Student Culture and Composition and the Development of Gender Role Attitudes
To get an impression of the general development of gender role attitudes, Table
3 provides the initial gender role attitudes and the two differences scores over the course of Grade 7 and between Grades 7 and 8. Boys expressed significantly more traditional gender role attitudes than girls (boys: B = 1.766,
SE = 0.042; girls: B = 1.312,
SE = 0.026; Diff (Δ) b-g = 0.455,
p < 0.001). Concerning the development, boys developed significantly more traditional gender role attitudes over the course of Grade 7 (Diff 2-1, B = 0.129,
SE = 0.022,
p < 0.001) and between Grades 7 and 8 (Diff 3-1, B = 0.257,
SE = 0.026,
p < 0.001). Girls, on the other hand, developed significantly less traditional gender role attitudes over the course of Grade 7 (Diff 2-1, B = −0.085,
SE = 0.018,
p < 0.001) and between Grades 7 and 8 (Diff 3-1, B = −0.140,
SE = 0.023,
p < 0.001).
Table 3
Baseline change model of the general development of male and female adolescents and their differences in the developmental trajectories
Gender role attitudes (t1) | 1.766 (0.042) | 1.312 (0.026) | 0.455*** |
Diff 2-1 | 0.129 (0.022)*** | −0.085 (0.018)*** | 0.214*** |
Diff 3-1 | 0.257 (0.026)*** | −0.140 (0.023)*** | 0.396*** |
To answer the first research question regarding how students’ gender role culture, sex composition, and SES composition were associated with the initial gender role attitudes in Grade 7 and their development over a two-year course, correlational analyses provide initial insights (Table
4). Focusing on boys first, it appears that students’ gender role culture was positively correlated with boys’ gender role attitudes at all three measurement points. This suggests that boys in schools with a more traditional student gender role culture express more individual traditional attitudes. A higher proportion of female students was associated with less traditional attitudes in boys — and, as expected, a more privileged SES composition of students was associated with less traditional gender role attitudes at all three measurement points.
Concerning the students’ gender role culture, a similar pattern emerged for girls. As expected, a more traditional gender role culture among students was associated with more traditional attitudes among girls at the individual level. Despite the assumptions, the students’ sex composition was seemingly unrelated to girls’ gender-role attitudes at all three measurement points. However, in line with the hypothesis, a more privileged SES composition among students was associated with less traditional gender role attitudes among girls at all three measurement points.
In a multivariate perspective, a different pattern emerged regarding the students’ compositional factors and the degree of traditional gender role attitudes at the first measurement point (Table
5). As already shown in the bivariate analyses, the students’ gender role culture was positively associated with boys’ and girls’ initial gender role attitudes (M1, boys: β = 0.461,
SE = 0.038,
p < 0.001; girls: β = 0.258,
SE = 0.054,
p < 0.001). The difference between these effects were significant, indicating that boys are more sensitive to the students’ gender role culture (M1, Diff (Δ) b-g, B = −0.615,
p < 0.001). However, the sex composition of the student body was positively associated with boys’ traditional gender role attitudes (M1, β = 0.261,
SE = 0.031,
p < 0.001) — a higher proportion of female students was associated with initially more traditional gender role attitudes. For girls, there were no significant effects concerning the students’ sex composition and traditional initial gender role attitudes in a multivariate perspective. The difference between these effects were significant (M1, Diff (Δ) b-g, B = −0.722,
p < 0.001). Students’ SES composition was only significantly associated with traditional initial gender role attitudes for boys (M1, β = 0.105,
SE = 0.036,
p = 0.003), which suggests that a more privileged SES composition is correlated with more traditional gender role attitudes among boys but not for girls. However, the difference between these effects was not significant.
Turning to the implications of students’ compositional factors for the development of gender role attitudes among boys and girls, the results indicate that boys developed in a less traditional direction in contexts with a more traditional student culture over the course of Grade 7 (M1, Diff 2-1, β = −0.189, SE = 0.062, p = 0.002) and between Grades 7 and 8 (M1, Diff 3-1, β = −0.264, SE = 0.059, p < 0.001). No significant effects emerged for the students’ sex composition and the development of gender role attitudes in boys. In line with the assumptions, a more privileged student SES composition was negatively associated with the development of traditional gender role attitudes in boys. This indicates that boys developed less traditional gender role attitudes over the course of Grade 7 (M1, Diff 2-1, β = −0.192, SE = 0.066, p = 0.004) and between Grades 7 and 8 (M1, Diff 3-1, β = −0.158, SE = 0.055, p = 0.004). Contrary to the assumptions, students’ cultural and compositional factors were unrelated to the development of girls’ gender role attitudes. Comparing the genders, the results indicate that boys are more sensitive to students’ gender role culture over the course of Grade 7 (M1, Diff (Δ) b-g, B = 0.420, p = 0.002) and between Grades 7 and 8 (M1, Diff (Δ) b-g, B = 0.486, p = 0.027). Compared to girls (M1, Diff (Δ) b-g, B = 0.085, p = 0.012), boys also develop less traditional gender role attitudes in schools with a higher SES composition over the course of Grade 7. The effect and difference patterns concerning the students’ compositional indicators and the degree of boys and girls traditional gender role attitudes at the beginning of Grade 7 and the development over the two-year period remained stable when controlling for teacher effects (Model 3).
