Skip to main content
Top
Gepubliceerd in: Psychological Research 1-2/2004

01-12-2004 | Original Article

Recognizing one’s own clapping: The role of temporal cues

Auteurs: Rüdiger Flach, Günther Knoblich, Wolfgang Prinz

Gepubliceerd in: Psychological Research | Uitgave 1-2/2004

Log in om toegang te krijgen
share
DELEN

Deel dit onderdeel of sectie (kopieer de link)

  • Optie A:
    Klik op de rechtermuisknop op de link en selecteer de optie “linkadres kopiëren”
  • Optie B:
    Deel de link per e-mail

Abstract

Do individuals recognize their own clapping? We investigated this question with an off-line paradigm in which the perception of the claps is temporally separated from their generation. In a first experiment, we demonstrated that acoustic recordings of clapping provide sufficient information for discriminating between one’s own performance and that of another participant. Self-recognition was still preserved when the claps were reproduced by uniform tones that only retained their temporal pattern. In a subsequent experiment, we verified that the general tempo provides an important cue for self-recognition. Finally, we showed that self-recognition does not rely on concurrent synchronization of actual movements with the auditory sequences. The results are discussed in the light of current theories about perception-action links.
Voetnoten
1
We are grateful to Bruno Repp for suggesting this interpretation. In Repp’s (1987) study, the participants were not asked to maintain a constant tempo in the recording session. In addition, they were not asked to accompany the recorded claps with synchronization responses in the test session.
 
2
We determined the general tempo of a sequence by the average of all complete sequence periods. The accuracy of sequence reproduction was determined by the ratios of adjacent intervals. These observed ratios were then compared with the expected ratios, as indicated in Fig. 1, by calculating the differences, squaring them, adding up the squared differences, and deriving the square root of the sum. This variable error has the advantage that it is logically independent of the differences in general tempo. Next, we assessed the reliability of these inter-individual difference measures. Overall correlations of these measures between the two sessions as well as individual comparisons for each participant pair showed that the general tempo, but not the accuracy, of sequence reproduction was a reliable inter-individual difference measure. A standard regression analysis of the self-recognition measure revealed only general tempo to be a significant predictor. Pairs of participants who showed large differences in general tempo were more likely to recognize their own clapping sequences than pairs of participants who did not show large differences in general tempo.
 
3
In contrast to Experiment 1, Experiment 2 revealed a significant effect of authorship on synchronization performance. Participants were less variable when synchronizing with self-generated sequences than when synchronizing with other sequences. Moreover, the more variable the participant’s synchronization performance was with regard to the other participant’s sequences than with regard to his or her own sequences, the more often the self-recognition judgments were correct. The fact that the analysis of the synchronization performance did not reveal more significant effects of authorship may be rooted in the fact that the overall synchronization performance was rather variable and error-prone. This fact was especially challenging for the pre-analysis of the synchronization data. Nonetheless, the analysis of the synchronization data reliably showed significant block effects for the constant and the variable error of synchronization, decreasing with increasing practice.
 
