Skip to main content
Top
Gepubliceerd in: Psychological Research 2/2017

21-01-2016 | Original Article

Measuring the emotion-specificity of rapid stimulus-driven attraction of attention to fearful faces: evidence from emotion categorization and a comparison with disgusted faces

Auteurs: Shah Khalid, Gernot Horstmann, Thomas Ditye, Ulrich Ansorge

Gepubliceerd in: Psychological Research | Uitgave 2/2017

Log in om toegang te krijgen
share
DELEN

Deel dit onderdeel of sectie (kopieer de link)

  • Optie A:
    Klik op de rechtermuisknop op de link en selecteer de optie “linkadres kopiëren”
  • Optie B:
    Deel de link per e-mail

Abstract

In the current study, we tested whether a fear advantage—rapid attraction of attention to fearful faces that is more stimulus-driven than to neutral faces—is emotion specific. We used a cueing task with face cues preceding targets. Cues were non-predictive of the target locations. In two experiments, we found enhanced cueing of saccades towards the targets with fearful face cues than with neutral face cues: Saccades towards targets were more efficient with cues and targets at the same position (under valid conditions) than at opposite positions (under invalid conditions), and this cueing effect was stronger with fearful than with neutral face cues. In addition, this cueing effect difference between fearful and neutral faces was absent with inverted faces as cues, indicating that the fear advantage is face-specific. We also show that emotion categorization of the face cues mirrored these effects: Participants were better at categorizing face cues as fearful or neutral with upright than with inverted faces (Experiment 1). Finally, in alternative blocks including disgusted faces instead of fearful faces, we found more similar cueing effects with disgusted faces and neutral faces, and with upright and inverted faces (Experiment 2). Jointly, these results demonstrate that the fear advantage is emotion-specific. Results are discussed in light of evolutionary explanations of the fear advantage.
Literatuur
go back to reference Bacon, W. F., & Egeth, H. E. (1994). Overriding stimulus-driven attentional capture. Perception and Psychophysics, 55, 485–496.CrossRefPubMed Bacon, W. F., & Egeth, H. E. (1994). Overriding stimulus-driven attentional capture. Perception and Psychophysics, 55, 485–496.CrossRefPubMed
go back to reference Bannerman, R. L., Milders, M., & Sahraie, A. (2010). Attentional bias to brief threat-related faces revealed by saccadic eye movements. Emotion, 10, 733–738.CrossRefPubMed Bannerman, R. L., Milders, M., & Sahraie, A. (2010). Attentional bias to brief threat-related faces revealed by saccadic eye movements. Emotion, 10, 733–738.CrossRefPubMed
go back to reference Becker, D. V., Anderson, U. S., Mortensen, C. R., Neufeld, S. L., & Neel, R. (2011). The face in the crowd effect unconfounded: happy faces, not angry faces, are more efficiently detected in single-and multiple-target visual search tasks. Journal of Experimental Psychology: General, 140, 637–659.CrossRef Becker, D. V., Anderson, U. S., Mortensen, C. R., Neufeld, S. L., & Neel, R. (2011). The face in the crowd effect unconfounded: happy faces, not angry faces, are more efficiently detected in single-and multiple-target visual search tasks. Journal of Experimental Psychology: General, 140, 637–659.CrossRef
go back to reference Boll, S., Gamer, M., Kalisch, R., & Büchel, C. (2011). Processing of facial expressions and their significance for the observer in subregions of the human amygdala. Neuroimage, 56(1), 299–306.CrossRefPubMed Boll, S., Gamer, M., Kalisch, R., & Büchel, C. (2011). Processing of facial expressions and their significance for the observer in subregions of the human amygdala. Neuroimage, 56(1), 299–306.CrossRefPubMed
