Skip to main content
Top
Gepubliceerd in: Psychological Research 8/2019

14-06-2018 | Original Article

Deep, effortless concentration: re-examining the flow concept and exploring relations with inattention, absorption, and personality

Auteurs: Jeremy Marty-Dugas, Daniel Smilek

Gepubliceerd in: Psychological Research | Uitgave 8/2019

Log in om toegang te krijgen
share
DELEN

Deel dit onderdeel of sectie (kopieer de link)

  • Optie A:
    Klik op de rechtermuisknop op de link en selecteer de optie “linkadres kopiëren”
  • Optie B:
    Deel de link per e-mail

Abstract

Conceptualizing the construct of flow in terms of ‘deep and effortless concentration’, we developed two measurement scales designed to index individual differences in flow during ‘internal’ tasks, such as thinking (deep effortless concentration: internal—DECI) and during ‘external’ tasks, such as while playing a sport (deep effortless concentration: external—DECE). These scales were highly correlated, indicating that individuals prone to experiencing flow in external contexts are also prone to experience flow in internal contexts. Nonetheless, a measurement model construing internal and external flow as related, but separate, constructs was found to fit the data significantly better than a model where they were construed as a single construct. We then explored associations between flow and various forms of everyday inattention. In addition, we explored the relation between flow and the Tellegen Absorption Scale (TAS), an index of absorption, as well as the Big Five personality traits. Amongst other things, we found that flow was negatively related to inattention, indicating that people who experience flow more frequently may experience relatively less inattention in everyday contexts.
Bijlagen
Alleen toegankelijk voor geautoriseerde gebruikers
Voetnoten
1
These conditions are often referred to as the ‘flow’, ‘boredom’, and ‘anxiety’ conditions, respectively, but we find that the terms optimal balance, underload, and overload are less ambiguous about the level of skill-challenge balance.
 
2
Of the original nine items, three were selected for the flow index. These were “I would love to solve math calculations of that kind again” “I was thrilled” and “Task demands were well matched to my ability”. The same items were also used in the 2016 paper.
 
3
For example items from an earlier version of the DFS-2, see Jackson & Marsh, 1996.
 
4
See Tellegen & Atkinson, 1974 for some example items.
 
5
SFPQ assesses seven facets using one item each (with three subscales to measure flow in different contexts), while the DFS-2 assess nine facets using four items per facet.
 
6
Four participants listed the same value for their age and gender (e.g., age = 29, gender = 29), and one participant marked an ‘x’. As gender was not a variable of interest for the present study, no participants were removed from analysis on the basis of gender.
 
7
Skew = 0.94. Kurtosis = 0.24.
 
8
We ran several previous iterations of this experiment during the development of the items for the internal and external flow scales. Each iteration varied in some way, either with the content of the items, the instructions, or the number of other questionnaires participants were presented with, before we finalized the items and instructions for the present study.
 
9
This is considerably more problematic for a trait scale, where individuals could experience each facet in entirely different contexts. For example, a frequent experience of clear goals at work, a frequent loss of self-consciousness while driving, and action-awareness merging while doodling.
 