Teacher Culture and Composition and the Development of Gender Role Attitudes
To answer how teachers’ gender role culture and sex composition were associated with the initial gender role attitudes of boys and girls, correlation analyses were conducted (Table
4). The initial gender role attitudes among boys and girls in Grade 7 were not significantly correlated with teachers’ gender role culture. However, teachers’ gender role culture was associated with boys’ gender role attitudes at the end of Grade 8, indicating that boys expressed more traditional gender role attitudes in schools with a more traditional teacher culture. The sex composition of the teaching staff was only significantly correlated with boys’ initial gender role attitudes at the beginning of Grade 7 and by the end of Grade 8, indicating that more female teachers in a school were associated with less traditional attitudes in boys. For girls, no significant associations between gender role attitudes and teachers’ gender role culture and sex composition were apparent.
Table 4
Correlational analyses separately for boys and girls
1 | Gender role attitudes (t1) | | 0.714*** | 0.637*** | 0.306*** | −0.053 | −0.257*** | 0.038 | 0.005 | −0.224*** | 0.441*** |
2 | Gender role attitudes (t2) | 0.718*** | | 0.713*** | 0.319*** | −0.069 | −0.280*** | 0.053 | 0.005 | −0.255*** | 0.354*** |
3 | Gender role attitudes (t3) | 0.612*** | 0.745*** | | 0.275*** | −0.061 | −0.247*** | 0.046 | −0.019 | −0.243*** | 0.289*** |
4 | Students’ gender role culture | 0.358*** | 0.258*** | 0.248*** | | −0.394*** | −0.761*** | 0.158 | −0.159 | −0.371*** | 0.084*** |
5 | Students’ sex composition | −0.175*** | −0.087* | −0.105** | −0.720*** | | 0.090 | −0.088 | 0.508 | 0.046 | 0.019 |
6 | Students’ SES composition | −0.296*** | −0.259*** | −0.246*** | −0.771*** | 0.412*** | | −0.108 | 0.249 | 0.491*** | −0.054* |
7 | Teachers’ gender role culture | 0.100 | 0.069 | 0.117** | 0.308* | −0.295 | −0.155 | | 0.028 | −0.029 | 0.083** |
8 | Teachers’ sex composition | −0.115* | −0.050 | −0.093* | −0.457*** | 0.633*** | 0.443*** | −0.319* | | 0.123 | 0.042 |
9 | Parental SES | −0.262*** | −0.242*** | −0.239*** | −0.365*** | 0.202*** | 0.461*** | −0.081 | 0.207*** | | −0.060** |
10 | Perceived peer pressure | 0.450*** | 0.354*** | 0.333*** | 0.144*** | −0.143*** | −0.095*** | 0.104** | −0.127*** | −0.095*** | |
The baseline change models confirm the correlational results indicating that teachers’ gender role culture was not associated with initial gender role attitudes among boys and girls in Grade 7 (Table
5, Model 2). The teachers’ sex composition correlated with traditional initial attitudes in boys (M2, β = 0.078,
SE = 0.038,
p = 0.038), suggesting that more female teachers in a school are associated with initially more traditional gender role attitudes among boys but not for girls. However, this difference was not significant. When controlling for the students’ cultural and compositional factors (M3), the significant effect of teachers’ sex composition for boys vanishes. For girls, a small significant effect of teachers’ gender role culture on the traditional initial attitudes for girls emerged, indicating that a more traditional teacher culture was associated with initially less traditional attitudes in girls (M3, β = −0.057,
SE = 0.028,
p = 0.044).
