Skip to main content
Top
Gepubliceerd in: Psychological Research 4/2021

04-04-2020 | Original Article

Critical bottom-up attentional factors in the handle orientation effect: asymmetric luminance transients and object-center eccentricity relative to fixation

Auteurs: Kiril Kostov, Armina Janyan

Gepubliceerd in: Psychological Research | Uitgave 4/2021

Log in om toegang te krijgen
share
DELEN

Deel dit onderdeel of sectie (kopieer de link)

  • Optie A:
    Klik op de rechtermuisknop op de link en selecteer de optie “linkadres kopiëren”
  • Optie B:
    Deel de link per e-mail

Abstract

In 1998, Tucker and Ellis found that keypress responses are faster when the task-irrelevant orientation of a graspable object’s handle corresponds to response hand location. Over the past 20 years, researchers have disagreed over the extent to which grasping affordance or spatial compatibility contributes to the effect. One of the causes behind the conflicting findings and interpretations may be that studies advocating the grasping affordance view have tended to overlook the contributions of low-level perceptual characteristics to the observed correspondence effects. The present study evaluated the role of visual salience and bottom-up attention in the occurrence of the effect. Experiment 1 involved a vertical orientation task (bimanual keypresses) using photographs of graspable objects, centered based on object width or pixel area. The same procedure was performed using a color discrimination task on solid-colored silhouettes, large (Experiment 2) and small-sized (Experiment 3), as well as silhouette outlines (Experiment 4). Similar result patterns across Experiments 1–3 were observed and discussed in the context of diverging findings in Experiment 4, prompting us to introduce the notion of asymmetry-based Simon effects, whereby location is coded at stimulus onset on the basis of asymmetric changes in luminance between both hemifields, coupled with object-center eccentricity relative to fixation. These low-level factors were critical in modulating the temporal dynamics, as well as the direction of compatibility effects (toward handles or bodies), irrespective of grasping affordance, task, object identity, or stimulus size. The present findings provide further evidence that the Tucker and Ellis paradigm for studying variable affordances is extremely vulnerable to location-coding, which may arise on the basis of exogenous deployments of attention. This problem is only exacerbated in a large portion of the relevant literature, whereby visually complex stimuli are primarily discussed in terms of their graspable nature and relation to task, rather than their low-level, attention-capturing features.
Bijlagen
Alleen toegankelijk voor geautoriseerde gebruikers
Voetnoten
1
A similar pattern of results was observed for upside-down trials, with a significant interaction between horizontal position and hand-handle compatibility (F(1, 54) = 44.165, p < .001, \({\upeta }_{p}^{2}\) = .450). A compatibility effect of 22 ms (p < .001) occurred in the direction of object bodies when the upside-down stimuli were centered according to their width. Conversely, a handle orientation effect of 19 ms (p < .001) was observed when stimuli were centered based on pixel area.
 
Literatuur
go back to reference Anderson, S. J., Yamagishi, N., & Karavia, V. (2002). Attentional processes link perception and action. Proceedings of the Royal Society of London, Series B, 269, 1225–1232. Anderson, S. J., Yamagishi, N., & Karavia, V. (2002). Attentional processes link perception and action. Proceedings of the Royal Society of London, Series B, 269, 1225–1232.
go back to reference Ansorge, U. (2003). Spatial Simon effects and compatibility effects induced by observed gaze direction. Visual Cognition, 10(3), 363–383. Ansorge, U. (2003). Spatial Simon effects and compatibility effects induced by observed gaze direction. Visual Cognition, 10(3), 363–383.
go back to reference Azaad, S., Laham, S. M., & Shields, P. (2019). A meta-analysis of the object-based compatibility effect. Cognition, 190, 105–127.PubMed Azaad, S., Laham, S. M., & Shields, P. (2019). A meta-analysis of the object-based compatibility effect. Cognition, 190, 105–127.PubMed
go back to reference Biederman, I., & Cooper, E. E. (1992). Size invariance in visual object priming. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 18(1), 121–133. Biederman, I., & Cooper, E. E. (1992). Size invariance in visual object priming. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 18(1), 121–133.
