Elsevier

Biological Psychology

Volume 86, Issue 3, March 2011, Pages 247-253
Biological Psychology

Decomposing unpleasantness: Differential exogenous attention to disgusting and fearful stimuli

https://doi.org/10.1016/j.biopsycho.2010.12.005Get rights and content

Abstract

Negative stimuli have consistently been shown to efficiently attract exogenous attention. Two different types of unpleasant stimuli, disgusting and fearful, sharing similar arousal and valence, are usually employed as a single category. However, since they diverge in several important aspects (biological functionality, associated feelings, and central and peripheral physiological correlates), it may be expected that their potential to capture attention differs. Event-related potentials and behavioral indices were recorded while participants were engaged in a digit categorization task in response to three types of irrelevant, distracting pictures: disgusting, fearful and neutral. Disgusting trials were associated with worse performance than fearful trials in the digit categorization task as revealed by reaction times and number of errors. Moreover, P2-associated cuneus activation and scalp anterior P2 amplitude were greater for disgusting than for fearful distracters. All these indices reveal that, under the experimental conditions employed in the present study, disgusting distracters are more efficient at attracting exogenous attention than are fearful distracters.

Research highlights

▶ Negative stimuli are separated into fearful and disgusting events. ▶ Disgusting and fearful stimuli differ in their capability to capture exogenous/automatic attention, disgusting showing a robust advantage. ▶ Behavioral (reaction times) and neural indices (P2-related cuneus activation as well as scalp P2 amplitudes) of exogenous/automatic attention were convergent. ▶ Results may not be explained by valence or arousal differences between disgusting and fearful stimuli. Idiosyncratic features of disgust may explain these results.

Introduction

Negatively arousing visual stimuli have been shown to efficiently attract exogenous attention (also termed automatic or bottom-up attention), as revealed by electrophysiological and behavioral responses to emotional distracters while subjects are engaged in a cognitive task (Carretié et al., 2008, Carretié et al., 2004, Carretié et al., 2009b, Constantine et al., 2001, Doallo et al., 2006, Huang and Luo, 2007, Thomas et al., 2007, Vuilleumier et al., 2001, Yuan et al., 2007). This ability to automatically capture attention has obvious adaptive and evolutionary advantages: the consequences of not detecting a negative event are often much more dramatic than the consequences of ignoring or reacting slowly to neutral or even appetitive stimuli. An important issue is that, in these studies on exogenous attention, negatively arousing stimuli are treated as a single category. In part, this is due to the fact that some of the most prototypical negative stimuli (e.g. spiders or bloody scenes) elicit a mixture of negative emotions (Olatunji and Sawchuk, 2005). Nevertheless, this type of event could be divided into different subcategories.

Fear and disgust are two emotions that share the same emotional valence (negative) and high ability to arouse (higher than that elicited by other negative emotions such as sadness: Russell, 1980). In functional terms, both types of emotion also share the same scope: avoiding the event that is causing, or may cause, displeasure. However, they differ in several respects. First, the subjective feeling associated with each of these two emotional states is clearly different. Second, their biological meaning is also divergent: while disgust is primarily related to contamination (Rozin, 2000), fear facilitates avoidance of a broader type of danger that may cause harm at many levels. Third, they appear to preferentially activate different brain areas. Whereas fear-related stimuli have been reported to mainly activate amygdala, disgusting events preferentially activate the anterior insula when processes other than exogenous attention are explored (see a review in Calder et al., 2001; but see, e.g. Schienle et al., 2006, Stark et al., 2007). Fourth, it has been proposed that, at the peripheral level, disgust is mainly parasympathetically mediated, while fear is a sympathetically mediated process (Levenson et al., 1990).

Therefore, a relevant question that arises once these differences are taken into account is whether disgusting stimuli and fearful stimuli have the same uniform ability to capture attention as previously assumed. Since the main reason why negative stimuli efficiently capture automatic attention is biological, one could contend that threatening and repugnant events, which have diverse biological meaning, should differ in their ability to attract attention. To the best of our knowledge, only two studies, both of a behavioral nature (no physiological responses were recorded), have compared the differences between automatic attention to fearful and to disgusting stimuli. In one of them, the emotional Stroop task was presented to subjects using neutral, threat-related and disgust-related words (Charash and McKay, 2002). While disgust-related words interfered with the cognitive ongoing task to a greater extent than did neutral words, no differences were found between fear-related and neutral words. In the second study (Cisler et al., 2009b), “fear words”, “disgust words” and neutral words were presented to subjects during a cognitive task in which their emotional content was irrelevant. In this case, fear words were found to interfere with the cognitive task more than disgust words. Therefore, although fear- and disgust-related attention differences have been found, the question as to the direction of these differences remains unanswered. Since words tend to be less arousing than other types of visual emotional stimuli (Hinojosa et al., 2009, Keil, 2006, Kissler et al., 2006, Mogg and Bradley, 1998, Vanderploeg et al., 1987), it would be advantageous to obtain additional data using pictorial stimulation.

