Skip to main content
Top
Gepubliceerd in: Child Psychiatry & Human Development 1/2024

Open Access 16-06-2022 | Original Article

The Moderating Role of Maternal Praise and Positivity in the Association Between Callous-Unemotional (CU) Traits and Later Aggression: A Prospective Study in Preschool Children in Colombia

Auteurs: Diana Obando, Jonathan Hill, Nicola Wright

Gepubliceerd in: Child Psychiatry & Human Development | Uitgave 1/2024

Abstract

Our previous findings in the UK and Colombia show that CU traits predict later aggression specifically among children who are already aggressive. We hypothesised that this effect would be reduced in the presence of maternal praise and positivity. In a sample of 220 mothers and children from Colombia, mother–child interactions were coded for maternal praise and positivity, and mothers reported on children’s CU traits at age 3.5 and aggression at ages 3.5 and 5 years. The results show three-way interactions between CU traits, child aggression and observed parenting at age 3.5 years in the prediction of later child aggression, and two-way interactions indicating a protective effect of positive parenting in the high aggressive children. Based on our finding, it is plausible that positive parenting may modify the effect of CU traits in the highly vulnerable group of children who are already aggressive in early childhood.
Opmerkingen

Supplementary Information

The online version contains supplementary material available at https://​doi.​org/​10.​1007/​s10578-022-01354-3.

Publisher's Note

Springer Nature remains neutral with regard to jurisdictional claims in published maps and institutional affiliations.

Introduction

The construct of callous-unemotional (CU) traits has proved highly productive in the identification of important heterogeneity within conduct problems (CPs), and hence in understanding their aetiology, and maintenance, and ultimately in the identification of treatments [1]. CU traits may create vulnerability to CPs, and in particular aggression, by removing an internal source of restraint, empathy for others’ distress. In that case, the effect of CU traits on later aggression may be attenuated in the presence of alternative sources of restraint. These may be internal, for example, higher levels of physiological arousal [2] or increased mentalisation [3]. External restraint in the presence of elevated CU traits may also be provided by positive reinforcement for prosocial behaviours, or by warm supportive parenting which may promote the internalisation of social norms and prosocial behaviour [4, 5]. We have previously shown that CU traits at age 3.5 are associated with aggression at age 5.0 years specifically among children who are already aggressive [6]. In this paper we use observations of mother–child interactions at age 3.5 years and maternal report of child behaviours at ages 3.5 and 5.0 years and asked whether this association is attenuated by high maternal praise and high positivity.

The Role of Positive Parenting in Relation to the Link Between CU Traits and Conduct Problems

A substantial literature, including a number of studies with genetically informed designs supports a role for positive parenting in the development of CU traits [79]. However, whilst there are theoretical accounts of the potential role of positive reinforcement and parental positivity in reducing CPs in children with CU traits [5, 10], the available evidence is inconsistent. Generally, prospective studies have failed to demonstrate that positive parenting moderates the association between CU traits and CPs. For example, studying a sample of aggressive children aged 9–12 years over 1 year, Pardini et al. [11] found that child reported parental warmth and involvement did not moderate associations between CU traits and later antisocial behaviour. Another study by Hyde et al. [12] which used a broad measure of observed positive parenting that included contingent use of praise with a general population high-risk sample of 364 children at age 2–4 years also found no evidence for moderation. By contrast, moderation was observed prospectively in a lower risk sample of 100 two parent families in which positive parenting was observed over multiple contexts at ages 38 and 52 months, CU traits and externalising behaviours were assessed at 67 months and externalising behaviours again at 80 and 100 months. Kochanska et al. [5] found that positive parenting reduced the prospective association between CU traits and later externalising behaviours. The positive parenting coded in this study was more dyadic than in other studies, reflecting shared positive affect and shared responsiveness between parent and child. However, this study collected both mother and father observed parenting and mother and father report of externalising and only two out of four interactions were significant.
In one study cross-sectional moderation was found that was not evident at follow up. Kroneman et al. [13] found that low maternal warmth predicted decreasing levels of CPs among girls with high CU behaviour (N = 1233; aged 7–8 years old at baseline) but the interaction was no longer significant after 5 years. Moderation has also been shown in several solely cross-sectional studies. In a study by Pasalich et al. [14] of clinic referred boys aged 4–12 years, parental warmth assessed in the Five-Minute Speech Sample (FMSS) [15] was associated with lower CPs, specifically in the presence of elevated CU traits. Likewise in another cross-sectional study aged 4 to 12 years by Clark and Frick [16] using parent report in the Alabama Parenting Questionnaire (APQ) [17] positive reinforcement was associated with lower CPs specifically in the presence of high CU traits. Studies using latent profile analysis can also provide pointers to the role of positive parenting. In a study of 1366 children aged 7–11 years at recruitment using the APQ [18], positive reinforcement and positive involvement were examined separately using child and mother report on the APQ. Latent profile analysis using data from the three time points yielded five groups, including a high-CU traits only group (9.4%) and a high-CPs and CU traits group (7.2%). Mothers of children in the high-CU traits only group reported higher levels of involvement and positive reinforcement than those of the high-CPs and CU traits group, consistent with a protective role for positive parenting.

CU traits in Low- and Middle- Income Countries

CU traits have almost exclusively been investigated in High-Income Countries (HICs) [19] and it is yet to be established whether the construct and proposed mechanisms generalize across cultures and socioeconomic conditions. Evidence from Low and Middle-Income Countries (LMICs) has mainly been provided from China. Support for the reliability and validity of the CU traits construct in Chinese school-age and adolescent children has been provided [20, 21]. In this sample of preschool Colombian children, we have reported on the reliability and factor structure of the main measure of CU traits in children, the Inventory of Callous-Unemotional traits [6] and provided evidence of validity in relation to aggression. A cross-country comparison study of nine different HIC and LMIC societies (China, Colombia, Italy, Jordan, Kenya, the Philippines, Sweden, Thailand, or the United States) [22] indicated that Colombian parents reported higher levels of warm parenting than the grand mean across countries, but there were no significant differences in the association between warmth and child aggression. In unpublished data from the sample analysed here we have shown associations between parent-report of positive parenting and child CU traits, and further evidence for specificity in associations between positive parenting and CU traits and negative parenting and child ODD symptoms [2, 23]. Therefore, whilst there is evidence for a difference in levels of parenting the existing limited evidence supports similar associations between positive parenting, CU traits and aggression in LMIC settings. However, no study in a LMIC has examined whether positive parenting moderates the association between CU traits and CPs. In a HIC outside of a Western setting, and with adolescents, Sng et al. [24] examined moderation by negative parenting practices only and found evidence for moderation.
In sum, the existing evidence on whether positive parenting moderates the association between CU traits and aggression is inconsistent, with more support found for moderation in cross-sectional studies and limited evidence from prospective studies. Recent findings from the sample analysed here, and from the UK Wirral Child Health and Development Study have indicated that one possible explanation is that in young children the effect of CU traits is mainly seen among those who are already aggressive [6]. In that case we need to examine the protective effect of positive parenting, specifically in this group. In the present study we therefore examined whether there is an effect of positive parenting in attenuating the association between CU traits and later aggression, specifically among children who are already aggressive. To test this, we modelled the three-way interaction between maternal positive parenting, CU traits and child aggression at age 3.5 years in the prediction of aggression at age 5 years. Positive parenting was assessed as observed maternal praise for following instructions, and warmth and reciprocity during play.