go back to reference Borghi, A. M., & Riggio, L. (2009). Sentence comprehension and simulation of object temporary, canonical and stable affordances. Brain Research, 1253, 117–128.PubMed Borghi, A. M., & Riggio, L. (2009). Sentence comprehension and simulation of object temporary, canonical and stable affordances. Brain Research, 1253, 117–128.PubMed
go back to reference Borghi, A. M., & Riggio, L. (2015). Stable and variable affordances are both automatic and flexible. Frontiers in Human Neuroscience, 9, 351.PubMed Borghi, A. M., & Riggio, L. (2015). Stable and variable affordances are both automatic and flexible. Frontiers in Human Neuroscience, 9, 351.PubMed
go back to reference Bub, D. N., & Masson, M. E. (2010). Grasping beer mugs: on the dynamics of alignment effects induced by handled objects. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 36(2), 341–358.PubMed Bub, D. N., & Masson, M. E. (2010). Grasping beer mugs: on the dynamics of alignment effects induced by handled objects. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 36(2), 341–358.PubMed
go back to reference Bub, D. N., Masson, M. E., & Kumar, R. (2018). Time course of motor affordances evoked by pictured objects and words. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 44(1), 53–68.PubMed Bub, D. N., Masson, M. E., & Kumar, R. (2018). Time course of motor affordances evoked by pictured objects and words. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 44(1), 53–68.PubMed
go back to reference Buckingham, G., Main, J. C., & Carey, D. P. (2011). Asymmetries in motor attention during a cued bimanual reaching task: Left and right handers compared. Cortex, 47, 432–440.PubMed Buckingham, G., Main, J. C., & Carey, D. P. (2011). Asymmetries in motor attention during a cued bimanual reaching task: Left and right handers compared. Cortex, 47, 432–440.PubMed
go back to reference Buetti, S., & Kerzel, D. (2010). Effects of saccades and response type on the Simon effect: If you look at the stimulus, the Simon effect may be gone. The Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology, 63(11), 2172–2189.PubMed Buetti, S., & Kerzel, D. (2010). Effects of saccades and response type on the Simon effect: If you look at the stimulus, the Simon effect may be gone. The Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology, 63(11), 2172–2189.PubMed
go back to reference Buhlmann, I., Umiltà, C., & Wascher, E. (2007). Response coding and visuomotor transformation in the Simon task: The role of action goals. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 33(6), 1269–1282.PubMed Buhlmann, I., Umiltà, C., & Wascher, E. (2007). Response coding and visuomotor transformation in the Simon task: The role of action goals. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 33(6), 1269–1282.PubMed
go back to reference Cho, D. T., & Proctor, R. W. (2010). The object-based Simon effect: grasping affordance or relative location of the graspable part? Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 36(4), 853–861.PubMed Cho, D. T., & Proctor, R. W. (2010). The object-based Simon effect: grasping affordance or relative location of the graspable part? Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 36(4), 853–861.PubMed
go back to reference Cho, D. T., & Proctor, R. W. (2011). Correspondence effects for objects with opposing left and right protrusions. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 37(3), 737.PubMed Cho, D. T., & Proctor, R. W. (2011). Correspondence effects for objects with opposing left and right protrusions. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 37(3), 737.PubMed
go back to reference Cho, D. T., & Proctor, R. W. (2013). Object-based correspondence effects for action-relevant and surface-property judgments with keypress responses: Evidence for a basis in spatial coding. Psychological Research Psychologische Forschung, 77(5), 618–636.PubMed Cho, D. T., & Proctor, R. W. (2013). Object-based correspondence effects for action-relevant and surface-property judgments with keypress responses: Evidence for a basis in spatial coding. Psychological Research Psychologische Forschung, 77(5), 618–636.PubMed
go back to reference Dagaev, N., Shtyrov, Y., & Myachykov, A. (2017). The role of executive control in the activation of manual affordances. Psychological Research Psychologische Forschung, 81(6), 1110–1124.PubMed Dagaev, N., Shtyrov, Y., & Myachykov, A. (2017). The role of executive control in the activation of manual affordances. Psychological Research Psychologische Forschung, 81(6), 1110–1124.PubMed
go back to reference De Jong, R., Liang, C.-C., & Lauber, E. (1994). Conditional and unconditional automaticity: A dual-process model of effects of spatial stimulus-response correspondence. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception & Performance, 20, 731–750. De Jong, R., Liang, C.-C., & Lauber, E. (1994). Conditional and unconditional automaticity: A dual-process model of effects of spatial stimulus-response correspondence. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception & Performance, 20, 731–750.
go back to reference Donk, M., Agter, F., & Pratt, J. (2009). Effects of luminance change in preview search: Offsets and onsets can be concurrently prioritized but not in isolation. Acta Psychologica, 130(3), 260–267.PubMed Donk, M., Agter, F., & Pratt, J. (2009). Effects of luminance change in preview search: Offsets and onsets can be concurrently prioritized but not in isolation. Acta Psychologica, 130(3), 260–267.PubMed
go back to reference Foulsham, T., & Kingstone, A. (2013). Optimal and preferred eye landing positions in objects and scenes. The Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology, 66(9), 1707–1728.PubMed Foulsham, T., & Kingstone, A. (2013). Optimal and preferred eye landing positions in objects and scenes. The Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology, 66(9), 1707–1728.PubMed
go back to reference Foulsham, T., & Underwood, G. (2009). Does conspicuity enhance distraction? Saliency and eye landing position when searching for objects. The Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology, 62(6), 1088–1098.PubMed Foulsham, T., & Underwood, G. (2009). Does conspicuity enhance distraction? Saliency and eye landing position when searching for objects. The Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology, 62(6), 1088–1098.PubMed
go back to reference Franconeri, S. L., Hollingworth, A., & Simons, D. J. (2005). Do new objects capture attention? Psychological Science: A Journal of the American Psychological Society/APS, 16(4), 275–281. Franconeri, S. L., Hollingworth, A., & Simons, D. J. (2005). Do new objects capture attention? Psychological Science: A Journal of the American Psychological Society/APS, 16(4), 275–281.
