B. (2004). Short-term memory and the Attentional Blink: Capacity versus content. Manuscript submitted for publication.
go back to reference Baddeley, A., Thomson, N., & Buchanan, M. (1975). Word length and the structure of short-term memory. Journal of Verbal Learning and Verbal Behavior, 14, 575–589. Baddeley, A., Thomson, N., & Buchanan, M. (1975). Word length and the structure of short-term memory. Journal of Verbal Learning and Verbal Behavior, 14, 575–589.
go back to reference Egeth, H. (1977). Attention and preattention. In G. H. Bower (Ed.), The psychology of learning and motivation (pp. 277–320). New York: Academic Press. Egeth, H. (1977). Attention and preattention. In G. H. Bower (Ed.), The psychology of learning and motivation (pp. 277–320). New York: Academic Press.
go back to reference Hommel, B. (1998). Automatic stimulus-response translation in dual-task performance. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 24, 1368–1384.CrossRefPubMed Hommel, B. (1998). Automatic stimulus-response translation in dual-task performance. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 24, 1368–1384.CrossRefPubMed
go back to reference Hommel, B., & Eglau, B. (2002). Control of stimulus-response translation in dual-task performance. Psychological Research, 66, 260–273.CrossRefPubMed Hommel, B., & Eglau, B. (2002). Control of stimulus-response translation in dual-task performance. Psychological Research, 66, 260–273.CrossRefPubMed
go back to reference Hommel, B., & Schneider, W.X. (2002). Visual attention and manual response selection: Distinct mechanisms operating on the same codes. Visual Cognition, 9, 392–420.CrossRef Hommel, B., & Schneider, W.X. (2002). Visual attention and manual response selection: Distinct mechanisms operating on the same codes. Visual Cognition, 9, 392–420.CrossRef
go back to reference Hommel, B., Müsseler, J., Aschersleben, G., & Prinz, W. (2001). The theory of event coding (TEC): A framework for perception and action planning. Behavioral and Brain Sciences, 24, 849–878.PubMed Hommel, B., Müsseler, J., Aschersleben, G., & Prinz, W. (2001). The theory of event coding (TEC): A framework for perception and action planning. Behavioral and Brain Sciences, 24, 849–878.PubMed
go back to reference Jiang, Y., Olson, I. R., & Chun, M. M. (2000). Organization of visual-short term memory. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, & Cognition, 26, 683–702. Jiang, Y., Olson, I. R., & Chun, M. M. (2000). Organization of visual-short term memory. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, & Cognition, 26, 683–702.
go back to reference Johnston, J. C., McCann, R. S., & Remington, R. W. (1995). Chronometric dissociation of input attention and central attention in human information processing. Psychological Science, 6, 365–369. Johnston, J. C., McCann, R. S., & Remington, R. W. (1995). Chronometric dissociation of input attention and central attention in human information processing. Psychological Science, 6, 365–369.
go back to reference Jolicoeur, P. (1999). Dual-task interference and visual encoding. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 25, 596–616.CrossRef Jolicoeur, P. (1999). Dual-task interference and visual encoding. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 25, 596–616.CrossRef
go back to reference Jolicoeur, P., & Dell’Acqua, R. (1998). The demonstration of short-term consolidation. Cognitive Psychology, 36, 138–202.CrossRefPubMed Jolicoeur, P., & Dell’Acqua, R. (1998). The demonstration of short-term consolidation. Cognitive Psychology, 36, 138–202.CrossRefPubMed
go back to reference Jolicoeur, P., & Dell’Acqua, R. (1999). Attentional and structural constraints on visual encoding. Psychological Research, 62, 154–164.CrossRef Jolicoeur, P., & Dell’Acqua, R. (1999). Attentional and structural constraints on visual encoding. Psychological Research, 62, 154–164.CrossRef
go back to reference Jolicoeur, P., Dell’Acqua, R., & Crebolder, J. (2000). Multitasking performance deficits: Forging some links between the attentional blink and the psychological refractory period. In S. Monsell & J. Driver (eds.), Control of cognitive processes: Attention & performance XVIII (pp. 309–330). Cambridge, MA: MIT Press. Jolicoeur, P., Dell’Acqua, R., & Crebolder, J. (2000). Multitasking performance deficits: Forging some links between the attentional blink and the psychological refractory period. In S. Monsell & J. Driver (eds.), Control of cognitive processes: Attention & performance XVIII (pp. 309–330). Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.