Methods

Participants and Procedure

The La Sabana Parent–Child Study participants were recruited through Facebook groups likely to be used by young mothers, such as ‘Latin Women League’ and ‘More Moms Colombia’. It is estimated that 91% of those between the ages of 14 and 65 in Colombia access Facebook [25]. Parents who responded to the online study information (n = 344) were contacted to discuss participation. Of these, 40 were excluded as the children did not meet the age inclusion criteria of age 3–4 years. Of the remaining 304, 235 (77.3%) provided informed consent to take part in the study and full data. For the baseline assessment, 235 families with children of 3.5 years (M = 3.31, SD = 0.48) participated, 48% girls/52% boys, and the mothers’ average age was 30.04 years (SD = 6.29) which is similar to general population statistics, where the average age at first pregnancy is 28 years old [26]. At follow-up, 18 months later, 220 (93%) participants provided data for the analyses presented here (mean age = 4.86; SD = 0.42; 51% girls/49% boys). The study was approved by the Research and Ethical Committee of the Psychology Department at La Sabana University.
Participants were recruited from three Colombian regions, each with different cultural and demographic features. The Pacific (n = 69) and Caribbean (n = 70) regions are characterized by high levels of poverty and extensive numbers of Afro-Colombian and Indigenous inhabitants, while the Central region (n = 96) has the lowest levels of poverty in the country, and it is predominantly mestizo (mix of European and Indigenous) [27]. Overall, 15% of the participants lived in rural areas which is similar to national statistics [28]. The majority (77%) were two parent families and 45% of the sample belonged to the lowest two household income classifications, based on the Colombian government system that classifies households into six categories (1 the lowest) determined by housing conditions and basic public services, such as sewerage and water supply [29]. Classifications 1 and 2 receive subsidies from the Colombian government. The rate of low socioeconomic status (SES) was similar to national statistics [28]. Regarding participants ethnicity, 38% mothers identified as mestizo, 9% Afro-Colombian, 5% Indigenous, 13% from ‘other’ ethnic groups, and 35% did not identify themselves as belonging to a specific ethnic group. The prevalence of self-reported Indigenous or Afro-Colombian status is close to the national estimate [30]. The sample differed from national estimates on the proportion with a university degree, with 50% of mothers and 48% of fathers with university degrees, compared to 22% in the general population [31].
Participants were recruited across three contrasting regions, and a widely available medium was used for recruitment, yielding a sample that in many aspects was representative of the general population of Colombia. The rate of low socioeconomic status (SES; 45%) was similar to those in published studies in Colombia [28], and the numbers from rural districts (15%) were similar to national statistics (15) [28]. The prevalence of self-reported Indigenous or Afro-Colombian status (14%) was very close to the national estimate of 14.4% [30]. A third of the sample reported experiencing community violence or displacement. The sample differed, however, in that 50% of mothers and 48% of fathers had a university degree, contrasted with 22% in the general population [31]. Thus, although we contacted parents through social media almost universally accessed by the general population, participation was probably biased towards more highly educated parents, limiting the generalizability of the findings.
The sample analysed here represents participants who provided questionnaire data at age 3.5 and 5.0 years and mother–child observational data at 3.5 years (N = 220).

Measures

Parental Report of Callous-Unemotional Traits

CU traits were measured at 3.5 years and at 5.0 years using the Spanish version of the parent-report preschool Inventory of Callous-Unemotional Traits (ICU) [32]. This inventory has 24 items scored using a 4-point scale (0 = not at all true, 1 = somewhat true, 3 = very true, and 4 = definitely true). Evidence for the reliability and validity of the ICU total score in preschool children in a HIC setting has been provided from a cross-sectional study by Kimonis et al. [33]. In a previous publication from this Colombian sample, we have replicated the best-fitting factor structure reported in Kimonis, a 12-item two-correlated factor structure, and found similar internal reliability and provided evidence for validity in relation to aggression [6]. Similar to Willoughby et al. [34] (2015) we have not been able to replicate the bi-factor structure of the ICU. However, in line with the recommendations of Ray et al. [35] and to retain comparability to the majority of the literature, we conducted the main analysis using the total 24-item ICU scale but report the results using the 12-item total in the appendix. The internal consistency in this sample for the 12-item total is α = 0.71 at age 3.5, and α = 0.78 at age 5 years, and for the 24-item total it is α = 0.82 at age 3 years, and α = 0.85 at age 5 years. The Spanish version of the inventory was shared by the authors, who approved its use in the present study.

Parental Report of Aggressive Behaviour: Child Behaviour Checklist (CBCL) [36]

The Spanish version of the CBCL for children aged 1.5 to 5 years was used to assess aggressive behaviours. In our data the aggressive behaviour syndrome scale had an internal consistency of 0.85 for the baseline and 0.88 for the follow-up, values that are like those previously reported with Colombian children (α = 0.86) [37].

Observation of Mother–Child Interactions

Video recordings were made of mothers and children in a standardized procedure used previously in National Institute for Child Health and Development studies (NICHD) [38]. Parents and children were provided with three bags of toys which they are asked to play with in a pre-set order over 15 min, with a pre-agreed signal the child is asked to tidy up the toys. Mother–child play was rated using the Parent–Child Interaction System (PARCHISY) [39] generating a parental positivity score as the average of positive affect (e.g., smiling and laughter: warm, friendly voice, inquisitive voice, looking at what the child is doing) and reciprocity (e.g., shared positive affect, eye contact and turn taking interaction) scales [40]. Unlabelled praise during tidy-up was assessed using the Dyadic Parent–Child Interaction Coding System (DPICS) [41] by identifying contingent neutral comment such as “that’s it”, “ok” or “keep going”, and contingent expressions of gratitude such as “thank you” (see appendix Table A1). Research assistants in Colombia were trained by reliable UK researchers, and high agreement between them on 30 independently rated recordings in English from the UK was achieved (all Intraclass Correlation Coefficients [ICCs] ≥ 0.78). There were too few instances of labelled praise in either UK or Colombian studies for reliability analyses.
Table 1
Summary of multiple linear regression models predicting age 5.0 aggression from maternal praise, CU traits and aggression
 