No studies on brain activity yet exist on the differential ability of fearful and disgusting stimuli to capture attention, although event-related potentials (ERPs) have been shown to be good indices of exogenous attention. More specifically, the P2 component of ERPs shows significant amplitude increments when a negative stimulus automatically attracts attention in a wide variety of tasks (Carretié et al., 2004, Doallo et al., 2006, Huang and Luo, 2007, Thomas et al., 2007, Yuan et al., 2007), in contrast to other components such as late positive potentials, which reflect top-down attention to emotional events (see the review by Olofsson et al., 2008; see also Hajcak et al., 2009). In previous experiments on attention to emotional pictures, P2 has been shown to originate in the visual cortex (Carretié et al., 2001, Carretié et al., 2004). This is an important issue, since Schienle et al. (2006), presenting disgusting and fearful pictures (but not exploring automatic attention: participants were asked explicitly to direct processing resources to pictures), found that maximal disgust > fear differences were produced in visual areas of the brain (fear > disgust was not explored). Stark et al. (2007) also explored brain responses to disgusting and fearful pictures that participants attended to in a top-down fashion. Again, disgust > fear contrasts yielded significant effects in visual areas, along with the insula. Fear > disgust analyses revealed extensive areas of visual cortices as the most sensitive, in statistical terms, to this contrast. Therefore, the role of P2 in automatic attention and its visual cortex origin make this component especially interesting for the scope of this study.

The present experiment was aimed at exploring whether a similar or differential pattern of exogenous attention exists in response to fear- and disgust-related distracters while subjects were engaged in a cognitive task. Both behavioral and neural indices of exogenous attention were recorded. Behavioral measures consisted of reaction times and number of errors in the cognitive task. Automatic capture of attention by distracters is reflected in poorer performance in the ongoing cognitive task. The neural index of attentional capture was P2, which, as explained, has been reported to reflect automatic attention to emotional stimuli. Analyses on this component involved source localization in order to spatially characterize possible experimental effects. Although fearful stimuli have been studied much more than disgusting stimuli in emotional attention research, thus becoming the prototypical negative category, the few existing data comparing both types of unpleasant events suggest that the latter are powerful attracters of exogenous attention. Moreover, some of the most frequently used fearful stimuli, such as threatening animals (e.g. spiders or snakes) or physical injury scenes, may be more associated with disgust than with fear (Olatunji and Sawchuk, 2005), so part of the attentional effect traditionally attributed to “negativity” may have been related to disgust processing. Therefore, we expected behavioral and neural indices of exogenous attention to be significantly modulated by disgustingness of distracters. In line with previous data described above, this disgust modulation of automatic attention could be even greater than fear modulation in several areas of the visual cortex.

Section snippets

Participants

Twenty-six subjects (21 women, age range of 19–30 years, mean = 22.73) participated in this experiment. All of them were students of Psychology at the Universidad Autónoma de Madrid and voluntarily took part in this experiment after providing their informed consent to participate, reporting normal or corrected-to-normal visual acuity.

Stimuli and procedure

Subjects were placed in an electrically shielded, sound-attenuated room. Three types of pictures were presented to participants, in a single run, through RGB

Detection, spatiotemporal characterization, and quantification of P2

Fig. 1 shows a selection of grand averages once the baseline value (prestimulus recording) had been subtracted from each ERP. These grand averages correspond to frontal areas, where the critical ERP component (anterior P2, as explained later) is prominent. The first analytical step was detecting and quantifying P2 (see Section 2.4). Seven temporal factors were extracted by tPCA and submitted to promax rotation (Fig. 2). Factor peak-latency and topography characteristics associated FT6 with P2.