Table 5
Baseline change models to predict the development of gender role attitudes between Grade 7 and 8
Intercept | | | | | | | | | |
Students’ gender role culture | 0.461 (0.038)*** | 0.258 (0.054)*** | *** | | | | 0.461 (0.037)*** | 0.245 (0.048)*** | *** |
Students’ sex composition | 0.261 (0.031)*** | 0.019 (0.024) | *** | | | | 0.247 (0.038)*** | −0.009 (0.039) | *** |
Students’ SES composition | 0.105 (0.036)** | 0.057 (0.039) | n.s. | | | | 0.100 (0.032)** | 0.037 (0.041) | n.s. |
Teachers’ gender role culture | | | | 0.015 (0.042) | −0.043 (0.039) | n.s. | −0.021 (0.018) | −0.057 (0.028)* | n.s. |
Teachers’ sex composition | | | | 0.078 (0.038)* | 0.003 (0.041) | n.s. | 0.017 (0.021) | 0.034 (0.040) | n.s. |
Parental SES | −0.104 (0.033)** | −0.093 (0.025)*** | | −0.136 (0.033)*** | −0.124 (0.026)*** | | −0.104 (0.033)** | −0.090 (0.025)*** | |
Perceived peer pressure | 0.442 (0.022)*** | 0.444 (0.020)*** | | 0.456 (0.024)*** | 0.456 (0.020)*** | | 0.444 (0.023)*** | 0.446 (0.020)*** | |
Vocational track | 0.191 (0.032)*** | 0.144 (0.035)*** | | 0.288 (0.044)*** | 0.196 (0.035)*** | | 0.192 (0.033)*** | 0.149 (0.035)*** | |
Art track | −0.020 (0.015) | −0.003 (0.029) | | −0.016 (0.017) | −0.008 (0.023) | | −0.023 (0.014) | −0.008 (0.028) | |
Technical track | 0.024 (0.025) | 0.043 (0.035) | | 0.110 (0.030)*** | 0.092 (0.033)** | | 0.028 (0.027) | 0.055 (0.035) | |
Diff 2-1 | | | | | | | | | |
Students’ gender role culture | −0.189 (0.062)** | 0.020 (0.067) | ** | | | | −0.247 (0.071)** | −0.021 (0.067) | ** |
Students’ sex composition | −0.022 (0.057) | −0.002 (0.048) | n.s. | | | | −0.096 (0.079) | −0.039 (0.067) | n.s. |
Students’ SES composition | −0.192 (0.066)** | −0.061 (0.083) | * | | | | −0.250 (0.073)** | −0.110 (0.082) | * |
Teachers’ gender role culture | | | | 0.017 (0.035) | 0.051 (0.045) | n.s. | 0.044 (0.037) | 0.043 (0.049) | n.s. |
Teachers’ sex composition | | | | 0.019 (0.040) | 0.018 (0.048) | n.s. | 0.094 (0.055) | 0.064 (0.058) | n.s. |
Parental SES | −0.049 (0.032) | −0.079 (0.037)* | | −0.092 (0.037)* | −0.107 (0.032)** | | −0.050 (0.032) | −0.084 (0.037)* | |
Perceived peer pressure | −0.096 (0.037)* | −0.116 (0.036)** | | −0.100 (0.038)** | −0.116 (0.035)** | | −0.094 (0.038)* | −0.117 (0.036)** | |
Vocational track | −0.153 (0.047)** | −0.064 (0.065) | | −0.176 (0.055)** | −0.054 (0.062) | | −0.159 (0.048)** | −0.074 (0.065) | |
Art track | −0.041 (0.020)* | −0.001 (0.051) | | −0.027 (0.014) | 0.006 (0.051) | | −0.040 (0.018)* | 0.001 (0.048) | |
Technical track | −0.073 (0.046) | −0.047 (0.054) | | −0.106 (0.046)* | −0.057 (0.049) | | −0.093 (0.049) | −0.076 (0.055) | |
Diff 3-1 | | | | | | | | | |
Students’ gender role culture | −0.264 (0.059)*** | −0.048 (0.089) | * | | | | −0.313 (0.055)*** | −0.057 (0.097) | * |
Students’ sex composition | −0.074 (0.054) | −0.007 (0.049) | n.s. | | | | −0.081 (0.063) | −0.005 (0.062) | n.s. |
Students’ SES composition | −0.158 (0.055)** | −0.076 (0.084) | n.s. | | | | −0.181 (0.059)** | −0.081 (0.093) | n.s. |
Teachers’ gender role culture | | | | 0.075 (0.047) | 0.042 (0.058) | n.s. | 0.112 (0.042)** | 0.045 (0.056) | n.s. |
Teachers’ sex composition | | | | −0.026 (0.034) | −0.012 (0.047) | n.s. | 0.005 (0.044) | 0.004 (0.061) | n.s. |
Parental SES | −0.035 (0.041) | −0.088 (0.035)* | | −0.043 (0.038) | −0.096 (0.037)* | | −0.034 (0.041) | −0.093 (0.036)** | |
Perceived peer pressure | −0.152 (0.043)*** | −0.192 (0.039)*** | | −0.168 (0.044)*** | −0.194 (0.037)*** | | −0.156 (0.044)*** | −0.195 (0.038)*** | |
Vocational track | −0.068 (0.040) | −0.113 (0.056)* | | −0.135 (0.041)** | −0.107 (0.059) | | −0.075 (0.040) | −0.118 (0.058)* | |
Art track | −0.037 (0.013)** | −0.068 (0.024)** | | −0.033 (0.016)* | −0.059 (0.027)* | | −0.032 (0.017) | −0.062 (0.026)* | |
Technical track | −0.010 (0.046) | −0.053 (0.038) | | −0.076 (0.035)* | −0.046 (0.045) | | −0.029 (0.048) | −0.067 (0.043) | |