ΔR2
p
Variable
β
p
Block 1
0.15
 < 0.001
Child sex
0.11
0.082
   
Higher family income
− 0.03
0.663
   
Married/cohab parents
− 0.05
0.483
   
Pacific region
− 0.01
0.915
   
Caribbean region
− 0.01
0.864
   
Maternal mood age 5.0
0.36
 < 0.001
Block 2
0.18
 < 0.001
Aggression age 3.5
0.39
 < 0.001
   
CU traits age 3.5 years
0.19
0.005
   
Maternal praise
0.11
0.080
Block 3
0.01
0.617
Aggression × CU traits 3.5
0.06
0.296
   
Aggression × praise 3.5
− 0.01
0.884
   
CU traits × praise
− 0.04
0.663
Block 4
0.01
0.047
Aggression × praise × CU traits
− 0.18
0.047
Block 1 effects were generated from a model with only Block 1, Block 2 effect from a model with Block1 plus Block 2, and Block 3 effect from a model with all three Blocks

Covariates

The Spanish version of the Edinburgh Postnatal Depression Scale (EPDS) [42] was used to assess maternal mood at age 5 to account for possible mood related biases in reporting. The EPDS has 10 items scored on a 4-point scale. This measure is widely used during the prenatal and postnatal periods with parents of young children [43]. Internal consistency of the EPDS was good both in a previous study of Colombian women (α = 0.78) [44] and in this study (α = 0.83). Child sex, household income (operationalised as a binary variable, 0 = lowest two DANE family income classifications, 1 = all other income classifications), single- versus two-parent family structure (0 = single parent, 1 = two cohabiting parents) and dummy variables for Caribbean and Pacific regions were included as covariates in regression analyses.

Data Analyses

Stata version 16 was used for the statistical analyses. Following square root transformations for skewed variables, bivariate associations between the study variables were examined using Pearson, point-biserial and tetrachoric correlations. Maternal praise was highly skewed and therefore a binary variable reflecting 0 = no praise, and 1 = 1 or more instances of praise was used for analysis. Maternal praise and positivity were examined in two separate regression models. The models were estimated in three blocks. Block 1 included demographic covariates and parental mood at age 5 years, Block 2 included age 3.5 years child aggression, age 3.5 years CU traits, and parenting, Block 3 included the two-way interactions terms, and Block 4 the three-way interaction between age 3.5 years aggression, CU traits and parenting, testing at each stage whether the addition of a block significantly increased the explained variance. The three-way interactions were then explored by examining the two-way interactions between CU traits and parenting at high and low levels of age 3.5 aggression, indexed using a median split.

Results

Bivariate associations between the transformed study variables and descriptive statistics for untransformed variables are presented in the Online Appendix Table A2.
Table 1 shows separate models for maternal report of aggression regressed on to Block 1 variables, then Block 2 after accounting for Block 1, Block 3 accounting for Block 1 and 2 and similarly Block 4 after accounting for Block 1, 2 and 3 variables, so that all the reported coefficients are directly interpretable. After accounting for sociodemographic confounders and maternal mood at time of reporting outcome in Block 2, age 3.5 years CU traits, aggression and, unexpectedly, maternal praise, all significantly predicted increased age 5.0 aggression. In Block 3, none of the two-way interaction terms was significant. However, in Block 4, the three-way interaction between CU traits, aggression and praise was significant (p = 0.025). In Table 2, we show the two-way interaction between CU traits and praise in groups below and above the median on age 3.5 years aggression.
Table 2
Summary of multiple linear regression models predicting age 5.0 years aggression from age 3.5 years CU traits and maternal praise in high and low aggression groups
Variable
Low Aggression
High Aggression
ΔR2
p
β
p
ΔR2
p
β
p
Block 2
0.05
0.075
  
0.09
0.004
  
Maternal praise
  
0.14
0.150
  
− 0.02
0.831
CU traits
  
0.20
0.046
  
0.33
 < 0.001
Block 3
0.02
0.121
  
0.02
0.143
  
CU traits × maternal praise
  
0.21
0.121
  
− 0.18
0.143
Block 1 effects were generated from a model with only Block 1, Block 2 effect from a model with Block1 plus Block 2
Whilst the two-way interaction in the high aggression group was not conventionally significant, it can be seen in the right panel of Fig. 1 that the association between age 3.5 years CU traits and age 5.0 aggression was attenuated in the children with high praise. Unexpectedly, whilst the two-way interaction in the low aggression group was also non-significant, it can be seen in Fig. 1 that the association between age 3.5 CU traits and later aggression was stronger in children whose mothers had praised them more. When we repeated this analysis using the short 12-item ICU (shown in Online Appendix Table A3), the three-way interaction was no longer significant (p = 0.056), which arose because this counterintuitive interaction was substantially attenuated (reduced from β = 0.22, p = 0.112, to β = 0.07, p = 0.610). The size and significance of the hypothesised two-way interaction in the high aggression group was slightly increased (− 0.24, p = 0.061).
We then estimated the same regression models to test for moderation by maternal positivity, shown in Table 3. There was a non-significant (p = 0.166) three-way interaction between CU traits, aggression and maternal positivity, which arose from different effects of positivity in children showing low vs high aggression at age 3.5 years. Among those with low aggression, maternal positivity did not modify the association between CU traits and later child aggression (p = 0.884), while there was a moderator effect in the high aggression group (p = 0.040; Table 4).
Table 3
Summary of multiple linear regression models predicting age 5.0 years aggression from maternal positivity, CU traits and aggression
 
ΔR2
p
Variable
β
p
Block 1
0.15
 < 0.001
Child sex
0.11
0.082
   
Higher family income
− 0.04
0.663
   
Married/cohab parents
− 0.05
0.462
   
Pacific region
− 0.01
0.915
   
Caribbean region
− 0.01
0.864
   
Maternal mood age 5.0
0.36
 < 0.001
Block 2
0.18
 < 0.001
Aggression age 3.5
0.36
 < 0.001
   
CU traits age 3.5 years
0.18
0.008
   
Maternal positivity
− 0.06
0.302
Block 3
0.02
0.140
Aggression × CU traits 3.5
0.08
0.218
   
Aggression × positivity 3.5
0.12
0.066
   
CU traits × positivity
− 0.09
0.183
Block 4
0.01
0.166
Aggression × positivity × CU traits
− 0.10
0.166
Block 1 effects were generated from a model with only Block 1, Block 2 effect from a model with Block1 plus Block 2, and Block 3 effect from a model with all three Blocks
Table 4
Summary of multiple linear regression models predicting age 5.0 years aggression from age 3.5 years CU trait and maternal positivity in the high and low aggression groups
 
Low Aggression
High Aggression
Variable
ΔR2
p
β
p
ΔR2
p
β
p
Block 2
0.05
0.073
  
0.09
0.003
  
Maternal positivity
  
− 0.14
0.187
  
− 0.02
0.883
CU traits
  
0.19
0.062
  
0.33
0.001
Block 3
0.01
0.844
  
0.03
0.040
  
CU traits × maternal positivity
  
0.02
0.884
  
− 0.20
0.040
Block 1 effects were generated from a model with only Block 1, Block 2 effect from a model with Block1 plus Block 2
Figure 2 shows the association between age 3.5 CU traits and age 5.0 aggression at three levels of age 3.5 maternal positivity (at mean and 1 SD above and below). It can be seen that in children with high aggression at age 3.5 years, the association between CU traits and age 5.0 aggression was attenuated in those whose mothers had shown high positivity. The analysis was repeated using the 12-item ICU (reported in the Online Appendix Tables A5 and A6) and the pattern of the results were the same, but the size of the effects was reduced, and all were non-significant.