go back to reference Jolicoeur, P., Tombu, M., Oriet, C., & Stevanovski, B. (2002). From perception to action: Making the connection. In W. Prinz & B. Hommel (Eds.), Common mechanisms in perception and action: Attention & performance XIX (pp. 558–586). Oxford: Oxford University Press. Jolicoeur, P., Tombu, M., Oriet, C., & Stevanovski, B. (2002). From perception to action: Making the connection. In W. Prinz & B. Hommel (Eds.), Common mechanisms in perception and action: Attention & performance XIX (pp. 558–586). Oxford: Oxford University Press.
go back to reference Karlin, L., & Kestenbaum, R. (1968). Effect of number of alternative on the psychological refractory period. Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology, 20, 167–178.PubMed Karlin, L., & Kestenbaum, R. (1968). Effect of number of alternative on the psychological refractory period. Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology, 20, 167–178.PubMed
go back to reference Logan, G. D. (1978). Attention in character-classification tasks: Evidence for the automaticity of component stages. Journal of Experimental Psychology: General, 107, 32–63. Logan, G. D. (1978). Attention in character-classification tasks: Evidence for the automaticity of component stages. Journal of Experimental Psychology: General, 107, 32–63.
go back to reference Logan, G. D. (1979). On the use of the concurrent memory load to measure attention and automaticity. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 5, 189–207.CrossRef Logan, G. D. (1979). On the use of the concurrent memory load to measure attention and automaticity. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 5, 189–207.CrossRef
go back to reference Logan, G. D. (1980). Short-term memory demands of reaction-time tasks that differ in complexity. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 6, 375–389.CrossRef Logan, G. D. (1980). Short-term memory demands of reaction-time tasks that differ in complexity. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 6, 375–389.CrossRef
go back to reference Luck, S. J., & Beach, N. J. (1998). Visual attention and the binding problem: A neurophysiological perspective. In R. D. Wright (Ed.), Visual attention (pp. 455–478). New York: Oxford University Press. Luck, S. J., & Beach, N. J. (1998). Visual attention and the binding problem: A neurophysiological perspective. In R. D. Wright (Ed.), Visual attention (pp. 455–478). New York: Oxford University Press.
go back to reference Luck, S. J., & Vogel, E. K. (1997). The capacity of visual working memory for features and conjunctions. Nature, 390, 279–281.PubMed Luck, S. J., & Vogel, E. K. (1997). The capacity of visual working memory for features and conjunctions. Nature, 390, 279–281.PubMed
go back to reference Olson, I. R., Chun, M. M., & Anderson, A. K. (2001). Effects of phonological length on the attentional blink. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 27, 1116–1123.CrossRefPubMed Olson, I. R., Chun, M. M., & Anderson, A. K. (2001). Effects of phonological length on the attentional blink. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 27, 1116–1123.CrossRefPubMed
go back to reference Pashler, H. (1991). Shifting visual attention and selecting motor responses: Distinct attentional mechanisms. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 17, 1023–1040.PubMed Pashler, H. (1991). Shifting visual attention and selecting motor responses: Distinct attentional mechanisms. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 17, 1023–1040.PubMed
go back to reference Pashler, H. (1994). Dual-task interference in simple tasks: Data and theory. Psychological Bulletin, 116, 220–244.PubMed Pashler, H. (1994). Dual-task interference in simple tasks: Data and theory. Psychological Bulletin, 116, 220–244.PubMed
go back to reference Pashler, H. (1998). The psychology of attention. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press. Pashler, H. (1998). The psychology of attention. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.
go back to reference Pashler, H., & Johnston, J. C. (1989). Chronometric evidence for central postponement in temporally overlapping tasks. Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology, 41 A, 19–45. Pashler, H., & Johnston, J. C. (1989). Chronometric evidence for central postponement in temporally overlapping tasks. Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology, 41 A, 19–45.