go back to reference Carretié, L., Ruiz-Padial, E., López-Martín, S., & Albert, J. (2011). Decomposing unpleasantness: differential exogenous attention to disgusting and fearful stimuli. Biological Psychology, 86, 247–253.CrossRefPubMed Carretié, L., Ruiz-Padial, E., López-Martín, S., & Albert, J. (2011). Decomposing unpleasantness: differential exogenous attention to disgusting and fearful stimuli. Biological Psychology, 86, 247–253.CrossRefPubMed
go back to reference Chapman, H. A., & Anderson, A. K. (2012). Understanding disgust. Annals of the New York Academy of Sciences, 1251, 62–76.CrossRefPubMed Chapman, H. A., & Anderson, A. K. (2012). Understanding disgust. Annals of the New York Academy of Sciences, 1251, 62–76.CrossRefPubMed
go back to reference Chapman, H. A., Johannes, K., Poppenk, J. L., Moscovitch, M., & Anderson, A. K. (2013). Evidence for the differential salience of disgust and fear in episodic memory. Journal of Experimental Psychology: General, 142, 1100–1112.CrossRef Chapman, H. A., Johannes, K., Poppenk, J. L., Moscovitch, M., & Anderson, A. K. (2013). Evidence for the differential salience of disgust and fear in episodic memory. Journal of Experimental Psychology: General, 142, 1100–1112.CrossRef
go back to reference Cisler, J. M., & Olatunji, B. O. (2010). Components of attentional biases in contamination fear: evidence for difficulty in disengagement. Behaviour Research and Therapy, 48, 74–78.CrossRefPubMed Cisler, J. M., & Olatunji, B. O. (2010). Components of attentional biases in contamination fear: evidence for difficulty in disengagement. Behaviour Research and Therapy, 48, 74–78.CrossRefPubMed
go back to reference Cisler, J. M., Olatunji, B. O., Lohr, J. M., & Williams, N. L. (2009). Attentional bias differences between fear and disgust: implications for the role of disgust in disgust-related anxiety disorders. Cognition and Emotion, 23, 675–687.CrossRefPubMedPubMedCentral Cisler, J. M., Olatunji, B. O., Lohr, J. M., & Williams, N. L. (2009). Attentional bias differences between fear and disgust: implications for the role of disgust in disgust-related anxiety disorders. Cognition and Emotion, 23, 675–687.CrossRefPubMedPubMedCentral
go back to reference Davis, F. C., Somerville, L. H., Ruberry, E. J., Berry, A. B., Shin, L. M., & Whalen, P. J. (2011). A tale of two negatives: differential memory modulation by threat-related facial expressions. Emotion, 11, 647–655.CrossRefPubMedPubMedCentral Davis, F. C., Somerville, L. H., Ruberry, E. J., Berry, A. B., Shin, L. M., & Whalen, P. J. (2011). A tale of two negatives: differential memory modulation by threat-related facial expressions. Emotion, 11, 647–655.CrossRefPubMedPubMedCentral
go back to reference Deubel, H., & Schneider, W. X. (1996). Saccade target selection and object recognition: evidence for a common attentional mechanism. Vision Research, 36, 1827–1837.CrossRefPubMed Deubel, H., & Schneider, W. X. (1996). Saccade target selection and object recognition: evidence for a common attentional mechanism. Vision Research, 36, 1827–1837.CrossRefPubMed
go back to reference Eastwood, J. D., Smilek, D., & Merikle, P. M. (2001). Differential attentional guidance by unattended faces expressing positive and negative emotion. Perception and Psychophysics, 63, 1004–1013.CrossRefPubMed Eastwood, J. D., Smilek, D., & Merikle, P. M. (2001). Differential attentional guidance by unattended faces expressing positive and negative emotion. Perception and Psychophysics, 63, 1004–1013.CrossRefPubMed
go back to reference Fischer, B., & Weber, H. (1993). Express saccades and visual attention. Behavioral and Brain Sciences, 16, 533–610.CrossRef Fischer, B., & Weber, H. (1993). Express saccades and visual attention. Behavioral and Brain Sciences, 16, 533–610.CrossRef
go back to reference Folk, C. L., Remington, R. W., & Johnston, J. C. (1992). Involuntary covert orienting is contingent on attentional control settings. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 18, 1030.PubMed Folk, C. L., Remington, R. W., & Johnston, J. C. (1992). Involuntary covert orienting is contingent on attentional control settings. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 18, 1030.PubMed