Literatuur
go back to reference Aherne, C., Moran, A. P., & Lonsdale, C. (2011). The effect of mindfulness training on athletes’ flow: an initial investigation. The Sport Psychologist, 25(2), 177–189.CrossRef Aherne, C., Moran, A. P., & Lonsdale, C. (2011). The effect of mindfulness training on athletes’ flow: an initial investigation. The Sport Psychologist, 25(2), 177–189.CrossRef
go back to reference Arbuckle, J. L. (2016). Amos (Version 24.0) (Computer Program). Chicago: IBM SPSS. Arbuckle, J. L. (2016). Amos (Version 24.0) (Computer Program). Chicago: IBM SPSS.
go back to reference Asakawa, K. (2010). Flow experience, culture, and well-being: How do autotelic Japanese college students feel, behave, and think in their daily lives? Journal of Happiness Studies, 11(2), 205–223.CrossRef Asakawa, K. (2010). Flow experience, culture, and well-being: How do autotelic Japanese college students feel, behave, and think in their daily lives? Journal of Happiness Studies, 11(2), 205–223.CrossRef
go back to reference Bishop, S. R., Lau, M., Shapiro, S., Carlson, L., Anderson, N. D., Carmody, J., Devins, G. (2004). Mindfulness: a proposed operational definition. Clinical Psychology: Science and Practice, 11(3), 230–241. Bishop, S. R., Lau, M., Shapiro, S., Carlson, L., Anderson, N. D., Carmody, J., Devins, G. (2004). Mindfulness: a proposed operational definition. Clinical Psychology: Science and Practice, 11(3), 230–241.
go back to reference Brown, K. W., & Ryan, R. M. (2003). The benefits of being present: mindfulness and its role in psychological well-being. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 84(4), 822.CrossRefPubMed Brown, K. W., & Ryan, R. M. (2003). The benefits of being present: mindfulness and its role in psychological well-being. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 84(4), 822.CrossRefPubMed
go back to reference Carriere, J. S. A., Seli, P., & Smilek, D. (2013). Wandering in both mind and body: individual differences in mind wandering and inattention predict fidgeting. Canadian Journal of Experimental Psychology/Revue Canadienne De Psychologie Expérimentale, 67(1), 19–31. https://doi.org/10.1037/a0031438.CrossRefPubMed Carriere, J. S. A., Seli, P., & Smilek, D. (2013). Wandering in both mind and body: individual differences in mind wandering and inattention predict fidgeting. Canadian Journal of Experimental Psychology/Revue Canadienne De Psychologie Expérimentale, 67(1), 19–31. https://​doi.​org/​10.​1037/​a0031438.CrossRefPubMed
go back to reference Catley, D.E.L.W.Y.N., & Duda, J.L. (1997). Psychological antecedents of the frequency and intensity of flow in golfers. International Journal of Sport Psychology, 28(4), 309–322. Catley, D.E.L.W.Y.N., & Duda, J.L. (1997). Psychological antecedents of the frequency and intensity of flow in golfers. International Journal of Sport Psychology, 28(4), 309–322.
go back to reference Cermakova, L., Moneta, G.B., & Spada, M.M. (2010). Dispositional flow as a mediator of the relationships between attentional control and approaches to studying during academic examination preparation. Educational Psychology, 30(5), 495–511.CrossRef Cermakova, L., Moneta, G.B., & Spada, M.M. (2010). Dispositional flow as a mediator of the relationships between attentional control and approaches to studying during academic examination preparation. Educational Psychology, 30(5), 495–511.CrossRef
go back to reference Costa, P. T., & McCrae, R. R. (2008). The revised neo personality inventory (neo-pi-r). The SAGE Handbook of Personality Theory and Assessment, 2, 179–198. Costa, P. T., & McCrae, R. R. (2008). The revised neo personality inventory (neo-pi-r). The SAGE Handbook of Personality Theory and Assessment, 2, 179–198.
go back to reference Crawford, H. J., Brown, A. M., & Moon, C. E. (1993). Sustained attentional and disattentional abilities: differences between low and highly hypnotizable persons. Journal of Abnormal Psychology, 102(4), 534.CrossRefPubMed Crawford, H. J., Brown, A. M., & Moon, C. E. (1993). Sustained attentional and disattentional abilities: differences between low and highly hypnotizable persons. Journal of Abnormal Psychology, 102(4), 534.CrossRefPubMed