go back to reference Gellatly, A., Cole, G., & Blurton, A. (1999). Do equiluminant object onsets capture visual attention? Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 25(6), 1609–1624.PubMed Gellatly, A., Cole, G., & Blurton, A. (1999). Do equiluminant object onsets capture visual attention? Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 25(6), 1609–1624.PubMed
go back to reference Gibson, J. J. (1979). The ecological approach to visual perception. Boston: Houghton Mifflin. Gibson, J. J. (1979). The ecological approach to visual perception. Boston: Houghton Mifflin.
go back to reference Goslin, J., Dixon, T., Fischer, M. H., Cangelosi, A., & Ellis, R. (2012). Electrophysiological examination of embodiment in vision and action. Psychological Science, 23(2), 152–157.PubMed Goslin, J., Dixon, T., Fischer, M. H., Cangelosi, A., & Ellis, R. (2012). Electrophysiological examination of embodiment in vision and action. Psychological Science, 23(2), 152–157.PubMed
go back to reference Heister, G., Ehrenstein, W. H., & Schroeder-Heister, P. (1987). Spatial S-R compatibility with unimanual two-finger choice reactions: Effects of irrelevant stimulus location. Perception & Psychophysics, 42, 195–201. Heister, G., Ehrenstein, W. H., & Schroeder-Heister, P. (1987). Spatial S-R compatibility with unimanual two-finger choice reactions: Effects of irrelevant stimulus location. Perception & Psychophysics, 42, 195–201.
go back to reference Henderson, J. M. (1993). Eye movement control during visual object processing: effects of initial fixation position and semantic constraint. Canadian Journal of Experimental Psychology/Revue canadienne de psychologie expérimentale, 47(1), 79.PubMed Henderson, J. M. (1993). Eye movement control during visual object processing: effects of initial fixation position and semantic constraint. Canadian Journal of Experimental Psychology/Revue canadienne de psychologie expérimentale, 47(1), 79.PubMed
go back to reference Hommel, B. (1993). The role of attention for the Simon effect. Psychological Research Psychologische Forschung, 55(3), 208–222.PubMed Hommel, B. (1993). The role of attention for the Simon effect. Psychological Research Psychologische Forschung, 55(3), 208–222.PubMed
go back to reference Hommel, B. (1994). Spontaneous decay of response-code activation. Psychological Research Psychologische Forschung, 56(4), 261–268.PubMed Hommel, B. (1994). Spontaneous decay of response-code activation. Psychological Research Psychologische Forschung, 56(4), 261–268.PubMed
go back to reference Iani, C., Baroni, G., Pellicano, A., & Nicoletti, R. (2011). On the relationship between Affordance and Simon effects: Are the effects really independent? Journal of Cognitive Psychology, 23, 121–131. Iani, C., Baroni, G., Pellicano, A., & Nicoletti, R. (2011). On the relationship between Affordance and Simon effects: Are the effects really independent? Journal of Cognitive Psychology, 23, 121–131.
go back to reference Johnson, C. J., Paivio, A., & Clark, J. M. (1996). Cognitive components of picture naming. Psychological Bulletin, 120(1), 113–139.PubMed Johnson, C. J., Paivio, A., & Clark, J. M. (1996). Cognitive components of picture naming. Psychological Bulletin, 120(1), 113–139.PubMed
go back to reference Koch, C., & Ullman, S. (1985). Shifts in selective visual attention: Towards the underlying neural circuitry. Human Neurobiology, 4, 219–227.PubMed Koch, C., & Ullman, S. (1985). Shifts in selective visual attention: Towards the underlying neural circuitry. Human Neurobiology, 4, 219–227.PubMed
go back to reference Kornblum, S., Hasbroucq, T., & Osman, A. (1990). Dimensional overlap: cognitive basis for S-R compatibility—A model and a taxonomy. Psychological Review, 97, 253–270.PubMed Kornblum, S., Hasbroucq, T., & Osman, A. (1990). Dimensional overlap: cognitive basis for S-R compatibility—A model and a taxonomy. Psychological Review, 97, 253–270.PubMed
go back to reference Kornblum, S., Stevens, G. T., Whipple, A., & Requin, J. (1999). The effects of irrelevant stimuli: 1. The time course of stimulus–stimulus and stimulus–response consistency effects with Stroop-like stimuli, Simon-like tasks, and their factorial combinations. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 25(3), 688–714. Kornblum, S., Stevens, G. T., Whipple, A., & Requin, J. (1999). The effects of irrelevant stimuli: 1. The time course of stimulus–stimulus and stimulus–response consistency effects with Stroop-like stimuli, Simon-like tasks, and their factorial combinations. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 25(3), 688–714.