go back to reference Fox, E., Russo, R., Bowles, R., & Dutton, K. (2001). Do threatening stimuli draw or hold visual attention in subclinical anxiety? Journal of Experimental Psychology: General, 130, 681.CrossRef Fox, E., Russo, R., Bowles, R., & Dutton, K. (2001). Do threatening stimuli draw or hold visual attention in subclinical anxiety? Journal of Experimental Psychology: General, 130, 681.CrossRef
go back to reference Fox, E., Russo, R., & Dutton, K. (2002). Attentional bias for threat: evidence for delayed disengagement from emotional faces. Cognition and Emotion, 16, 355–379.CrossRefPubMedPubMedCentral Fox, E., Russo, R., & Dutton, K. (2002). Attentional bias for threat: evidence for delayed disengagement from emotional faces. Cognition and Emotion, 16, 355–379.CrossRefPubMedPubMedCentral
go back to reference Fridlund, A. (1994). Human facial expression: An evolutionary view. San Diego: Academic Press. Fridlund, A. (1994). Human facial expression: An evolutionary view. San Diego: Academic Press.
go back to reference Frischen, A., Eastwood, J. D., & Smilek, D. (2008). Visual search for faces with emotional expressions. Psychological Bulletin, 134, 662–676.CrossRefPubMed Frischen, A., Eastwood, J. D., & Smilek, D. (2008). Visual search for faces with emotional expressions. Psychological Bulletin, 134, 662–676.CrossRefPubMed
go back to reference Hoffman, J. E., & Subramaniam, B. (1995). The role of visual attention in saccadic eye movements. Perception and Psychophysics, 57, 787–795.CrossRefPubMed Hoffman, J. E., & Subramaniam, B. (1995). The role of visual attention in saccadic eye movements. Perception and Psychophysics, 57, 787–795.CrossRefPubMed
go back to reference Horstmann, G. (2003). What do facial expressions convey: feeling states, behavioral intentions, or action requests? Emotion, 3, 150–166.CrossRefPubMed Horstmann, G. (2003). What do facial expressions convey: feeling states, behavioral intentions, or action requests? Emotion, 3, 150–166.CrossRefPubMed
go back to reference Horstmann, G. (2009). Visual search for schematic affective faces: stability and variability of search slopes with different instances. Cognition and Emotion, 23, 355–379.CrossRef Horstmann, G. (2009). Visual search for schematic affective faces: stability and variability of search slopes with different instances. Cognition and Emotion, 23, 355–379.CrossRef
go back to reference Horstmann, G., & Ansorge, U. (2009). Visual search for facial expressions of emotion: a comparison of dynamic and static faces. Emotion, 9, 29–38.CrossRefPubMed Horstmann, G., & Ansorge, U. (2009). Visual search for facial expressions of emotion: a comparison of dynamic and static faces. Emotion, 9, 29–38.CrossRefPubMed
go back to reference Horstmann, G., & Bauland, A. (2006). Search asymmetries with real faces: testing the anger-superiority effect. Emotion, 6, 193–207.CrossRefPubMed Horstmann, G., & Bauland, A. (2006). Search asymmetries with real faces: testing the anger-superiority effect. Emotion, 6, 193–207.CrossRefPubMed
go back to reference Horstmann, G., Scharlau, I., & Ansorge, U. (2006). More efficient rejection of happy than of angry face distractors in visual search. Psychonomic Bulletin & Review, 13, 1067–1073.CrossRef Horstmann, G., Scharlau, I., & Ansorge, U. (2006). More efficient rejection of happy than of angry face distractors in visual search. Psychonomic Bulletin & Review, 13, 1067–1073.CrossRef
go back to reference Jin, Z., & Reeves, A. (2009). Attentional release in the saccadic gap effect. Vision Research, 49, 2045–2055.CrossRefPubMed Jin, Z., & Reeves, A. (2009). Attentional release in the saccadic gap effect. Vision Research, 49, 2045–2055.CrossRefPubMed
go back to reference Koster, E. H., Crombez, G., Van Damme, S., Verschuere, B., & De Houwer, J. (2004). Does imminent threat capture and hold attention? Emotion, 4, 312.CrossRefPubMed Koster, E. H., Crombez, G., Van Damme, S., Verschuere, B., & De Houwer, J. (2004). Does imminent threat capture and hold attention? Emotion, 4, 312.CrossRefPubMed