Discussion

Against a background of inconsistent evidence as to whether positive parenting moderates the association between CU traits and CPs, we asked whether the protective effect of positive parenting may be seen specifically in children who are already aggressive. In relation both to maternal praise and maternal positivity (warmth and reciprocity), in children who were already aggressive, the prospective association between CU traits at age 3.5 years and aggression at 5 years was reduced in the presence of positive parenting, consistent with our hypotheses.
This was the first study to examine whether moderation by positive parenting of the association between CU and aggression is confined to children already aggressive [5, 11, 12]. Our results provide evidence for the hypothesis that both maternal praise and maternal positivity moderate the association between CU traits and aggression, but only in children who are already aggressive. Among children already aggressive at age 3.5 years, standardised betas for the two-way interactions between both parenting variables (praise and positivity) and CU traits predicting age 5.0 years aggression were very similar, reflecting that CU traits predicted later aggression only in the presence of low positive parenting. We interpret the values of ‘p’ for the interactions, which fell either side of the conventional significance level, mainly in the light of the very similar effect sizes for the interactions, consistent with American Statistical Society advice [45].
Although there was a significant three-way interaction for maternal praise in analyses using the 24-item measure of CU traits (ICU), this arose in part because in the children with low aggression at age 3.5 years CU traits were more strongly associated with later aggression in the presence of high maternal praise. This puzzling pattern of associations did not emerge when the analyses were conducted using the 12-item ICU, and the corresponding 3-way interaction, while still evident also became non-significant. Therefore, our findings provide good evidence that positive parenting attenuates the association between CU traits and later aggression, in children who are already aggressive, but they do not provide clear evidence for a contrast with children with lower levels of aggression.
It is possible that the differences in the findings when using the 12-item and 24-item versions of the ICU are explained by items omitted from the shorter version which may not best reflect the CU traits construct. The 12-item total has been shown to be the best fitting factor structure for the ICU in pre- and school-age children. It removes five of the six items from the unemotional scale, which have been found to show opposite to expected associations with external correlates and has been speculated to assess shy and withdrawn behaviour rather than unemotionality as it is conceptualised in CU traits [46]. The other items excluded from the 12-item version assess unconcern about performance. Whilst this component of CU traits has been included in the DSM limited prosocial specifier CPs, it is not included in most conceptualisations of CU traits or psychopathic traits [4749]. It may be that the unconcern about performance items are less relevant to pre- and school-aged children. A a similar 11-item version (excluding the one remaining unemotional item) has been shown to be the best fitting factor structure of the ICU in Chinese adolescents and undergraduate students [20, 21] and Chinese and UK 10–11 years old [50]. These results suggest that the excluded items are less central or not part of the CU traits construct.
As we noted earlier, there are strong theoretical reasons to suggest that positive parenting will be protective for CPs or aggression in children with elevated CU traits. Drawing on behavioural and neurological evidence that individuals with CU traits are less responsive to punishment but may be more responsive to reward [51, 52]. Reidy et al. [10] have argued specifically for a role for positive reinforcement of prosocial behaviour, suggesting that it may be a particularly relevant restraint on antisocial behaviour amongst individuals with CU traits due to their reward-dominant processing style. Work on the development of conscience [4, 53, 54] has suggested that warm and mutually responsive positive parenting may reduce antisocial behaviour by promoting the internalisation of social rules, but only in children who are temperamentally fearless and thus may be at risk of aggressive behaviour. Whilst some existing studies have examined the moderation between positive parenting and CU traits in CP on aggressive samples [11], the majority have used general population or samples at elevated risk due to sociodemographic variables and have been based on the expectation that CU traits are related to later aggression as a main effect. Our previous findings across Colombian and UK samples were consistent with that assumption but also indicated that the strength of the association between CU traits and later aggression was much stronger in the presence of pre-existing aggression. This finding that we replicated across informants and in Colombian and UK samples created a new context for the study of the role of positive parenting. This heterogeneity may account at least in part for the negative findings in relation to CU traits as a main effect. Our previous findings suggest that among children with low levels of aggression, CU traits do not present an increased risk for aggression, and therefore there is little or no effect to be modified by quality of parenting. The findings reported here are consistent with, but do not provide conclusive evidence in support of, this explanation.
The strengths of the study include the use of a prospective design in which parenting at 3.5 years was observed, thus reducing possible shared method variance effects on the rating of parenting and psychopathology. Further, this is the first study to examine whether positive parenting moderates the association between CU traits and CPs in a LMIC setting. Both positivity and praise showed the expected negative association with CU traits found in HICs. The participants were recruited across three contrasting regions, and a widely available medium was used for recruitment, yielding a sample that in many aspects was representative of the general population of Colombia. The rate of low socioeconomic status was similar to those in published studies in Colombia [28], and the numbers from rural districts were similar to national statistics (15) [28]. The prevalence of self-reported Indigenous or Afro-Colombian status was very close to the national estimate [30].
A limitation of the study is that 50% of mothers and 48% of fathers had a university degree, contrasted with 22% in the general population of Colombia [31]. Thus, although we contacted parents through social media almost universally accessed by the general population, participation was probably biased towards more highly educated parents, limiting the generalizability of the findings. A further limitation is that it is possible that associations between parenting and child aggression arose because mothers who show less positive parenting perceive their children more negatively. The study findings would have been strengthened by collecting additional informants on child behaviour. Additional limitations include that statistical power for the hypothesised three-way interaction was modest, which created challenges in evaluating statistical significance, and created risks not only of Type 1 but also Type 2 errors.
Future directions from this research include examination of whether the moderation of CU traits by positive parenting in aggressive children persists over longer follow up periods. Evidence has shown that the association between parenting and both CPs and CU traits is bidirectional [8] and this may change over development with certain parenting behaviours potentially becoming more or less relevant to the emergence versus the maintenance of CU traits and CPs [46]. Analyses which can take into account parenting behaviours at the outcome age, and continuity or discontinuity in parenting behaviour over time, are needed. Finally, recent work has indicated that psychopathic traits—the constellation of CU traits, grandiose traits and sensation seeking—are a better predictor of future antisocial behaviour than CU traits alone [47]. The majority of child literature has focused on CU traits alone, and more work needs to be done to examine whether the same mechanisms underpin psychopathic traits. Future studies should examine first whether psychopathic traits create vulnerability is already aggressive children and if they do whether this is moderated by positive parenting.
The present findings indicate that further study of the role of parenting in children who have high levels both of CU traits and aggression, is warranted. On the one hand this seems to be a particularly high-risk group, and on the other, they might prove to be particularly responsive to positive parenting, and hence perhaps to interventions to promote positive parenting. In a recent publication, Kimonis et al. [55] reported that an adapted parent–child interaction therapy designed to promote positive parenting produced reductions in CP in pre-schoolers with CU traits, which were sustained at a 3-month follow-up. Future investigations which employ longer term follow-up are needed to inform whether targeting positive parenting will result in persistent reductions in CPs in children with CU traits.