go back to reference Kostov, K., & Janyan, A. (2015). Reversing the affordance effect: negative stimulus–response compatibility observed with images of graspable objects. Cognitive Processing, 16(1), 287–291.PubMed Kostov, K., & Janyan, A. (2015). Reversing the affordance effect: negative stimulus–response compatibility observed with images of graspable objects. Cognitive Processing, 16(1), 287–291.PubMed
go back to reference Lien, M. C., Gray, D., Jardin, E., & Proctor, R. W. (2014). Further evidence that object-based correspondence effects are primarily modulated by object location not by grasping affordance. Journal of Cognitive Psychology, 26(6), 679–698. Lien, M. C., Gray, D., Jardin, E., & Proctor, R. W. (2014). Further evidence that object-based correspondence effects are primarily modulated by object location not by grasping affordance. Journal of Cognitive Psychology, 26(6), 679–698.
go back to reference Lien, M. C., Jardin, E., & Proctor, R. W. (2013). An electrophysiological study of the object-based correspondence effect: Is the effect triggered by an intended grasping action? Attention, Perception, & Psychophysics, 75, 1862–1882. Lien, M. C., Jardin, E., & Proctor, R. W. (2013). An electrophysiological study of the object-based correspondence effect: Is the effect triggered by an intended grasping action? Attention, Perception, & Psychophysics, 75, 1862–1882.
go back to reference Lu, C. H., & Proctor, R. W. (1995). The influence of irrelevant location information on performance: A review of the Simon and spatial Stroop effects. Psychonomic Bulletin and Review, 2, 174–207.PubMed Lu, C. H., & Proctor, R. W. (1995). The influence of irrelevant location information on performance: A review of the Simon and spatial Stroop effects. Psychonomic Bulletin and Review, 2, 174–207.PubMed
go back to reference Lugli, L., Baroni, G., Nicoletti, R., & Umiltà, C. (2016). The simon effect with saccadic eye movements. Experimental Psychology, 63(2), 107–116.PubMed Lugli, L., Baroni, G., Nicoletti, R., & Umiltà, C. (2016). The simon effect with saccadic eye movements. Experimental Psychology, 63(2), 107–116.PubMed
go back to reference Masson, M. E. (2018). Intentions and actions. Canadian Journal of Experimental Psychology/Revue canadienne de psychologie expérimentale, 72(4), 219.PubMed Masson, M. E. (2018). Intentions and actions. Canadian Journal of Experimental Psychology/Revue canadienne de psychologie expérimentale, 72(4), 219.PubMed
go back to reference Matheson, H., Newman, A. J., Satel, J., & McMullen, P. (2014). Handles of manipulable objects attract covert visual attention: ERP evidence. Brain and Cognition, 86, 17–23.PubMed Matheson, H., Newman, A. J., Satel, J., & McMullen, P. (2014). Handles of manipulable objects attract covert visual attention: ERP evidence. Brain and Cognition, 86, 17–23.PubMed
go back to reference Matheson, H., & Thompson-Schill, S. (2019). Investigating grounded conceptualization: Stimulus-response compatibility for tool handles is due to spatial attention. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 45(4), 441–457.PubMed Matheson, H., & Thompson-Schill, S. (2019). Investigating grounded conceptualization: Stimulus-response compatibility for tool handles is due to spatial attention. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 45(4), 441–457.PubMed
go back to reference Myachykov, A., Ellis, R., Cangelosi, A., & Fischer, M. H. (2013). Visual and linguistic cues to graspable objects. Experimental Brain Research, 229(4), 545–559.PubMed Myachykov, A., Ellis, R., Cangelosi, A., & Fischer, M. H. (2013). Visual and linguistic cues to graspable objects. Experimental Brain Research, 229(4), 545–559.PubMed
go back to reference Nicoletti, R., & Umiltà, C. (1989). Splitting visual space with attention. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception & Performance, 15, 164–169. Nicoletti, R., & Umiltà, C. (1989). Splitting visual space with attention. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception & Performance, 15, 164–169.