go back to reference Kowler, E., Anderson, E., Dosher, B., & Blaser, E. (1995). The role of attention in the programming of saccades. Vision Research, 35, 1897–1916.CrossRefPubMed Kowler, E., Anderson, E., Dosher, B., & Blaser, E. (1995). The role of attention in the programming of saccades. Vision Research, 35, 1897–1916.CrossRefPubMed
go back to reference Krusemark, E. A., & Li, W. (2011). Do all threats work the same way? Divergent effects of fear and disgust on sensory perception and attention. The Journal of Neuroscience, 31, 3429–3434.CrossRefPubMedPubMedCentral Krusemark, E. A., & Li, W. (2011). Do all threats work the same way? Divergent effects of fear and disgust on sensory perception and attention. The Journal of Neuroscience, 31, 3429–3434.CrossRefPubMedPubMedCentral
go back to reference Lang, P. J., Bradley, M. M., & Cuthbert, B. N. (1997). International affective picture system (IAPS): Technical manual and affective ratings. NIMH Center for the Study of Emotion and Attention, pp 39–58. Lang, P. J., Bradley, M. M., & Cuthbert, B. N. (1997). International affective picture system (IAPS): Technical manual and affective ratings. NIMH Center for the Study of Emotion and Attention, pp 39–58.
go back to reference Laux, L., Glanzmann, P., Schaffner, P., & Spielberger, C. D. (1981). Das State-Trait-Anxiety-Inventory (STAI) [The state-trait anxiety inventory (STAI)]. German version. Göttingen: Hogrefe. Laux, L., Glanzmann, P., Schaffner, P., & Spielberger, C. D. (1981). Das State-Trait-Anxiety-Inventory (STAI) [The state-trait anxiety inventory (STAI)]. German version. Göttingen: Hogrefe.
go back to reference Leder, H., & Bruce, V. (2000). When inverted faces are recognized: the role of configural information in face recognition. The Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology, 53A, 513–536.CrossRef Leder, H., & Bruce, V. (2000). When inverted faces are recognized: the role of configural information in face recognition. The Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology, 53A, 513–536.CrossRef
go back to reference Lundqvist, D., Flykt, A., & Öhman, A. (1998). The Karolinska directed emotional faces—KDEF [CD ROM]. Stockholm: Karolinska Institutet, Department of Clinical Neuroscience, Psychology Section. Lundqvist, D., Flykt, A., & Öhman, A. (1998). The Karolinska directed emotional faces—KDEF [CD ROM]. Stockholm: Karolinska Institutet, Department of Clinical Neuroscience, Psychology Section.
go back to reference N’Diaye, K., Sander, D., & Vuilleumier, P. (2009). Self-relevance processing in the human amygdala: gaze direction, facial expression, and emotion intensity. Emotion, 9, 798–806.CrossRefPubMed N’Diaye, K., Sander, D., & Vuilleumier, P. (2009). Self-relevance processing in the human amygdala: gaze direction, facial expression, and emotion intensity. Emotion, 9, 798–806.CrossRefPubMed
go back to reference Öhman, A. (1993). Fear and anxiety as emotional phenomenon: Clinical phenomenology, evolutionary perspectives, and information-processing mechanisms. In M. Lewis & J. M. Haviland (Eds.), Handbook of emotions (pp. 511–536). New York: Guilford Press. Öhman, A. (1993). Fear and anxiety as emotional phenomenon: Clinical phenomenology, evolutionary perspectives, and information-processing mechanisms. In M. Lewis & J. M. Haviland (Eds.), Handbook of emotions (pp. 511–536). New York: Guilford Press.
go back to reference Öhman, A., Flykt, A., & Esteves, F. (2001). Emotion drives attention: detecting the snake in the grass. Journal of Experimental Psychology: General, 130, 466–478.CrossRef Öhman, A., Flykt, A., & Esteves, F. (2001). Emotion drives attention: detecting the snake in the grass. Journal of Experimental Psychology: General, 130, 466–478.CrossRef
go back to reference Posner, M. I. (1980). Orienting of attention. Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology, 32A, 3–25.CrossRef Posner, M. I. (1980). Orienting of attention. Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology, 32A, 3–25.CrossRef