Literatuur
go back to reference Decety, J., & Jeannerod, M. (1996). Mentally simulated movements in virtual reality: Does Fitt’s law hold in motor imagery. Behavioural Brain Research, 72, 127–134.CrossRef Decety, J., & Jeannerod, M. (1996). Mentally simulated movements in virtual reality: Does Fitt’s law hold in motor imagery. Behavioural Brain Research, 72, 127–134.CrossRef
go back to reference Drake, C., Penel, A., & Bigand, E. (2000). Tapping in time with mechanically and expressively performed music. Music Perception, 18, 1–23. Drake, C., Penel, A., & Bigand, E. (2000). Tapping in time with mechanically and expressively performed music. Music Perception, 18, 1–23.
go back to reference Flach, R., Knoblich, G., & Prinz, W. (2003). Off-line authorship effects in action perception. Brain & Cognition, 53, 503–513.CrossRef Flach, R., Knoblich, G., & Prinz, W. (2003). Off-line authorship effects in action perception. Brain & Cognition, 53, 503–513.CrossRef
go back to reference Fraisse, P. (1982). Rhythm and tempo. In D. Deutsch (Ed.), The Psychology of Music (pp. 149–180). New York, Academic Press. Fraisse, P. (1982). Rhythm and tempo. In D. Deutsch (Ed.), The Psychology of Music (pp. 149–180). New York, Academic Press.
go back to reference Gérard, C., & Rosenfeld, M. (1995). Pratique musicale et régulations temporelles [Musical practice and temporal control]. L’Année psychologique, 95, 571–591. Gérard, C., & Rosenfeld, M. (1995). Pratique musicale et régulations temporelles [Musical practice and temporal control]. L’Année psychologique, 95, 571–591.
go back to reference Greenwald, A. G. (1970). Sensory feedback mechanisms in performance control: With special reference to the ideo-motor mechanism. Psychological Review, 77, 73–99.PubMed Greenwald, A. G. (1970). Sensory feedback mechanisms in performance control: With special reference to the ideo-motor mechanism. Psychological Review, 77, 73–99.PubMed
go back to reference Hommel, B., Müsseler, J., Aschersleben, G., & Prinz, W. (2001). The theory of event coding (TEC). A framework for perception and action. Behavioral & Brain Sciences, 24, 849–937.PubMed Hommel, B., Müsseler, J., Aschersleben, G., & Prinz, W. (2001). The theory of event coding (TEC). A framework for perception and action. Behavioral & Brain Sciences, 24, 849–937.PubMed
go back to reference Ivry, R. B., & Hazeltine, E. (1995). Perception and production of temporal intervals across a range of durations: Evidence for a common timing mechanism. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 21, 3–18.CrossRefPubMed Ivry, R. B., & Hazeltine, E. (1995). Perception and production of temporal intervals across a range of durations: Evidence for a common timing mechanism. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 21, 3–18.CrossRefPubMed
go back to reference Ivry, R. B., & Keele, S. W. (1989). Timing functions of the cerebellum. Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience, 1, 136–152. Ivry, R. B., & Keele, S. W. (1989). Timing functions of the cerebellum. Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience, 1, 136–152.
go back to reference Keele, S. W., Pokorny, R. A., Corcos, D. M., & Ivry, R. (1985). Do perception and motor production share common timing mechanisms: A correlational analysis. Acta Psychologica, 60, 173–191.CrossRefPubMed Keele, S. W., Pokorny, R. A., Corcos, D. M., & Ivry, R. (1985). Do perception and motor production share common timing mechanisms: A correlational analysis. Acta Psychologica, 60, 173–191.CrossRefPubMed
go back to reference Knoblich, G., & Prinz, W. (2001). Recognition of self-generated actions from kinematic displays of drawing. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 27, 456–465.CrossRefPubMed Knoblich, G., & Prinz, W. (2001). Recognition of self-generated actions from kinematic displays of drawing. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 27, 456–465.CrossRefPubMed
go back to reference Knoblich, G., Seigerschmidt, E., Flach, R., & Prinz, W. (2002). Authorship effects in the prediction of handwriting strokes: Evidence for action simulation during action perception. Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology, 55, 1027–1046.CrossRef Knoblich, G., Seigerschmidt, E., Flach, R., & Prinz, W. (2002). Authorship effects in the prediction of handwriting strokes: Evidence for action simulation during action perception. Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology, 55, 1027–1046.CrossRef
go back to reference Parncutt, R. (1994). A perceptual model of pulse salience and metrical accent in musical rhythms. Music Perception, 11, 409–464. Parncutt, R. (1994). A perceptual model of pulse salience and metrical accent in musical rhythms. Music Perception, 11, 409–464.