go back to reference Nicoletti, R., & Umiltà, C. (1994). Attention shifts produce spatial stimulus codes. Psychological Research Psychologische Forschung, 56, 144–150.PubMed Nicoletti, R., & Umiltà, C. (1994). Attention shifts produce spatial stimulus codes. Psychological Research Psychologische Forschung, 56, 144–150.PubMed
go back to reference Notebaert, W., Soetens, E., & Melis, A. (2001). Sequential analysis of a Simon task–evidence for an attention-shift account. Psychological Research Psychologische Forschung, 65(3), 170–184.PubMed Notebaert, W., Soetens, E., & Melis, A. (2001). Sequential analysis of a Simon task–evidence for an attention-shift account. Psychological Research Psychologische Forschung, 65(3), 170–184.PubMed
go back to reference Nothdurft, H. C. (2000). Salience from feature contrast: additivity across dimensions. Vision Research, 40, 1183–1201.PubMed Nothdurft, H. C. (2000). Salience from feature contrast: additivity across dimensions. Vision Research, 40, 1183–1201.PubMed
go back to reference Nothdurft, H. C. (2002). Attention shifts to salient targets. Vision Research, 42, 1287–1306.PubMed Nothdurft, H. C. (2002). Attention shifts to salient targets. Vision Research, 42, 1287–1306.PubMed
go back to reference Nothdurft, H. C. (2006). Salience and target selection in visual search. Visual Cognition, 14(4–8), 514–542. Nothdurft, H. C. (2006). Salience and target selection in visual search. Visual Cognition, 14(4–8), 514–542.
go back to reference Nuthmann, A., & Henderson, J. M. (2010). Object-based attentional selection in scene viewing. Journal of Vision, 10(8), 1–19. Nuthmann, A., & Henderson, J. M. (2010). Object-based attentional selection in scene viewing. Journal of Vision, 10(8), 1–19.
go back to reference Osiurak, F., & Badets, A. (2016). Tool use and affordance: Manipulation-based versus reasoning-based approaches. Psychological Review, 123(5), 534.PubMed Osiurak, F., & Badets, A. (2016). Tool use and affordance: Manipulation-based versus reasoning-based approaches. Psychological Review, 123(5), 534.PubMed
go back to reference Ottoboni, G., Iani, C., Tessari, A., & Rubichi, S. (2013). Modulation of the affordance effect through transfer of learning. The Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology, 66(12), 2295–2302.PubMed Ottoboni, G., Iani, C., Tessari, A., & Rubichi, S. (2013). Modulation of the affordance effect through transfer of learning. The Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology, 66(12), 2295–2302.PubMed
go back to reference Pajak, M., & Nuthmann, A. (2013). Object-based saccadic selection during scene perception: Evidence from viewing position effects. Journal of Vision, 13(5), 1–21. Pajak, M., & Nuthmann, A. (2013). Object-based saccadic selection during scene perception: Evidence from viewing position effects. Journal of Vision, 13(5), 1–21.
go back to reference Pappas, Z. (2014). Dissociating Simon and affordance compatibility effects: Silhouettes and photographs. Cognition, 133(3), 716–728.PubMed Pappas, Z. (2014). Dissociating Simon and affordance compatibility effects: Silhouettes and photographs. Cognition, 133(3), 716–728.PubMed
go back to reference Pellicano, A., Iani, C., Borghi, A. M., Rubichi, S., & Nicoletti, R. (2010). Simon-like and functional affordance effects with tools: The effects of object perceptual discrimination and object action state. Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology, 63(11), 2190–2201. Pellicano, A., Iani, C., Borghi, A. M., Rubichi, S., & Nicoletti, R. (2010). Simon-like and functional affordance effects with tools: The effects of object perceptual discrimination and object action state. Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology, 63(11), 2190–2201.
go back to reference Pellicano, A., Koch, I., & Binkofski, F. (2017). Location-coding account versus affordance-activation account in handle-to-hand correspondence effects: Evidence of Simon-like effects based on the coding of action direction. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 43(9), 1647.PubMed Pellicano, A., Koch, I., & Binkofski, F. (2017). Location-coding account versus affordance-activation account in handle-to-hand correspondence effects: Evidence of Simon-like effects based on the coding of action direction. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 43(9), 1647.PubMed
go back to reference Pellicano, A., Lugli, L., Baroni, G., & Nicoletti, R. (2009). The Simon effect with conventional signals: A time-course analysis. Experimental Psychology, 56(4), 219–227.PubMed Pellicano, A., Lugli, L., Baroni, G., & Nicoletti, R. (2009). The Simon effect with conventional signals: A time-course analysis. Experimental Psychology, 56(4), 219–227.PubMed
go back to reference Pellicano, A., Lugli, L., Binkofski, F., Rubichi, S., Iani, C., & Nicoletti, R. (2019). The unimanual handle-to-hand correspondence effect: evidence for a location coding account. Psychological Research, 83(7), 1383–1399.PubMed Pellicano, A., Lugli, L., Binkofski, F., Rubichi, S., Iani, C., & Nicoletti, R. (2019). The unimanual handle-to-hand correspondence effect: evidence for a location coding account. Psychological Research, 83(7), 1383–1399.PubMed
go back to reference Phillips, J. C., & Ward, R. (2002). S-R correspondence effects of irrelevant visual affordance: Timecourse and specificity of response activation. Visual Cognition, 9(4–5), 540–558. Phillips, J. C., & Ward, R. (2002). S-R correspondence effects of irrelevant visual affordance: Timecourse and specificity of response activation. Visual Cognition, 9(4–5), 540–558.