Discussion

The present results indicate that fearfulness and disgustingness differ in the way they influence exogenous attention. Such a conclusion is based on both neural and behavioral responses. The correlation between both types of responses suggests their functional association. At the behavioral level, disgusting distracters were those interfering maximally with the ongoing cognitive task, as revealed by reaction times and number of errors: participants responded slower and more inaccurately to the

Acknowledgements

This work was supported by the grants PSI2008-03688 and PSI2009-12368 from the Ministerio de Ciencia e Innovación (MICINN) of Spain. MICINN also supports Jacobo Albert through a Juan de la Cierva contract (JCI-2010-07766).

References (53)

  • Y.X. Huang et al.

    Attention shortage resistance of negative stimuli in an implicit emotional task

    Neuroscience Letters

    (2007)
  • A. Keil

    Macroscopic brain dynamics during verbal and pictorial processing of affective stimuli

    Progress in Brain Research

    (2006)
  • J.L. Kenemans et al.

    Visual stimulus change and the orienting reaction: event-related potential evidence for a two-stage process

    Biological Psychology

    (1992)
  • J.L. Kenemans et al.

    Initial- and change-orienting reactions: an analysis based on visual single-trial event-related potentials

    Biological Psychology

    (1989)
  • J. Kissler et al.

    Emotional and semantic networks in visual word processing: insights from ERP studies

    Progress in Brain Research

    (2006)
  • K. Mogg et al.

    A cognitive-motivational analysis of anxiety

    Behaviour Research and Therapy

    (1998)
  • J.K. Olofsson et al.

    Affective picture processing: an integrative review of ERP findings

    Biological Psychology

    (2008)
  • A.C. Page

    Blood–injury phobia

    Clinical Psychology Review

    (1994)
  • M.V. Peelen et al.

    Endogenous and exogenous attention shifts are mediated by the same large-scale neural network

    NeuroImage

    (2004)
  • A. Schienle et al.

    fMRI responses to pictures of mutilation and contamination

    Neuroscience Letters

    (2006)
  • D.J. Simons

    Attentional capture and inattentional blindness

    Trends in Cognitive Sciences

    (2000)
  • R. Stark et al.

    Hemodynamic brain correlates of disgust and fear ratings

    NeuroImage

    (2007)
  • S.J. Thomas et al.

    Event-related potentials during an emotional Stroop task

    International Journal of Psychophysiology

    (2007)
  • R.D. Vanderploeg et al.

    Judgments of emotion in words and faces: ERP correlates

    International Journal of Psychophysiology

    (1987)
  • P. Vuilleumier et al.

    Effects of attention and emotion on face processing in the human brain an event-related fMRI study

    Neuron

    (2001)
  • S.R. Woody et al.

    Disgust as a motivator of avoidance of spiders

    Journal of Anxiety Disorders

    (2005)
  • Cited by (75)

    • Decoding the neural responses to experiencing disgust and sadness

      2022, Brain Research
      Citation Excerpt :

      Therefore, it cannot be concluded that the differences were the result of the evoked emotions, or to some other property of the videos. Our results show that disgust and sadness have distinct EEG responses that are not related to differences in valence and arousal, further consolidating the notion that discrete emotions evoke distinct neural responses (Carretié et al., 2011; Hot and Sequeira, 2013; Zhao et al., 2018; Wheaton et al., 2013). These findings implicate that a more systematic approach of affective neuroscience is needed to gain a better understanding of the neural responses to experiences of emotions.

    • Common and separable behavioral and neural mechanisms underlie the generalization of fear and disgust

      2022, Progress in Neuro-Psychopharmacology and Biological Psychiatry
    • Exploration of Exogenous Attention to Disgust and Fear Pictures with Different Spatial Frequencies Through Event-related Potentials

      2022, Neuroscience
      Citation Excerpt :

      Whereas disgust warns about toxic substances and potentially contagious and disease-causing stimuli, fear is associated with more imminent threats to life as, for example, the attack of an animal that can cause harm or even death immediately (Woody and Teachman, 2000; Cisler et al., 2009). Therefore, both types of emotions may capture exogenous attention in different ways because, whereas fearful stimuli calls for an immediate and fast response after its detection, disgust stimuli do not require such an urgent response but probably demand deeper examination that can turn, for example, the disgusting stimulus into a potentially eatable product (Carretié et al., 2011) or into an important source of information about situations as illness or attacks that can save life (own or others). In recent years, a considerable amount of research has been carried out with the aim of investigating the differential effects of fear and disgust stimuli on the capture of automatic attention.

    View all citing articles on Scopus
    View full text