go back to reference Rozin, P., Haidt, J., & McCauley, C. R. (1993). Disgust. In M. Lewis & J. Haviland (Eds.), Handbook of emotions (pp. 575–594). New York: Guilford. Rozin, P., Haidt, J., & McCauley, C. R. (1993). Disgust. In M. Lewis & J. Haviland (Eds.), Handbook of emotions (pp. 575–594). New York: Guilford.
go back to reference Saslow, M. G. (1967). Effects of components of displacement-step stimuli upon latency for saccadic eye movement. Journal of the Optical Society of America, 57, 1024–1029.CrossRefPubMed Saslow, M. G. (1967). Effects of components of displacement-step stimuli upon latency for saccadic eye movement. Journal of the Optical Society of America, 57, 1024–1029.CrossRefPubMed
go back to reference Savage, R. A., Lipp, O. V., Craig, B. M., Becker, S. I., & Horstmann, G. (2013). In search of the emotional face: anger versus happiness superiority in visual search. Emotion, 13, 758–768.CrossRefPubMed Savage, R. A., Lipp, O. V., Craig, B. M., Becker, S. I., & Horstmann, G. (2013). In search of the emotional face: anger versus happiness superiority in visual search. Emotion, 13, 758–768.CrossRefPubMed
go back to reference Susskind, J. M., Lee, D. H., Cusi, A., Feiman, R., Grabski, W., & Anderson, A. K. (2008). Expressing fear enhances sensory acquisition. Nature Neuroscience, 11, 843–850.CrossRefPubMed Susskind, J. M., Lee, D. H., Cusi, A., Feiman, R., Grabski, W., & Anderson, A. K. (2008). Expressing fear enhances sensory acquisition. Nature Neuroscience, 11, 843–850.CrossRefPubMed
go back to reference van Hooff, J. C., Devue, C., Vieweg, P. E., & Theeuwes, J. (2013). Disgust-and not fear-evoking images hold our attention. Acta Psychologica, 143, 1–6.CrossRefPubMed van Hooff, J. C., Devue, C., Vieweg, P. E., & Theeuwes, J. (2013). Disgust-and not fear-evoking images hold our attention. Acta Psychologica, 143, 1–6.CrossRefPubMed
go back to reference Vermeulen, N., Godefroid, J., & Mermillod, M. (2009). Emotional modulation of attention: fear increases but disgust reduces the attentional blink. PLoS ONE, 4(11), e7924.CrossRefPubMedPubMedCentral Vermeulen, N., Godefroid, J., & Mermillod, M. (2009). Emotional modulation of attention: fear increases but disgust reduces the attentional blink. PLoS ONE, 4(11), e7924.CrossRefPubMedPubMedCentral
go back to reference Vogt, J., Lozo, L., Koster, E. H., & De Houwer, J. (2011). On the role of goal relevance in emotional attention: disgust evokes early attention to cleanliness. Cognition and Emotion, 25, 466–477.CrossRefPubMed Vogt, J., Lozo, L., Koster, E. H., & De Houwer, J. (2011). On the role of goal relevance in emotional attention: disgust evokes early attention to cleanliness. Cognition and Emotion, 25, 466–477.CrossRefPubMed
go back to reference Whalen, P. J. (1998). Fear, vigilance, and ambiguity: initial neuroimaging studies of the human amygdala. Current Directions in Psychological Science, 7, 177–188.CrossRef Whalen, P. J. (1998). Fear, vigilance, and ambiguity: initial neuroimaging studies of the human amygdala. Current Directions in Psychological Science, 7, 177–188.CrossRef
go back to reference Yang, E., Zald, D. H., & Blake, R. (2007). Fearful expressions gain preferential access to awareness during continuous flash suppression. Emotion, 7, 882–886.CrossRefPubMedPubMedCentral Yang, E., Zald, D. H., & Blake, R. (2007). Fearful expressions gain preferential access to awareness during continuous flash suppression. Emotion, 7, 882–886.CrossRefPubMedPubMedCentral
Metagegevens
Titel
Measuring the emotion-specificity of rapid stimulus-driven attraction of attention to fearful faces: evidence from emotion categorization and a comparison with disgusted faces
Auteurs
Shah Khalid
Gernot Horstmann
Thomas Ditye
Ulrich Ansorge
Publicatiedatum
21-01-2016
Uitgeverij
Springer Berlin Heidelberg
Gepubliceerd in
Psychological Research / Uitgave 2/2017
Print ISSN: 0340-0727
Elektronisch ISSN: 1430-2772
DOI
https://doi.org/10.1007/s00426-016-0743-8

Andere artikelen Uitgave 2/2017

Psychological Research 2/2017 Naar de uitgave