Summary

The evidence as to whether positive parenting attenuates the association between CU traits and conduct problems is inconsistent. Building on our previous finding that CU traits predict later aggression specifically in children who are already aggressive, we asked whether positive parenting attenuates this association between CU traits and later aggression. The sample comprised 220 mothers of children (48% female) recruited at age 3.5 years through social media across three regions in Colombia. Mother–child interactions were coded for maternal positive reinforcement and positivity, and mothers reported on their children’s CU traits and aggression at age 3.5 years, and aggression again at 5.0 years. In multiple linear regression, there was a significant three-way interaction between maternal praise, CU traits and aggression (p = 0.025). In the presence of high aggression, high maternal praise was associated with a reduced association between CU traits and later aggression, although this two-way interaction was not significant (p = 0.097). The three-way interaction between maternal positivity, CU traits and aggression was not significant (p = 0.093), but similar to maternal praise, maternal positivity had a protective effect among children already aggressive, and the two-way interaction was significant (p = 0.045). The findings provide evidence that positive parenting, both praise and maternal positivity, moderates the association between CU traits and later aggression in children who are already aggressive. This topic has great potential clinical relevance and future longitudinal studies with an intervention design are required.

Declarations

Conflict of interest

The authors declare that they have no conflict of interest.

Ethical Approval

All procedures performed in studies involving human participants were in accordance with the ethical standards of the institutional or national research committee and with the 1964 Helsinki declaration and its later amendments or comparable ethical standards.
Informed consent was obtained from all individual participants included in the study.
Open AccessThis article is licensed under a Creative Commons Attribution 4.0 International License, which permits use, sharing, adaptation, distribution and reproduction in any medium or format, as long as you give appropriate credit to the original author(s) and the source, provide a link to the Creative Commons licence, and indicate if changes were made. The images or other third party material in this article are included in the article's Creative Commons licence, unless indicated otherwise in a credit line to the material. If material is not included in the article's Creative Commons licence and your intended use is not permitted by statutory regulation or exceeds the permitted use, you will need to obtain permission directly from the copyright holder. To view a copy of this licence, visit http://​creativecommons.​org/​licenses/​by/​4.​0/​.

Publisher's Note

Springer Nature remains neutral with regard to jurisdictional claims in published maps and institutional affiliations.

Onze productaanbevelingen

BSL Psychologie Totaal

Met BSL Psychologie Totaal blijf je als professional steeds op de hoogte van de nieuwste ontwikkelingen binnen jouw vak. Met het online abonnement heb je toegang tot een groot aantal boeken, protocollen, vaktijdschriften en e-learnings op het gebied van psychologie en psychiatrie. Zo kun je op je gemak en wanneer het jou het beste uitkomt verdiepen in jouw vakgebied.