go back to reference Crust, L., & Swann, C. (2013). The relationship between mental toughness and dispositional flow. European Journal of Sport Science, 13(2), 215–220.CrossRef Crust, L., & Swann, C. (2013). The relationship between mental toughness and dispositional flow. European Journal of Sport Science, 13(2), 215–220.CrossRef
go back to reference Csikszentmihalyi, M. (1978). Attention and the Holistic Approach to Behaviour. In K. S. Pope & J. L. Singer (Eds.), The Stream of Consciousness (pp. 335–358). New York: Plenum.CrossRef Csikszentmihalyi, M. (1978). Attention and the Holistic Approach to Behaviour. In K. S. Pope & J. L. Singer (Eds.), The Stream of Consciousness (pp. 335–358). New York: Plenum.CrossRef
go back to reference Csikszentmihalyi, M. (1988). The flow experience and its significance for human psychology. In M. Csikszentmihalyi & I. S. Csikszentmihalyi (Eds.), Optimal experience: psychological studies of flow in consciousness (pp. 15–35). Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.CrossRef Csikszentmihalyi, M. (1988). The flow experience and its significance for human psychology. In M. Csikszentmihalyi & I. S. Csikszentmihalyi (Eds.), Optimal experience: psychological studies of flow in consciousness (pp. 15–35). Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.CrossRef
go back to reference Csikszentmihalyi, M. (2000). Beyond boredom and anxiety. Jossey-Bass. (Original work published 1975). Csikszentmihalyi, M. (2000). Beyond boredom and anxiety. Jossey-Bass. (Original work published 1975).
go back to reference Csikszentmihalyi, M., & LeFevre, J. (1989). Optimal experience in work and leisure. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 56(5), 815.CrossRefPubMed Csikszentmihalyi, M., & LeFevre, J. (1989). Optimal experience in work and leisure. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 56(5), 815.CrossRefPubMed
go back to reference Csikszentmihalyi, M., & Nakamura, J. (2010). Effortless attention in everyday life: A systematic phenomenology. In B. Bruya (Ed.), Effortless attention: a new perspective in the cognitive science of attention and action (pp. 179–190). Cambridge: The MIT Press.CrossRef Csikszentmihalyi, M., & Nakamura, J. (2010). Effortless attention in everyday life: A systematic phenomenology. In B. Bruya (Ed.), Effortless attention: a new perspective in the cognitive science of attention and action (pp. 179–190). Cambridge: The MIT Press.CrossRef
go back to reference de Manzano, Ö, Cervenka, S., Jucaite, A., Hellenäs, O., Farde, L., & Ullén, F. (2013). Individual differences in the proneness to have flow experiences are linked to dopamine D2-receptor availability in the dorsal striatum. Neuroimage, 67, 1–6.CrossRefPubMed de Manzano, Ö, Cervenka, S., Jucaite, A., Hellenäs, O., Farde, L., & Ullén, F. (2013). Individual differences in the proneness to have flow experiences are linked to dopamine D2-receptor availability in the dorsal striatum. Neuroimage, 67, 1–6.CrossRefPubMed
go back to reference Derryberry, D., & Reed, M. A. (2002). Anxiety-related attentional biases and their regulation by attentional control. Journal of Abnormal Psychology, 111(2), 225.CrossRefPubMed Derryberry, D., & Reed, M. A. (2002). Anxiety-related attentional biases and their regulation by attentional control. Journal of Abnormal Psychology, 111(2), 225.CrossRefPubMed
go back to reference Feldman, G., Hayes, A., Kumar, S., Greeson, J., & Laurenceau, J. P. (2007). Mindfulness and emotion regulation: The development and initial validation of the Cognitive and Affective Mindfulness Scale-Revised (CAMS-R). Journal of Psychopathology and Behavioral Assessment, 29(3), 177.CrossRef Feldman, G., Hayes, A., Kumar, S., Greeson, J., & Laurenceau, J. P. (2007). Mindfulness and emotion regulation: The development and initial validation of the Cognitive and Affective Mindfulness Scale-Revised (CAMS-R). Journal of Psychopathology and Behavioral Assessment, 29(3), 177.CrossRef