go back to reference Proctor, R. W., Lien, M. C., & Thompson, L. (2017). Do silhouettes and photographs produce fundamentally different object-based correspondence effects? Cognition, 169, 91–101.PubMed Proctor, R. W., Lien, M. C., & Thompson, L. (2017). Do silhouettes and photographs produce fundamentally different object-based correspondence effects? Cognition, 169, 91–101.PubMed
go back to reference Proctor, R. W., & Miles, J. D. (2014). Does the concept of affordance add anything to explanations of stimulus–response compatibility effects. The Psychology of Learning and Motivation, 60, 227–266. Proctor, R. W., & Miles, J. D. (2014). Does the concept of affordance add anything to explanations of stimulus–response compatibility effects. The Psychology of Learning and Motivation, 60, 227–266.
go back to reference Proctor, R. W., & Vu, K.-P. L. (2006). Stimulus-response compatibility principle: data, theory, and application. Boca Raton: Taylor & Francis. Proctor, R. W., & Vu, K.-P. L. (2006). Stimulus-response compatibility principle: data, theory, and application. Boca Raton: Taylor & Francis.
go back to reference Ratcliff, R. (1979). Group reaction time distributions and an analysis of distribution statistics. Psychological Bulletin, 86, 446–461.PubMed Ratcliff, R. (1979). Group reaction time distributions and an analysis of distribution statistics. Psychological Bulletin, 86, 446–461.PubMed
go back to reference Riggio, L., Iani, C., Gherri, E., Benatti, F., Rubichi, S., & Nicoletti, R. (2008). The role of attention in the occurrence of the affordance effect. Acta Psychologica, 127, 449–458.PubMed Riggio, L., Iani, C., Gherri, E., Benatti, F., Rubichi, S., & Nicoletti, R. (2008). The role of attention in the occurrence of the affordance effect. Acta Psychologica, 127, 449–458.PubMed
go back to reference Roberts, K. L., & Humphreys, G. W. (2011). Action-related objects influence the distribution of visuospatial attention. The Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology, 64(4), 669–688.PubMed Roberts, K. L., & Humphreys, G. W. (2011). Action-related objects influence the distribution of visuospatial attention. The Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology, 64(4), 669–688.PubMed
go back to reference Roswarski, T. E., & Proctor, R. W. (1996). Multiple spatial codes and temporal overlap in choice-reaction tasks. Psychological Research Psychologische Forschung, 59(3), 196–211. Roswarski, T. E., & Proctor, R. W. (1996). Multiple spatial codes and temporal overlap in choice-reaction tasks. Psychological Research Psychologische Forschung, 59(3), 196–211.
go back to reference Rubichi, S., Nicoletti, R., Iani, C., & Umiltà, C. (1997). The Simon effect occurs relative to the direction of an attention shift. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 23, 1353–1364.PubMed Rubichi, S., Nicoletti, R., Iani, C., & Umiltà, C. (1997). The Simon effect occurs relative to the direction of an attention shift. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 23, 1353–1364.PubMed
go back to reference Ruz, M., & Lupiáñez, J. (2002). A review of attentional capture: On its automaticity and sensitivity to endogenous control. Psicológica, 23(2), 283–309. Ruz, M., & Lupiáñez, J. (2002). A review of attentional capture: On its automaticity and sensitivity to endogenous control. Psicológica, 23(2), 283–309.
go back to reference Saccone, E. J., Churches, O. F., & Nicholls, M. E. (2016). Explicit spatial compatility is not critical to the object handle effect. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 42(10), 1643–1653.PubMed Saccone, E. J., Churches, O. F., & Nicholls, M. E. (2016). Explicit spatial compatility is not critical to the object handle effect. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 42(10), 1643–1653.PubMed
go back to reference Schneider, W., Eschman, A., & Zuccolotto, A. (2002). E-Prime user’s guide. Pittsburgh: Psychology Software Tools Inc. Schneider, W., Eschman, A., & Zuccolotto, A. (2002). E-Prime user’s guide. Pittsburgh: Psychology Software Tools Inc.