go back to reference Povel, D.-J. (1981). Internal representation of simple temporal patterns. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 7, 3–18.CrossRefPubMed Povel, D.-J. (1981). Internal representation of simple temporal patterns. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 7, 3–18.CrossRefPubMed
go back to reference Prinz, W. (1997). Perception and action planning. European Journal of Cognitive Psychology, 9, 129–154. Prinz, W. (1997). Perception and action planning. European Journal of Cognitive Psychology, 9, 129–154.
go back to reference Repp, B. (1987). The sound of two hands clapping: An exploratory study. Journal of the Acoustical Society of America, 81, 1100–1109.PubMed Repp, B. (1987). The sound of two hands clapping: An exploratory study. Journal of the Acoustical Society of America, 81, 1100–1109.PubMed
go back to reference Rimoldi, H. J. A. (1951). Personal tempo. Journal of Abnormal and Social Psychology, 46, 283–303. Rimoldi, H. J. A. (1951). Personal tempo. Journal of Abnormal and Social Psychology, 46, 283–303.
go back to reference Sirigu, A., Duhamel, J.-R., Cohen, L., Pillon, B., Dubois, B., & Agid, Y. (1996). The mental representation of hand movements after parietal damage. Science, 273, 1564–1568.PubMed Sirigu, A., Duhamel, J.-R., Cohen, L., Pillon, B., Dubois, B., & Agid, Y. (1996). The mental representation of hand movements after parietal damage. Science, 273, 1564–1568.PubMed
go back to reference Smith, J. (1982). Reproduction and representation of musical rhythms: The effects of musical skill. In D. Rogers & J. A. Sloboda (Eds.), The acquisition of symbolic skills (pp. 273–282). New York, Plenum. Smith, J. (1982). Reproduction and representation of musical rhythms: The effects of musical skill. In D. Rogers & J. A. Sloboda (Eds.), The acquisition of symbolic skills (pp. 273–282). New York, Plenum.
go back to reference Smoll, F. L. (1975). Preferred tempo of motor performance: Individual differences in within-individual variability. Journal of Motor Behavior, 7, 259–263. Smoll, F. L. (1975). Preferred tempo of motor performance: Individual differences in within-individual variability. Journal of Motor Behavior, 7, 259–263.
go back to reference Smoll, F. L., & Schutz, R. W. (1978). Relationships among measures of preferred tempo and motor rhythm. Perceptual and Motor Skills, 46, 883–894.PubMed Smoll, F. L., & Schutz, R. W. (1978). Relationships among measures of preferred tempo and motor rhythm. Perceptual and Motor Skills, 46, 883–894.PubMed
go back to reference Treisman, M., Faulkner, A., Naish, P. L. N., & Brogan, D. (1990). The internal clock: Evidence for a temporal oscillator underlying time perception with some estimates of its characteristic frequency. Perception, 19, 705–743.PubMed Treisman, M., Faulkner, A., Naish, P. L. N., & Brogan, D. (1990). The internal clock: Evidence for a temporal oscillator underlying time perception with some estimates of its characteristic frequency. Perception, 19, 705–743.PubMed
go back to reference Treisman, M., Faulkner, A., & Naish, P. L. N. (1992). On the relation between time perception and the timing of motor action: Evidence for a temporal oscillator controlling the timing of movement. Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology, 45A, 255–263. Treisman, M., Faulkner, A., & Naish, P. L. N. (1992). On the relation between time perception and the timing of motor action: Evidence for a temporal oscillator controlling the timing of movement. Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology, 45A, 255–263.
go back to reference Vorberg, D., & Wing, A. (1996). Modeling variability and dependence in timing. In H. Heuer & S. W. Keele (Eds.), Handbook of Perception and Action (vol. 2, pp. 181–262). San Diego, CA: Academic Press. Vorberg, D., & Wing, A. (1996). Modeling variability and dependence in timing. In H. Heuer & S. W. Keele (Eds.), Handbook of Perception and Action (vol. 2, pp. 181–262). San Diego, CA: Academic Press.
go back to reference Zelaznik, H. N., Spencer, R. M., & Doffin, J. G. (2000). Temporal precision in tapping and circle drawing movements at preferred rates is not correlated: Further evidence against timing as a general-purpose ability. Journal of Motor Behavior, 32, 193–199.PubMed Zelaznik, H. N., Spencer, R. M., & Doffin, J. G. (2000). Temporal precision in tapping and circle drawing movements at preferred rates is not correlated: Further evidence against timing as a general-purpose ability. Journal of Motor Behavior, 32, 193–199.PubMed
Metagegevens
Titel
Recognizing one’s own clapping: The role of temporal cues
Auteurs
Rüdiger Flach
Günther Knoblich
Wolfgang Prinz
Publicatiedatum
01-12-2004
Uitgeverij
Springer-Verlag
Gepubliceerd in
Psychological Research / Uitgave 1-2/2004
Print ISSN: 0340-0727
Elektronisch ISSN: 1430-2772
DOI
https://doi.org/10.1007/s00426-003-0165-2

Andere artikelen Uitgave 1-2/2004

Psychological Research 1-2/2004 Naar de uitgave