BSL Academy Accare GGZ collective

Bijlagen

Supplementary Information

Below is the link to the electronic supplementary material.
Literatuur
1.
go back to reference Frick PJ, Ray JV, Thornton LC, Kahn RE (2014) Can callous-unemotional traits enhance the understanding, diagnosis, and treatment of serious conduct problems in children and adolescents? A comprehensive review. Psychol Bull 140:1–57PubMedCrossRef Frick PJ, Ray JV, Thornton LC, Kahn RE (2014) Can callous-unemotional traits enhance the understanding, diagnosis, and treatment of serious conduct problems in children and adolescents? A comprehensive review. Psychol Bull 140:1–57PubMedCrossRef
2.
go back to reference Wright N, Hill J, Pickles A, Sharp H (2019) Callous-unemotional traits, low cortisol reactivity and physical aggression in children: findings from the Wirral Child Health and Development Study. Transl Psychiatry 9:79PubMedPubMedCentralCrossRef Wright N, Hill J, Pickles A, Sharp H (2019) Callous-unemotional traits, low cortisol reactivity and physical aggression in children: findings from the Wirral Child Health and Development Study. Transl Psychiatry 9:79PubMedPubMedCentralCrossRef
3.
go back to reference Taubner S, White LO, Zimmermann J, Fonagy P, Nolte T (2013) Attachment-related mentalization moderates the relationship between psychopathic traits and proactive aggression in adolescence. J Abnorm Child Psychol 4:929–938CrossRef Taubner S, White LO, Zimmermann J, Fonagy P, Nolte T (2013) Attachment-related mentalization moderates the relationship between psychopathic traits and proactive aggression in adolescence. J Abnorm Child Psychol 4:929–938CrossRef
4.
go back to reference Kochanska G, Aksan N, Joy ME (2007) Children’s fearfulness as a moderator of parenting in early socialization: two longitudinal studies. Dev Psychol 43:222PubMedCrossRef Kochanska G, Aksan N, Joy ME (2007) Children’s fearfulness as a moderator of parenting in early socialization: two longitudinal studies. Dev Psychol 43:222PubMedCrossRef
5.
go back to reference Kochanska G, Kim S, Boldt LJ, Yoon JE (2013) Children’s callous-unemotional traits moderate links between their positive relationships with parents at preschool age and externalizing behavior problems at early school age. J Child Psychol Psychiatry 54:1251–1260PubMedPubMedCentralCrossRef Kochanska G, Kim S, Boldt LJ, Yoon JE (2013) Children’s callous-unemotional traits moderate links between their positive relationships with parents at preschool age and externalizing behavior problems at early school age. J Child Psychol Psychiatry 54:1251–1260PubMedPubMedCentralCrossRef
6.
go back to reference Obando D, Hill J, Sharp H, Pickles A, Fisher L, Wright N (2020) Synergy between callous–unemotional traits and aggression in preschool children: Cross-informant and cross-cultural replication in the UK Wirral Child Health and Development Study, and the Colombian La Sabana Parent-Child Study. Dev Psychopathol 23:1–9 Obando D, Hill J, Sharp H, Pickles A, Fisher L, Wright N (2020) Synergy between callous–unemotional traits and aggression in preschool children: Cross-informant and cross-cultural replication in the UK Wirral Child Health and Development Study, and the Colombian La Sabana Parent-Child Study. Dev Psychopathol 23:1–9
7.
go back to reference Henry J, Dionne G, Viding E, Petitclerc A, Feng B, Vitaro F et al (2018) A longitudinal twin study of callous-unemotional traits during childhood. J Abnorm Psychol 127:374PubMedCrossRef Henry J, Dionne G, Viding E, Petitclerc A, Feng B, Vitaro F et al (2018) A longitudinal twin study of callous-unemotional traits during childhood. J Abnorm Psychol 127:374PubMedCrossRef
8.
go back to reference Hawes DJ, Dadds MR, Frost AD, Hasking PA (2011) Do childhood callous-unemotional traits drive change in parenting practices? J Clin Child Psychol 40:507–518CrossRef Hawes DJ, Dadds MR, Frost AD, Hasking PA (2011) Do childhood callous-unemotional traits drive change in parenting practices? J Clin Child Psychol 40:507–518CrossRef
9.
go back to reference Waller R, Hyde LW, Klump KL, Burt SA (2018) Parenting is an environmental predictor of callous-unemotional traits and aggression: a monozygotic twin differences study. J Am Acad Child Adolesc Psychiatry 57:955–963PubMedPubMedCentralCrossRef Waller R, Hyde LW, Klump KL, Burt SA (2018) Parenting is an environmental predictor of callous-unemotional traits and aggression: a monozygotic twin differences study. J Am Acad Child Adolesc Psychiatry 57:955–963PubMedPubMedCentralCrossRef
10.
go back to reference Reidy DE, Krusemark E, Kosson DS, Kearns MC, Smith-Darden J, Kiehl KA (2017) The development of severe and chronic violence among youth: the role of psychopathic traits and reward processing. Child Psychiatry Hum Dev 48:967–982PubMedPubMedCentralCrossRef Reidy DE, Krusemark E, Kosson DS, Kearns MC, Smith-Darden J, Kiehl KA (2017) The development of severe and chronic violence among youth: the role of psychopathic traits and reward processing. Child Psychiatry Hum Dev 48:967–982PubMedPubMedCentralCrossRef
11.
go back to reference Pardini DA, Lochman JE, Powell N (2007) The development of callous-unemotional traits and antisocial behavior in children: are there shared and/or unique predictors? J Clin Child Psychol 36:319–333CrossRef Pardini DA, Lochman JE, Powell N (2007) The development of callous-unemotional traits and antisocial behavior in children: are there shared and/or unique predictors? J Clin Child Psychol 36:319–333CrossRef
12.
go back to reference Hyde LW, Shaw DS, Gardner F, Cheong J, Dishion TJ, Wilson M (2013) Dimensions of callousness in early childhood: links to problem behavior and family intervention effectiveness. Dev Psychopathol 25:347PubMedPubMedCentralCrossRef Hyde LW, Shaw DS, Gardner F, Cheong J, Dishion TJ, Wilson M (2013) Dimensions of callousness in early childhood: links to problem behavior and family intervention effectiveness. Dev Psychopathol 25:347PubMedPubMedCentralCrossRef
13.
go back to reference Kroneman LM, Hipwell AE, Loeber R, Koot HM, Pardini DA (2011) Contextual risk factors as predictors of disruptive behavior disorder trajectories in girls: the moderating effect of callous-unemotional features. J Child Psychol Psychiatry 52:167–175PubMedCrossRef Kroneman LM, Hipwell AE, Loeber R, Koot HM, Pardini DA (2011) Contextual risk factors as predictors of disruptive behavior disorder trajectories in girls: the moderating effect of callous-unemotional features. J Child Psychol Psychiatry 52:167–175PubMedCrossRef
14.
go back to reference Pasalich DS, Dadds MR, Hawes DJ, Brennan J (2011) Do callous-unemotional traits moderate the relative importance of parental coercion versus warmth in child conduct problems? An observational study. J Child Psychol Psychiatry 52:1308–1315PubMedCrossRef Pasalich DS, Dadds MR, Hawes DJ, Brennan J (2011) Do callous-unemotional traits moderate the relative importance of parental coercion versus warmth in child conduct problems? An observational study. J Child Psychol Psychiatry 52:1308–1315PubMedCrossRef
15.
go back to reference Magaña AB, Goldstein MJ, Karno M, Miklowitz DJ, Jenkins J, Falloon IR (1986) A brief method for assessing expressed emotion in relatives of psychiatric patients. Psychiatry Res 17:203–212PubMedCrossRef Magaña AB, Goldstein MJ, Karno M, Miklowitz DJ, Jenkins J, Falloon IR (1986) A brief method for assessing expressed emotion in relatives of psychiatric patients. Psychiatry Res 17:203–212PubMedCrossRef