go back to reference Hamari, J., & Koivisto, J. (2014). Measuring flow in gamification: Dispositional flow scale-2. Computers in Human Behavior, 40, 133–143.CrossRef Hamari, J., & Koivisto, J. (2014). Measuring flow in gamification: Dispositional flow scale-2. Computers in Human Behavior, 40, 133–143.CrossRef
go back to reference Han, S. (1988). The relationship between life satisfaction and flow in elderly Korean immigrants. In M. Csikszentmihalyi & I. S. Csikszentmihalyi (Eds.), Optimal experience: psychological studies of flow in consciousness (pp. 138–149). Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.CrossRef Han, S. (1988). The relationship between life satisfaction and flow in elderly Korean immigrants. In M. Csikszentmihalyi & I. S. Csikszentmihalyi (Eds.), Optimal experience: psychological studies of flow in consciousness (pp. 138–149). Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.CrossRef
go back to reference Harari, Y. N. (2008). Combat flow: military, political, and ethical dimensions of subjective well-being in war. Review of General Psychology, 12(3), 253.CrossRef Harari, Y. N. (2008). Combat flow: military, political, and ethical dimensions of subjective well-being in war. Review of General Psychology, 12(3), 253.CrossRef
go back to reference Harmat, L., de Manzano, Ö, Theorell, T., Högman, L., Fischer, H., & Ullén, F. (2015). Physiological correlates of the flow experience during computer game playing. International Journal of Psychophysiology, 97(1), 1–7.CrossRefPubMed Harmat, L., de Manzano, Ö, Theorell, T., Högman, L., Fischer, H., & Ullén, F. (2015). Physiological correlates of the flow experience during computer game playing. International Journal of Psychophysiology, 97(1), 1–7.CrossRefPubMed
go back to reference Hockey, R. (2013). The psychology of fatigue: work, effort and control. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.CrossRef Hockey, R. (2013). The psychology of fatigue: work, effort and control. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.CrossRef
go back to reference Jackson, S. A., & Eklund, R. C. (2002). Assessing flow in physical activity: the flow state scale-2 and dispositional flow scale-2. Journal of Sport and Exercise Psychology, 24(2), 133–150.CrossRef Jackson, S. A., & Eklund, R. C. (2002). Assessing flow in physical activity: the flow state scale-2 and dispositional flow scale-2. Journal of Sport and Exercise Psychology, 24(2), 133–150.CrossRef
go back to reference Jackson, S. A., Ford, S. K., Kimiecik, J. C., & Marsh, H. W. (1998). Psychological correlates of flow in sport. Journal of Sport and exercise Psychology, 20(4), 358–378.CrossRef Jackson, S. A., Ford, S. K., Kimiecik, J. C., & Marsh, H. W. (1998). Psychological correlates of flow in sport. Journal of Sport and exercise Psychology, 20(4), 358–378.CrossRef
go back to reference Jackson, S. A., & Marsh, H. W. (1996). Development and validation of a scale to measure optimal experience: the Flow State Scale. Journal of Sport and Exercise Psychology, 18(1), 17–35.CrossRef Jackson, S. A., & Marsh, H. W. (1996). Development and validation of a scale to measure optimal experience: the Flow State Scale. Journal of Sport and Exercise Psychology, 18(1), 17–35.CrossRef
go back to reference Jackson, S. A., Thomas, P. R., Marsh, H. W., & Smethurst, C. J. (2001). Relationships between flow, self-concept, psychological skills, and performance. Journal of Applied Sport Psychology, 13(2), 129–153.CrossRef Jackson, S. A., Thomas, P. R., Marsh, H. W., & Smethurst, C. J. (2001). Relationships between flow, self-concept, psychological skills, and performance. Journal of Applied Sport Psychology, 13(2), 129–153.CrossRef
go back to reference John, O. P., Donahue, E. M., & Kentle, R. L. (1991). The Big Five Inventory–Versions 4a and 54. Berkeley: University of California. John, O. P., Donahue, E. M., & Kentle, R. L. (1991). The Big Five Inventory–Versions 4a and 54. Berkeley: University of California.