go back to reference Raymond, J. E., Shapiro, K. L., & Arnell, K. M. (1992). Temporary suppression of visual processing in an RSVP task: An attentional blink? Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 18, 849–860.PubMed Raymond, J. E., Shapiro, K. L., & Arnell, K. M. (1992). Temporary suppression of visual processing in an RSVP task: An attentional blink? Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 18, 849–860.PubMed
go back to reference Shapiro, K. L., Arnell, K. M., & Raymond, J. E. (1997). The attentional blink: A view on attention and glimpse on consciousness. Trends in Cognitive Science, 1, 291–296. Shapiro, K. L., Arnell, K. M., & Raymond, J. E. (1997). The attentional blink: A view on attention and glimpse on consciousness. Trends in Cognitive Science, 1, 291–296.
go back to reference Smith, M. C. (1969). The effect of varying information on the psychological refractory period. Acta Psychologica, 30, 220–231.CrossRef Smith, M. C. (1969). The effect of varying information on the psychological refractory period. Acta Psychologica, 30, 220–231.CrossRef
go back to reference Stoet, G., & Hommel, B. (1999). Action planning and the temporal binding of response codes. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 25, 1625–1640.CrossRef Stoet, G., & Hommel, B. (1999). Action planning and the temporal binding of response codes. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 25, 1625–1640.CrossRef
go back to reference Stoet, G., & Hommel, B. (2002). Interaction between feature binding in perception and action. In W. Prinz & B. Hommel (Eds.), Common mechanisms in perception and action: Attention & performance XIX (pp. 538–552). Oxford: Oxford University Press. Stoet, G., & Hommel, B. (2002). Interaction between feature binding in perception and action. In W. Prinz & B. Hommel (Eds.), Common mechanisms in perception and action: Attention & performance XIX (pp. 538–552). Oxford: Oxford University Press.
go back to reference Telford, C. W. (1931). The refractory phase of voluntary and associative responses. Journal of Experimental Psychology, 14, 1–36. Telford, C. W. (1931). The refractory phase of voluntary and associative responses. Journal of Experimental Psychology, 14, 1–36.
go back to reference Van Selst, M., & Jolicoeur, P. (1997). Decision and response. Cognitive Psychology, 33, 266–307.PubMed Van Selst, M., & Jolicoeur, P. (1997). Decision and response. Cognitive Psychology, 33, 266–307.PubMed
go back to reference Ward, R., Duncan, J., & Shapiro, K. (1996). The slow time-course of visual attention. Cognitive Psychology, 30, 79–109.CrossRefPubMed Ward, R., Duncan, J., & Shapiro, K. (1996). The slow time-course of visual attention. Cognitive Psychology, 30, 79–109.CrossRefPubMed
go back to reference Welford, A. T. (1952). The “psychological refractory period” and the timing of high-speed performance—a review and a theory. British Journal of Psychology, 43, 2–19. Welford, A. T. (1952). The “psychological refractory period” and the timing of high-speed performance—a review and a theory. British Journal of Psychology, 43, 2–19.
go back to reference Woodman, G. F., Vogel, E. K., & Luck, S. J. (2001). Visual search remains efficient when visual working memory is full. Psychological Science, 12, 219–224.CrossRefPubMed Woodman, G. F., Vogel, E. K., & Luck, S. J. (2001). Visual search remains efficient when visual working memory is full. Psychological Science, 12, 219–224.CrossRefPubMed
Metagegevens
Titel
Selection and consolidation of objects and actions
Auteurs
Bernhard Hommel
Christian F. Doeller
Publicatiedatum
01-01-2005
Uitgeverij
Springer-Verlag
Gepubliceerd in
Psychological Research / Uitgave 3/2005
Print ISSN: 0340-0727
Elektronisch ISSN: 1430-2772
DOI
https://doi.org/10.1007/s00426-004-0171-z

Andere artikelen Uitgave 3/2005

Psychological Research 3/2005 Naar de uitgave