16.
go back to reference Clark JE, Frick PJ (2016) Positive parenting and callous-unemotional traits: their association with school behavior problems in young children. J Clin Child Psychol 47:1–13 Clark JE, Frick PJ (2016) Positive parenting and callous-unemotional traits: their association with school behavior problems in young children. J Clin Child Psychol 47:1–13
17.
go back to reference Frick PJ (1991) The Alabama parenting questionnaire. Unpublished rating scale, University of Alabama Frick PJ (1991) The Alabama parenting questionnaire. Unpublished rating scale, University of Alabama
18.
go back to reference Wall TD, Frick PJ, Fanti KA, Kimonis ER, Lordos A (2016) Factors differentiating callous-unemotional children with and without conduct problems. J Child Psychol Psychiatry 57:976–983PubMedCrossRef Wall TD, Frick PJ, Fanti KA, Kimonis ER, Lordos A (2016) Factors differentiating callous-unemotional children with and without conduct problems. J Child Psychol Psychiatry 57:976–983PubMedCrossRef
20.
go back to reference Wang MC, Shou Y, Liang J, Lai H, Zeng H, Chen L et al (2019) Further validation of the inventory of callous–unemotional traits in Chinese children: cross-informants invariance and longitudinal invariance. Assessment 27:1668–1680PubMedCrossRef Wang MC, Shou Y, Liang J, Lai H, Zeng H, Chen L et al (2019) Further validation of the inventory of callous–unemotional traits in Chinese children: cross-informants invariance and longitudinal invariance. Assessment 27:1668–1680PubMedCrossRef
21.
go back to reference Zhang X, Shou Y, Wang M, Zhong C, Luo J, Gao Y et al (2019) Assessing callous-unemotional traits in Chinese detained boys: factor structure and construct validity of the inventory of callous-unemotional traits. Front Psychol 10:1841PubMedPubMedCentralCrossRef Zhang X, Shou Y, Wang M, Zhong C, Luo J, Gao Y et al (2019) Assessing callous-unemotional traits in Chinese detained boys: factor structure and construct validity of the inventory of callous-unemotional traits. Front Psychol 10:1841PubMedPubMedCentralCrossRef
22.
go back to reference Deater-Deckard K, Lansford JE, Malone PS, Alampay LP, Sorbring E, Bacchini D, Bombi AS, Bornstein MH, Chang L, Di Giunta L, Dodge KA (2011) The association between parental warmth and control in thirteen cultural groups. J Fam Psychol 25:790PubMedPubMedCentralCrossRef Deater-Deckard K, Lansford JE, Malone PS, Alampay LP, Sorbring E, Bacchini D, Bombi AS, Bornstein MH, Chang L, Di Giunta L, Dodge KA (2011) The association between parental warmth and control in thirteen cultural groups. J Fam Psychol 25:790PubMedPubMedCentralCrossRef
23.
go back to reference Obando D, Wright N, Pickles A, Hill J (2020) Positive and negative parenting, callous-unemotional traits and oppositional defiant disorder behaviours in preschool Colombian children. Unpublished manuscript Obando D, Wright N, Pickles A, Hill J (2020) Positive and negative parenting, callous-unemotional traits and oppositional defiant disorder behaviours in preschool Colombian children. Unpublished manuscript
24.
go back to reference Sng KI, Hawes DJ, Raine A, Ang RP, Ooi YP, Fung DS (2018) Callous unemotional traits and the relationship between aggressive parenting practices and conduct problems in Singaporean families. Child Abuse Negl 81:225–234PubMedCrossRef Sng KI, Hawes DJ, Raine A, Ang RP, Ooi YP, Fung DS (2018) Callous unemotional traits and the relationship between aggressive parenting practices and conduct problems in Singaporean families. Child Abuse Negl 81:225–234PubMedCrossRef
28.
go back to reference Buitrago-Lopez A, Van den Hooven EH, Rueda-Clausen CF, Serrano N, Ruiz AJ, Pereira MA, Mueller NT (2015) Socioeconomic status is positively associated with measures of adiposity and insulin resistance, but inversely associated with dyslipidaemia in Colombian children. J Epidemiol Community Health 69:580–587PubMedCrossRef Buitrago-Lopez A, Van den Hooven EH, Rueda-Clausen CF, Serrano N, Ruiz AJ, Pereira MA, Mueller NT (2015) Socioeconomic status is positively associated with measures of adiposity and insulin resistance, but inversely associated with dyslipidaemia in Colombian children. J Epidemiol Community Health 69:580–587PubMedCrossRef
29.
go back to reference Departamento Administrativo Nacional de Estadística – DANE (2011) Nueva metodología de estratificación socioeconómica. Departamento Administrativo Nacional de Estadistica. Dirección de Geoestadística. Grupo de Diseño Nueva Metodología de Estratificación, Mimeo. Departamento Administrativo Nacional de Estadística – DANE (2011) Nueva metodología de estratificación socioeconómica. Departamento Administrativo Nacional de Estadistica. Dirección de Geoestadística. Grupo de Diseño Nueva Metodología de Estratificación, Mimeo.
33.
go back to reference Kimonis ER, Fanti KA, Anastassiou-Hadjicharalambous X, Mertan B, Goulter N, Katsimicha E (2016) Can callous-unemotional traits be reliably measured in preschoolers? J Abnorm Psychol 44:625–638CrossRef Kimonis ER, Fanti KA, Anastassiou-Hadjicharalambous X, Mertan B, Goulter N, Katsimicha E (2016) Can callous-unemotional traits be reliably measured in preschoolers? J Abnorm Psychol 44:625–638CrossRef
34.
go back to reference Willoughby MT, Mills-Koonce WR, Waschbusch DA, Gottfredson NC (2015) An examination of the parent report version of the inventory of callous-unemotional traits in a community sample of first-grade children. Assessment 22:76–85PubMedCrossRef Willoughby MT, Mills-Koonce WR, Waschbusch DA, Gottfredson NC (2015) An examination of the parent report version of the inventory of callous-unemotional traits in a community sample of first-grade children. Assessment 22:76–85PubMedCrossRef
35.
go back to reference Ray JV, Frick PJ, Thornton LC, Steinberg L, Cauffman E (2016) Positive and negative item wording and its influence on the assessment of callous-unemotional traits. Psychol Assess 28:394PubMedCrossRef Ray JV, Frick PJ, Thornton LC, Steinberg L, Cauffman E (2016) Positive and negative item wording and its influence on the assessment of callous-unemotional traits. Psychol Assess 28:394PubMedCrossRef
36.
go back to reference Achenbach TM, Rescorla LA (2000) Manual for the ASEBA preschool forms and profiles: an integrated system of multi-informant assessment. University of Vermont Department of Psychiatry, Burlington Achenbach TM, Rescorla LA (2000) Manual for the ASEBA preschool forms and profiles: an integrated system of multi-informant assessment. University of Vermont Department of Psychiatry, Burlington
37.
go back to reference Hewitt N, Vila J, Juárez F (2016) Predicción de los problemas de banda ancha y banda estrecha mediante la integración de los modelos transaccional, cognitivo-social y de regulación emocional (Doctoral dissertation, Tesis Doctoral). http://hdl.handle.net/10481/42435). accessed 17 Mar 2017 Hewitt N, Vila J, Juárez F (2016) Predicción de los problemas de banda ancha y banda estrecha mediante la integración de los modelos transaccional, cognitivo-social y de regulación emocional (Doctoral dissertation, Tesis Doctoral). http://​hdl.​handle.​net/​10481/​42435). accessed 17 Mar 2017
38.
go back to reference Child Agency code from NICHD Early Child Care Research Network (1999) Child care and mother–child interaction in the first 3 years of life. Dev Psychol 35:1399–1413CrossRef Child Agency code from NICHD Early Child Care Research Network (1999) Child care and mother–child interaction in the first 3 years of life. Dev Psychol 35:1399–1413CrossRef