go back to reference John, O. P., Naumann, L. P., & Soto, C. J. (2008). Paradigm shift to the integrative Big Five trait taxonomy: History, measurement, and conceptual issues. In O. P. John, R. W. Robins & L. A. Pervin (Eds.), Handbook of personality: theory and research (pp. 114–158). New York: Guilford Press. John, O. P., Naumann, L. P., & Soto, C. J. (2008). Paradigm shift to the integrative Big Five trait taxonomy: History, measurement, and conceptual issues. In O. P. John, R. W. Robins & L. A. Pervin (Eds.), Handbook of personality: theory and research (pp. 114–158). New York: Guilford Press.
go back to reference Keller, J., Ringelhan, S., & Blomann, F. (2011). Does skills–demands compatibility result in intrinsic motivation? Experimental test of a basic notion proposed in the theory of flow experiences. The Journal of Positive Psychology, 6(5), 408–417.CrossRef Keller, J., Ringelhan, S., & Blomann, F. (2011). Does skills–demands compatibility result in intrinsic motivation? Experimental test of a basic notion proposed in the theory of flow experiences. The Journal of Positive Psychology, 6(5), 408–417.CrossRef
go back to reference Kennedy, P., Miele, D. B., & Metcalfe, J. (2014). The cognitive antecedents and motivational consequences of the feeling of being in the zone. Consciousness and Cognition, 30, 48–61.CrossRefPubMed Kennedy, P., Miele, D. B., & Metcalfe, J. (2014). The cognitive antecedents and motivational consequences of the feeling of being in the zone. Consciousness and Cognition, 30, 48–61.CrossRefPubMed
go back to reference Kline, R. B. (1998). Principles and practice of structural equation modelling. New York: Guilford Press. Kline, R. B. (1998). Principles and practice of structural equation modelling. New York: Guilford Press.
go back to reference Landhäußer, A., & Keller, J. (2012). Flow and its affective, cognitive, and performance-related consequences. In Advances in flow research (pp. 65–85). New York: Springer.CrossRef Landhäußer, A., & Keller, J. (2012). Flow and its affective, cognitive, and performance-related consequences. In Advances in flow research (pp. 65–85). New York: Springer.CrossRef
go back to reference Lee, E. (2005). The relationship of motivation and flow experience to academic procrastination in university students. The Journal of Genetic Psychology, 166(1), 5–15.CrossRefPubMed Lee, E. (2005). The relationship of motivation and flow experience to academic procrastination in university students. The Journal of Genetic Psychology, 166(1), 5–15.CrossRefPubMed
go back to reference Levitin, D. J. (2014). The organized mind: Thinking straight in the age of information overload. Penguin. Levitin, D. J. (2014). The organized mind: Thinking straight in the age of information overload. Penguin.
go back to reference Macbeth, J. (1988). Ocean cruising. In M. Csikszentmihalyi & I. S. Csikszentmihalyi (Eds.), Optimal experience: psychological studies of flow in consciousness (pp. 214–231). Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.CrossRef Macbeth, J. (1988). Ocean cruising. In M. Csikszentmihalyi & I. S. Csikszentmihalyi (Eds.), Optimal experience: psychological studies of flow in consciousness (pp. 214–231). Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.CrossRef
go back to reference Martin, A. J., & Jackson, S. A. (2008). Brief approaches to assessing task absorption and enhanced subjective experience: Examining ‘short’ and ‘core’ flow in diverse performance domains. Motivation and Emotion, 32(3), 141–157.CrossRef Martin, A. J., & Jackson, S. A. (2008). Brief approaches to assessing task absorption and enhanced subjective experience: Examining ‘short’ and ‘core’ flow in diverse performance domains. Motivation and Emotion, 32(3), 141–157.CrossRef
go back to reference Moore, B. A. (2013). Propensity for experiencing flow: The roles of cognitive flexibility and mindfulness. The Humanistic Psychologist, 41(4), 319.CrossRef Moore, B. A. (2013). Propensity for experiencing flow: The roles of cognitive flexibility and mindfulness. The Humanistic Psychologist, 41(4), 319.CrossRef