When focusing on the implications of teacher effects for the development of gender role attitudes, no significant effects for either boys or girls were found (M2). However, when controlling for the students’ compositional factors, a significant effect of the teachers’ gender role culture on the development of gender role attitudes between Grade 7 and 8 for boys emerged (M3, Diff 3-1, β = 0.112, SE = 0.042, p = 0.009), indicating that boys are developing in a more traditional direction in schools with a more traditional teacher culture. For girls, no significant effects of the teachers’ cultural and compositional factors on the development of gender role attitudes were apparent. Although some of the effects of teacher composition and culture showed up for only one gender, the effect differences were not statistically significant when controlling for the student culture and composition, suggesting that the factors are not inherently different in their effects between the genders.
Sensitivity Analyses
Due to the special tracking system in Belgium, the schools provide different, potentially gendered, environments (Van Houtte & Vantieghem,
2020). There are more boys in the technical tracks, and more girls in the art tracks. Thus, gendered contexts arise as a result of the choice behavior, which in turn can reinforce traditional gender roles (cf. Eagly et al.,
2020). Therefore, it was important to control for the different tracks in the latent change models from the beginning. However, when performing an analysis without the control variables, stable patterns of the developmental effects were found compared to the presented results of the analysis with the control variables. Differences were evident in boys’ initial attitudes, since SES composition did not significantly correlate with boys’ initial gender role attitudes when not controlling for parental SES and school track. For girls, teachers’ gender role culture was not significantly associated with their initial gender role attitudes. This highlights the importance of controlling for the different school tracks, which can be gendered due to the tracking system.
Due to the longitudinal design, selective dropout cannot always be ruled out. Usually, dropout is rather systematic, with higher achieving and socially positively selected students showing a higher compliance (e.g., Damian et al.,
2015). Therefore, it was tested whether the dropout was systematically related to the relevant constructs used in the present study. Examining non-participation in the last wave, the results reveal (Table
7 in the appendix) that panel mortality was statistically stronger among students from less socioeconomically privileged families, students with a migration background and students with more traditional gender role attitudes. As this attrition has a systematic component related to gender role attitudes, it is essential to include all individuals in the analyses and not using missing data strategies such as pairwise or listwise deletion, as these rely on more restrictive assumptions (i.e., missing completely at random, which is not the case here, Graham,
2009). FIML was used to retain all students, which minimizes the risk of bias due to selective dropout and maintains maximal test power, as all available information is used. However, to check for robustness, an analysis was conducted with students who participated at all three measurement points (i.e., relying on casewise deletion strategy;
N = 4322). The overall pattern of students’ and teachers’ culture and composition and the development of gender role attitudes remained the same for male and female adolescents. In the full model, no significant effects were found regarding teachers’ gender role culture and girls’ initial gender role attitudes. For boys, the negative effect of students’ sex composition and the positive effect of teachers’ gender role culture on the gender role attitude development during Grade 7 turned significant when only considering students who participated at all measurement points. However, it is unclear whether this is due to the more restrictively selected sample, the reduced test power, or even biased due to the assumptions this sample selection makes (missing completely at random which does not apply here, see Table
7).
As previous research presented overall developmental models in the form of growth curve models, the present study used developmental models that were more sensitive to changes between the measurement points. Therefore, baseline change models were used, which offered the opportunity to examine interindividual differences in intraindividual changes on an error-free measurement level (Geiser,
2011). However, to check for robustness, latent growth curve models were examined, which reproduced and supported the results from the baseline change models.