39.
go back to reference Deater-Deckard K (2000) Parenting and child behavioral adjustment in early childhood: a quantitative genetic approach to studying family processes. Child Dev 71:468–484PubMedCrossRef Deater-Deckard K (2000) Parenting and child behavioral adjustment in early childhood: a quantitative genetic approach to studying family processes. Child Dev 71:468–484PubMedCrossRef
40.
go back to reference Deater-Deckard K, Li M, Bell MA (2016) Multifaceted emotion regulation, stress and affect in mothers of young children. Cogn Emot 30:444–457PubMedCrossRef Deater-Deckard K, Li M, Bell MA (2016) Multifaceted emotion regulation, stress and affect in mothers of young children. Cogn Emot 30:444–457PubMedCrossRef
41.
go back to reference Eyberg SM, Nelson MM, Duke M, Boggs SR (2004) Manual for the dyadic parent- child interaction coding system, 3rd edn. University of Florida, Gainesville Eyberg SM, Nelson MM, Duke M, Boggs SR (2004) Manual for the dyadic parent- child interaction coding system, 3rd edn. University of Florida, Gainesville
42.
go back to reference Cox JL, Holden JM, Sagovsky R (1987) Detection of postnatal depression: development of the 10-item Edinburgh Postnatal Depression Scale. BJPsych 150:782–786PubMed Cox JL, Holden JM, Sagovsky R (1987) Detection of postnatal depression: development of the 10-item Edinburgh Postnatal Depression Scale. BJPsych 150:782–786PubMed
43.
go back to reference Huntley F, Wright N, Pickles A, Sharp H, Hill J (2017) Maternal mental health and child problem behaviours: disentangling the role of depression and borderline personality dysfunction. BJPsych 3:300–305CrossRef Huntley F, Wright N, Pickles A, Sharp H, Hill J (2017) Maternal mental health and child problem behaviours: disentangling the role of depression and borderline personality dysfunction. BJPsych 3:300–305CrossRef
44.
go back to reference Campo-Arias A, Ayola-Castillo C, Peinado-Valencia HM, Amor-Parra M, Cogollo Z (2007) Escala de Edinburgh para depresión posparto: consistencia interna y estructura factorial en mujeres embarazadas de Cartagena, Colombia. Rev Colomb Obstet Ginecol 58:277–283CrossRef Campo-Arias A, Ayola-Castillo C, Peinado-Valencia HM, Amor-Parra M, Cogollo Z (2007) Escala de Edinburgh para depresión posparto: consistencia interna y estructura factorial en mujeres embarazadas de Cartagena, Colombia. Rev Colomb Obstet Ginecol 58:277–283CrossRef
45.
go back to reference Wasserstein RL, Lazar NA (2016) The ASA statement on p-values: context, process, and purpose. Am Stat 70:129–133CrossRef Wasserstein RL, Lazar NA (2016) The ASA statement on p-values: context, process, and purpose. Am Stat 70:129–133CrossRef
46.
go back to reference Waller R, Hyde LW, Grabell AS, Alves ML, Olson SL (2015) Differential associations of early callous-unemotional, oppositional, and ADHD behaviors: multiple domains within early-starting conduct problems? J Child Psychol Psychiatry 56:657–666PubMedCrossRef Waller R, Hyde LW, Grabell AS, Alves ML, Olson SL (2015) Differential associations of early callous-unemotional, oppositional, and ADHD behaviors: multiple domains within early-starting conduct problems? J Child Psychol Psychiatry 56:657–666PubMedCrossRef
47.
go back to reference Colins OF, Andershed H, Frogner L, Lopez-Romero L, Veen V, Andershed AK (2014) A new measure to assess psychopathic personality in children: The Child Problematic Traits Inventory. J Psychopathol Behav Assess 3:4–21CrossRef Colins OF, Andershed H, Frogner L, Lopez-Romero L, Veen V, Andershed AK (2014) A new measure to assess psychopathic personality in children: The Child Problematic Traits Inventory. J Psychopathol Behav Assess 3:4–21CrossRef
48.
go back to reference Hare RD (1991) Manual for the revised psychopathy checklist, 1st edn. Multi-Health Systems, Toronto Hare RD (1991) Manual for the revised psychopathy checklist, 1st edn. Multi-Health Systems, Toronto
49.
go back to reference Levenson M, Kiehl K, Fitzpatrick C (1995) Assessing psychopathic attributes in a noninstitutionalized population. J Pers Soc Psychol 68:151–158PubMedCrossRef Levenson M, Kiehl K, Fitzpatrick C (1995) Assessing psychopathic attributes in a noninstitutionalized population. J Pers Soc Psychol 68:151–158PubMedCrossRef
50.
go back to reference Allen JL, Shou Y, Wang MC, Bird E (2020) Assessing the measurement invariance of the Inventory of Callous-Unemotional traits in school students in China and the United Kingdom. Child Psychiatry Hum Dev 52:1–12 Allen JL, Shou Y, Wang MC, Bird E (2020) Assessing the measurement invariance of the Inventory of Callous-Unemotional traits in school students in China and the United Kingdom. Child Psychiatry Hum Dev 52:1–12
51.
go back to reference Blair R, Colledge E, Murray L, Mitchell D (2001) A selective impairment in the processing of sad and fearful expressions in children with psychopathic tendencies. J Abnorm Psychol 29:491–498CrossRef Blair R, Colledge E, Murray L, Mitchell D (2001) A selective impairment in the processing of sad and fearful expressions in children with psychopathic tendencies. J Abnorm Psychol 29:491–498CrossRef
52.
go back to reference Frick PJ, White SF (2008) Research review: The importance of callous-unemotional traits for developmental models of aggressive and antisocial behavior. J Child Psychol Psychiatry 49:359–375PubMedCrossRef Frick PJ, White SF (2008) Research review: The importance of callous-unemotional traits for developmental models of aggressive and antisocial behavior. J Child Psychol Psychiatry 49:359–375PubMedCrossRef
53.
go back to reference Kochanska G (1997) Multiple pathways to conscience for children with different temperaments: from toddlerhood to age 5. Dev Psychol 33:228PubMedCrossRef Kochanska G (1997) Multiple pathways to conscience for children with different temperaments: from toddlerhood to age 5. Dev Psychol 33:228PubMedCrossRef
54.
go back to reference López-Romero L, Cutrín O, Maneiro L, Domínguez-Álvarez B, Romero E (2021) Psychopathic traits in childhood: insights from parental warmth and fearless temperament via conscience development. Brain Sci 11:923PubMedPubMedCentralCrossRef López-Romero L, Cutrín O, Maneiro L, Domínguez-Álvarez B, Romero E (2021) Psychopathic traits in childhood: insights from parental warmth and fearless temperament via conscience development. Brain Sci 11:923PubMedPubMedCentralCrossRef
55.
go back to reference Kimonis ER, Fleming G, Briggs N, Brouwer-French L, Frick PJ, Hawes DJ et al (2019) Parent–child interaction therapy adapted for preschoolers with callous-unemotional traits: an open trial pilot study. J Clin Child Psychol 48:S347–S361CrossRef Kimonis ER, Fleming G, Briggs N, Brouwer-French L, Frick PJ, Hawes DJ et al (2019) Parent–child interaction therapy adapted for preschoolers with callous-unemotional traits: an open trial pilot study. J Clin Child Psychol 48:S347–S361CrossRef
Metagegevens
Titel
The Moderating Role of Maternal Praise and Positivity in the Association Between Callous-Unemotional (CU) Traits and Later Aggression: A Prospective Study in Preschool Children in Colombia
Auteurs
Diana Obando
Jonathan Hill
Nicola Wright
Publicatiedatum
16-06-2022
Uitgeverij
Springer US
Gepubliceerd in
Child Psychiatry & Human Development / Uitgave 1/2024
Print ISSN: 0009-398X
Elektronisch ISSN: 1573-3327
DOI
https://doi.org/10.1007/s10578-022-01354-3

Andere artikelen Uitgave 1/2024

Child Psychiatry & Human Development 1/2024 Naar de uitgave