go back to reference Mosing, M. A., Magnusson, P. K., Pedersen, N. L., Nakamura, J., Madison, G., & Ullén, F. (2012). Heritability of proneness for psychological flow experiences. Personality and Individual Differences, 53(5), 699–704.CrossRef Mosing, M. A., Magnusson, P. K., Pedersen, N. L., Nakamura, J., Madison, G., & Ullén, F. (2012). Heritability of proneness for psychological flow experiences. Personality and Individual Differences, 53(5), 699–704.CrossRef
go back to reference Nadon, R., Hoyt, I. P., Register, P. A., & Kihlstrom, J. F. (1991). Absorption and hypnotizability: Context effects reexamined. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 60(1), 144.CrossRefPubMed Nadon, R., Hoyt, I. P., Register, P. A., & Kihlstrom, J. F. (1991). Absorption and hypnotizability: Context effects reexamined. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 60(1), 144.CrossRefPubMed
go back to reference Nakamura, J., & Csikszentmihalyi, M. (2002). The concept of flow. In C. R. Snyder & S. J. Lopez (Eds.), Handbook of positive psychology (pp. 89–105). New York: Oxford University Press. Nakamura, J., & Csikszentmihalyi, M. (2002). The concept of flow. In C. R. Snyder & S. J. Lopez (Eds.), Handbook of positive psychology (pp. 89–105). New York: Oxford University Press.
go back to reference Pain, M. A., Harwood, C., & Anderson, R. (2011). Pre-competition imagery and music: the impact on flow and performance in competitive soccer. The Sport Psychologist, 25(2), 212–232.CrossRef Pain, M. A., Harwood, C., & Anderson, R. (2011). Pre-competition imagery and music: the impact on flow and performance in competitive soccer. The Sport Psychologist, 25(2), 212–232.CrossRef
go back to reference Pates, J., Karageorghis, C. I., Fryer, R., & Maynard, I. (2003). Effects of asynchronous music on flow states and shooting performance among netball players. Psychology of Sport and Exercise, 4(4), 415–427.CrossRef Pates, J., Karageorghis, C. I., Fryer, R., & Maynard, I. (2003). Effects of asynchronous music on flow states and shooting performance among netball players. Psychology of Sport and Exercise, 4(4), 415–427.CrossRef
go back to reference Peifer, C., Schulz, A., Schächinger, H., Baumann, N., & Antoni, C. H. (2014). The relation of flow-experience and physiological arousal under stress—can u shape it? Journal of Experimental Social Psychology, 53, 62–69.CrossRef Peifer, C., Schulz, A., Schächinger, H., Baumann, N., & Antoni, C. H. (2014). The relation of flow-experience and physiological arousal under stress—can u shape it? Journal of Experimental Social Psychology, 53, 62–69.CrossRef
go back to reference Ralph, B. C., Seli, P., Wilson, K. E., & Smilek, D. (2018) Volitional media multitasking: Awareness of performance costs and modulation of media multitasking as a function of task demand. Psychological Research (under review). Ralph, B. C., Seli, P., Wilson, K. E., & Smilek, D. (2018) Volitional media multitasking: Awareness of performance costs and modulation of media multitasking as a function of task demand. Psychological Research (under review).
go back to reference Raven, J.C. (1962). Advanced progressive matrices. Set II. London: H. K. Lewis & Co. Distributed in the USA by The Psychological Corporation. San Antonio. Texas. Raven, J.C. (1962). Advanced progressive matrices. Set II. London: H. K. Lewis & Co. Distributed in the USA by The Psychological Corporation. San Antonio. Texas.
go back to reference Raven, J.C. (1965). Advanced Progressive Matrices. Sets I and II. London: H. K. Lewis & Co. Distributed in the USA by The Psychological Corporation. San Antonio, Texas. Raven, J.C. (1965). Advanced Progressive Matrices. Sets I and II. London: H. K. Lewis & Co. Distributed in the USA by The Psychological Corporation. San Antonio, Texas.