go back to reference Simon, J. R. (1990). The effects of an irrelevant directional cue on human information processing. In R. W. Proctor & T. G. Reeve (Eds.), Stimulus–response compatibility: An integrated perspective. Amsterdam: Elsevier. Simon, J. R. (1990). The effects of an irrelevant directional cue on human information processing. In R. W. Proctor & T. G. Reeve (Eds.), Stimulus–response compatibility: An integrated perspective. Amsterdam: Elsevier.
go back to reference Song, X., Chen, J., & Proctor, R. W. (2014). Correspondence effects with torches: Grasping affordance or visual feature asymmetry? The Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology, 67(4), 665–675.PubMed Song, X., Chen, J., & Proctor, R. W. (2014). Correspondence effects with torches: Grasping affordance or visual feature asymmetry? The Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology, 67(4), 665–675.PubMed
go back to reference Symes, E., Ellis, R., & Tucker, M. (2005). Dissociating object-based and space-based affordances. Visual Cognition, 12, 1337–1360. Symes, E., Ellis, R., & Tucker, M. (2005). Dissociating object-based and space-based affordances. Visual Cognition, 12, 1337–1360.
go back to reference Theeuwes, J. (1995). Abrupt luminance change pops out; abrupt color change does not. Perception & Psychophysics, 57(5), 637–644. Theeuwes, J. (1995). Abrupt luminance change pops out; abrupt color change does not. Perception & Psychophysics, 57(5), 637–644.
go back to reference Theeuwes, J. (2010). Top–down and bottom–up control of visual selection. Acta Psychologica, 135(2), 77–99.PubMed Theeuwes, J. (2010). Top–down and bottom–up control of visual selection. Acta Psychologica, 135(2), 77–99.PubMed
go back to reference Thill, S., Caligiore, D., Borghi, A. M., Ziemke, T., & Baldassarre, G. (2013). Theories and computational models of affordance and mirror systems: an integrative review. Neuroscience & Biobehavioral Reviews, 37(3), 491–521. Thill, S., Caligiore, D., Borghi, A. M., Ziemke, T., & Baldassarre, G. (2013). Theories and computational models of affordance and mirror systems: an integrative review. Neuroscience & Biobehavioral Reviews, 37(3), 491–521.
go back to reference Tipper, S. P., Paul, M. A., & Hayes, A. E. (2006). Vision-for-action: The effects of object property discrimination and action state on affordance compatibility effects. Psychonomic Bulletin & Review, 13(3), 493–498. Tipper, S. P., Paul, M. A., & Hayes, A. E. (2006). Vision-for-action: The effects of object property discrimination and action state on affordance compatibility effects. Psychonomic Bulletin & Review, 13(3), 493–498.
go back to reference Treisman, A. M., & Gelade, G. (1980). A feature-integration theory of attention. Cognitive Psychology, 12(1), 97–136.PubMed Treisman, A. M., & Gelade, G. (1980). A feature-integration theory of attention. Cognitive Psychology, 12(1), 97–136.PubMed
go back to reference Tucker, M., & Ellis, R. (1998). On the relations between seen objects and components of potential actions. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 24(3), 830–846.PubMed Tucker, M., & Ellis, R. (1998). On the relations between seen objects and components of potential actions. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 24(3), 830–846.PubMed
go back to reference Tucker, M., & Ellis, R. (2001). The potentiation of grasp types during visual object categorization. Visual Cognition, 8(6), 769–800. Tucker, M., & Ellis, R. (2001). The potentiation of grasp types during visual object categorization. Visual Cognition, 8(6), 769–800.
go back to reference Turatto, M., & Galfano, G. (2000). Color, form and luminance capture attention in visual search. Vision Research, 40(13), 1639–1643.PubMed Turatto, M., & Galfano, G. (2000). Color, form and luminance capture attention in visual search. Vision Research, 40(13), 1639–1643.PubMed
go back to reference Umiltà, C., & Nicoletti, R. (1985). Attention and coding effects in S-R compatibility due to irrelevant spatial cues. In M. I. Posner & O. S. M. Marin (Eds.), Attention and Performance XI (pp. 457–471). Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum. Umiltà, C., & Nicoletti, R. (1985). Attention and coding effects in S-R compatibility due to irrelevant spatial cues. In M. I. Posner & O. S. M. Marin (Eds.), Attention and Performance XI (pp. 457–471). Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum.
go back to reference van der Linden, L., Mathôt, S., & Vitu, F. (2015). The role of object affordances and center of gravity in eye movements toward isolated daily-life objects. Journal of Vision, 15(5), 1–18. van der Linden, L., Mathôt, S., & Vitu, F. (2015). The role of object affordances and center of gravity in eye movements toward isolated daily-life objects. Journal of Vision, 15(5), 1–18.