go back to reference Ross, S. R., & Keiser, H. N. (2014). Autotelic personality through a five-factor lens: individual differences in flow-propensity. Personality and Individual Differences, 59, 3–8.CrossRef Ross, S. R., & Keiser, H. N. (2014). Autotelic personality through a five-factor lens: individual differences in flow-propensity. Personality and Individual Differences, 59, 3–8.CrossRef
go back to reference Sato, I. (1988). Bosozoku: flow in Japanese motorcycle gangs. In M. Csikszentmihalyi & I. S. Csikszentmihalyi (Eds.), Optimal experience: Psychological studies of flow in consciousness (pp. 92–117). Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.CrossRef Sato, I. (1988). Bosozoku: flow in Japanese motorcycle gangs. In M. Csikszentmihalyi & I. S. Csikszentmihalyi (Eds.), Optimal experience: Psychological studies of flow in consciousness (pp. 92–117). Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.CrossRef
go back to reference Sirois, F. M. (2014). Absorbed in the moment? An investigation of procrastination, absorption and cognitive failures. Personality and individual differences, 71, 30–34.CrossRef Sirois, F. M. (2014). Absorbed in the moment? An investigation of procrastination, absorption and cognitive failures. Personality and individual differences, 71, 30–34.CrossRef
go back to reference Smilek, D., Carriere, J. S., & Cheyne, J. A. (2010). Out of mind, out of sight: eye blinking as in-dicator and embodiment of mind wandering. Psychological Science, 21(6), 786–789.CrossRefPubMed Smilek, D., Carriere, J. S., & Cheyne, J. A. (2010). Out of mind, out of sight: eye blinking as in-dicator and embodiment of mind wandering. Psychological Science, 21(6), 786–789.CrossRefPubMed
go back to reference Smyser, C. H., & Baron, D. A. (1993). Hypnotizability, absorption, and subscales of the Dissociative Experiences Scale in a nonclinical population. Dissociation: Progress in the Dissociative Disorders, 6(1), 42–46. Smyser, C. H., & Baron, D. A. (1993). Hypnotizability, absorption, and subscales of the Dissociative Experiences Scale in a nonclinical population. Dissociation: Progress in the Dissociative Disorders, 6(1), 42–46.
go back to reference Stavrou, N. A., Jackson, S. A., Zervas, Y., & Karteroliotis, K. (2007). Flow experience and athletes’ performance with reference to the orthogonal model of flow. The Sport Psychologist, 21(4), 438–457.CrossRef Stavrou, N. A., Jackson, S. A., Zervas, Y., & Karteroliotis, K. (2007). Flow experience and athletes’ performance with reference to the orthogonal model of flow. The Sport Psychologist, 21(4), 438–457.CrossRef
go back to reference Swann, C. (2016). Flow in Sport. In L. Harmat, F. O. Anderson, F. Ullén, J. Wright & G. Sadlo (Eds.), Flow Experience (pp. 51–64). Switzerland: Springer.CrossRef Swann, C. (2016). Flow in Sport. In L. Harmat, F. O. Anderson, F. Ullén, J. Wright & G. Sadlo (Eds.), Flow Experience (pp. 51–64). Switzerland: Springer.CrossRef
go back to reference Swann, C., Keegan, R. J., Piggott, D., & Crust, L. (2012). A systematic review of the experience, occurrence, and controllability of flow states in elite sport. Psychology of Sport and Exercise, 13(6), 807–819.CrossRef Swann, C., Keegan, R. J., Piggott, D., & Crust, L. (2012). A systematic review of the experience, occurrence, and controllability of flow states in elite sport. Psychology of Sport and Exercise, 13(6), 807–819.CrossRef
go back to reference Tellegen, A. (1992). Note on structure and meaning of the MPQ Absorption scale. University of Minnesota (Un-published manuscript). Tellegen, A. (1992). Note on structure and meaning of the MPQ Absorption scale. University of Minnesota (Un-published manuscript).
go back to reference Tellegen, A., & Atkinson, G. (1974). Openness to absorbing and self-altering experiences (“absorption”), a trait related to hypnotic susceptibility. Journal of abnormal psychology, 83(3), 268.CrossRefPubMed Tellegen, A., & Atkinson, G. (1974). Openness to absorbing and self-altering experiences (“absorption”), a trait related to hypnotic susceptibility. Journal of abnormal psychology, 83(3), 268.CrossRefPubMed