go back to reference Wallace, R. J. (1971). S-R compatibility and the idea of a response code. Journal of Experimental Psychology, 88, 354–360.PubMed Wallace, R. J. (1971). S-R compatibility and the idea of a response code. Journal of Experimental Psychology, 88, 354–360.PubMed
go back to reference Wascher, E., Schatz, U., Kuder, T., & Verleger, R. (2001). Validity and boundary conditions of automatic response activation in the Simon task. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 27(3), 731–751.PubMed Wascher, E., Schatz, U., Kuder, T., & Verleger, R. (2001). Validity and boundary conditions of automatic response activation in the Simon task. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 27(3), 731–751.PubMed
go back to reference Wiegand, K., & Wascher, E. (2007). Response coding in the Simon task. Psychological Research Psychologische Forschung, 71(4), 401–410.PubMed Wiegand, K., & Wascher, E. (2007). Response coding in the Simon task. Psychological Research Psychologische Forschung, 71(4), 401–410.PubMed
go back to reference Wolfe, J. M. (1998). Visual search. In H. Pashler (Ed.), Attention (pp. 13–73). Hove: Psychology Press. Wolfe, J. M. (1998). Visual search. In H. Pashler (Ed.), Attention (pp. 13–73). Hove: Psychology Press.
go back to reference Wolfe, J. M., & Horowitz, T. S. (2004). What attributes guide the deployment of visual attention and how do they do it? Nature reviews neuroscience, 5(6), 495.PubMed Wolfe, J. M., & Horowitz, T. S. (2004). What attributes guide the deployment of visual attention and how do they do it? Nature reviews neuroscience, 5(6), 495.PubMed
go back to reference Xiong, A., Proctor, R. W., & Zelaznik, H. N. (2019). Visual salience, not the graspable part of a pictured eating utensil, grabs attention. Attention, Perception, & Psychophysics, 81(5), 1454–1463. Xiong, A., Proctor, R. W., & Zelaznik, H. N. (2019). Visual salience, not the graspable part of a pictured eating utensil, grabs attention. Attention, Perception, & Psychophysics, 81(5), 1454–1463.
go back to reference Yantis, S. (1993). Stimulus-driven attentional capture and attentional control settings. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 19, 676–681.PubMed Yantis, S. (1993). Stimulus-driven attentional capture and attentional control settings. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 19, 676–681.PubMed
go back to reference Yantis, S., & Jonides, J. (1984). Abrupt visual onsets and selective attention: evidence from visual search. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human perception and performance, 10(5), 601–621.PubMed Yantis, S., & Jonides, J. (1984). Abrupt visual onsets and selective attention: evidence from visual search. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human perception and performance, 10(5), 601–621.PubMed
go back to reference Yantis, S., & Jonides, J. (1996). Attentional capture by abrupt onsets: New perceptual objects or visual masking? Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 22(6), 1505–1513.PubMed Yantis, S., & Jonides, J. (1996). Attentional capture by abrupt onsets: New perceptual objects or visual masking? Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 22(6), 1505–1513.PubMed
go back to reference Yu, A. B., Abrams, R. A., & Zacks, J. M. (2014). Limits on action priming by pictures of objects. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 40(5), 1861–1873.PubMed Yu, A. B., Abrams, R. A., & Zacks, J. M. (2014). Limits on action priming by pictures of objects. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 40(5), 1861–1873.PubMed
go back to reference Zhao, Q., & Koch, C. (2013). Learning saliency-based visual attention: A review. Signal Processing, 93(6), 1401–1407. Zhao, Q., & Koch, C. (2013). Learning saliency-based visual attention: A review. Signal Processing, 93(6), 1401–1407.
go back to reference Zorzi, M., Mapelli, D., Rusconi, E., & Umiltà, C. (2003). Automatic spatial coding of perceived gaze direction is revealed by the Simon effect. Psychonomic Bulletin & Review, 10(2), 423–429. Zorzi, M., Mapelli, D., Rusconi, E., & Umiltà, C. (2003). Automatic spatial coding of perceived gaze direction is revealed by the Simon effect. Psychonomic Bulletin & Review, 10(2), 423–429.
Metagegevens
Titel
Critical bottom-up attentional factors in the handle orientation effect: asymmetric luminance transients and object-center eccentricity relative to fixation
Auteurs
Kiril Kostov
Armina Janyan
Publicatiedatum
04-04-2020
Uitgeverij
Springer Berlin Heidelberg
Gepubliceerd in
Psychological Research / Uitgave 4/2021
Print ISSN: 0340-0727
Elektronisch ISSN: 1430-2772
DOI
https://doi.org/10.1007/s00426-020-01329-w

Andere artikelen Uitgave 4/2021

Psychological Research 4/2021 Naar de uitgave