go back to reference Ullén, F., de Manzano, Ö, Almeida, R., Magnusson, P. K., Pedersen, N. L., Nakamura, J., Madison, G. (2012). Proneness for psychological flow in everyday life: Associations with personality and intelligence. Personality and Individual Differences, 52(2), 167–172.CrossRef Ullén, F., de Manzano, Ö, Almeida, R., Magnusson, P. K., Pedersen, N. L., Nakamura, J., Madison, G. (2012). Proneness for psychological flow in everyday life: Associations with personality and intelligence. Personality and Individual Differences, 52(2), 167–172.CrossRef
go back to reference Ullén, F., de Manzano, Ö, Theorell, T., & Harmat, L. (2010). 10 The physiology of effortless attention: correlates of state flow and flow proneness. Effortless attention: a new perspective in the cognitive science of attention and action, p. 205. Ullén, F., de Manzano, Ö, Theorell, T., & Harmat, L. (2010). 10 The physiology of effortless attention: correlates of state flow and flow proneness. Effortless attention: a new perspective in the cognitive science of attention and action, p. 205.
go back to reference Ullén, F., Harmat, L., Theorell, T., & Madison, G. (2016). Flow and individual differences—a phenotypic analysis of data from more than 10,000 twin individuals. In L. Harmat, F. O. Anderson, F. Ullén, J. Wright & G. Sadlo (Eds.), Flow experience (pp. 267–288). Springer International Publishing. Ullén, F., Harmat, L., Theorell, T., & Madison, G. (2016). Flow and individual differences—a phenotypic analysis of data from more than 10,000 twin individuals. In L. Harmat, F. O. Anderson, F. Ullén, J. Wright & G. Sadlo (Eds.), Flow experience (pp. 267–288). Springer International Publishing.
go back to reference Ulrich, M., Keller, J., & Grön, G. (2016). Neural signatures of experimentally induced flow experiences identified in a typical fMRI block design with BOLD imaging. Social cognitive and affective neuroscience, 11(3), 496–507.CrossRefPubMed Ulrich, M., Keller, J., & Grön, G. (2016). Neural signatures of experimentally induced flow experiences identified in a typical fMRI block design with BOLD imaging. Social cognitive and affective neuroscience, 11(3), 496–507.CrossRefPubMed
go back to reference Ulrich, M., Keller, J., Hoenig, K., Waller, C., & Grön, G. (2014). Neural correlates of experimentally induced flow experiences. Neuroimage, 86, 194–202.CrossRefPubMed Ulrich, M., Keller, J., Hoenig, K., Waller, C., & Grön, G. (2014). Neural correlates of experimentally induced flow experiences. Neuroimage, 86, 194–202.CrossRefPubMed
go back to reference Wang, Z., Irwin, M., Cooper, C., & Srivastava, J. (2015). Multidimensions of media multitasking and adaptive media selection. Human Communication Research, 41(1), 102–127.CrossRef Wang, Z., Irwin, M., Cooper, C., & Srivastava, J. (2015). Multidimensions of media multitasking and adaptive media selection. Human Communication Research, 41(1), 102–127.CrossRef
go back to reference Wright, J. J., Sadlo, G., & Stew, G. (2006). Challenge-skills and mindfulness: an exploration of the conundrum of flow process. OTJR: Occupation, participation and health, 26(1), 25–32. Wright, J. J., Sadlo, G., & Stew, G. (2006). Challenge-skills and mindfulness: an exploration of the conundrum of flow process. OTJR: Occupation, participation and health, 26(1), 25–32.
go back to reference Wright, J. J., Sadlo, G., & Stew, G. (2007). Further explorations into the conundrum of flow process. Journal of Occupational Science, 14(3), 136–144.CrossRef Wright, J. J., Sadlo, G., & Stew, G. (2007). Further explorations into the conundrum of flow process. Journal of Occupational Science, 14(3), 136–144.CrossRef
Metagegevens
Titel
Deep, effortless concentration: re-examining the flow concept and exploring relations with inattention, absorption, and personality
Auteurs
Jeremy Marty-Dugas
Daniel Smilek
Publicatiedatum
14-06-2018
Uitgeverij
Springer Berlin Heidelberg
Gepubliceerd in
Psychological Research / Uitgave 8/2019
Print ISSN: 0340-0727
Elektronisch ISSN: 1430-2772
DOI
https://doi.org/10.1007/s00426-018-1031-6

Andere artikelen Uitgave 8/2019

Psychological Research 8/2019 Naar de uitgave