Skip to main content
Top
Gepubliceerd in: Psychological Research 1/2014

01-01-2014 | Original Article

Rapid processing of closure and viewpoint-invariant symmetry: behavioral criteria for feedforward processing

Auteurs: Filipp Schmidt, Thomas Schmidt

Gepubliceerd in: Psychological Research | Uitgave 1/2014

Log in om toegang te krijgen
share
DELEN

Deel dit onderdeel of sectie (kopieer de link)

  • Optie A:
    Klik op de rechtermuisknop op de link en selecteer de optie “linkadres kopiëren”
  • Optie B:
    Deel de link per e-mail

Abstract

To pin down the processing characteristics of symmetry and closure in contour processing, we investigated their ability to activate rapid motor responses in a primed flanker task. In three experiments, participants selected as quickly and accurately as possible the one of two target contours possessing symmetry or closure. Target pairs were preceded by prime pairs whose spatial arrangement was consistent or inconsistent with respect to the required response. We tested for the efficiency and automaticity of symmetry and closure processing. For both cues, priming effects were present in full magnitude in the fastest motor responses consistent with a simple feedforward model. Priming effects from symmetry cues were independent of skewing and the orientation of their symmetry axis but sometimes failed to increase with increasing prime-target interval. We conclude that closure and (possibly) viewpoint-independent symmetry cues are extracted rapidly during the first feedforward wave of neuronal processing.
Voetnoten
1
Here, we focus on bilateral (mirror, reflection) symmetry only, because this form of symmetry is the most salient, most investigated and most relevant to humans (Treder, 2010). We will use the term “symmetry” to refer to “bilateral symmetry”.
 
2
Figure-ground segregation is the process by which the visual system distinguishes a figure from its background. It is crucial for object recognition and for physical interactions with our environment. The identification of an image area as a figure or background strongly depends on the visual attributes of that area. Importantly, an area that is symmetric or enclosed is more likely to be seen as a figure than an (adjacent) area that is asymmetric (e.g., Bahnsen, 1928; Machilsen, Pauwels, & Wagemans, 2009) or open (e.g., Koffka, 1935; Kovács & Julesz, 1993).
 
3
The notion of closure as a basic feature that is detected and processed by the visual system in a parallel fashion is not without controversy (e.g., Enns, 1986). Also, there is no unequivocal evidence that the processing of closure is automatic.
 
4
Note that the rapid-chase criteria do not guarantee that the system is strictly feedback-free (e.g., VanRullen & Koch, 2003) but establish it to be indistinguishable from a pure feedforward system.
 
5
While grouping strength can be easily matched for some grouping dimensions (e.g., similarity in brightness or size; Schmidt & Schmidt, submitted), matching is difficult to achieve with more complex grouping principles. Imagine, for example, participants adjusting the amount of symmetry in a given figure such that it is equal to the perceived amount of closure in another figure.
 
6
Note that errors in inconsistent trials represent motor responses that were misled by the conflicting prime information. This follows from response priming experiments with pointing responses, in which primes initiate a response toward them and sometimes provoke a full-fledged movement to their position (cf. Schmidt et al., 2006), and experiments measuring lateralized readiness potentials (Eimer & Schlaghecken, 1998; Leuthold & Kopp, 1998; Vath & Schmidt, 2007). Priming effects in error rates, as in response times, increase with prime-target SOA because the prime signal has more time to influence the response before the target signal becomes effective (cf. Schmidt et al., 2011; Vorberg et al., 2003).
 
Literatuur
go back to reference Bahnsen, P. (1928). Eine Untersuchung über Symmetrie und Asymmetrie bei visuellen Wahrnehmungen. Zeitschrift für Psychologie, 108, 129–154. Bahnsen, P. (1928). Eine Untersuchung über Symmetrie und Asymmetrie bei visuellen Wahrnehmungen. Zeitschrift für Psychologie, 108, 129–154.
go back to reference Cousineau, D. (2005). Confidence intervals in within-subject designs: A simpler solution to Loftus and Masson’s method. Tutorials in Quantitative Methods for Psychology, 1, 42–45. Cousineau, D. (2005). Confidence intervals in within-subject designs: A simpler solution to Loftus and Masson’s method. Tutorials in Quantitative Methods for Psychology, 1, 42–45.
go back to reference Dakin, S. C., & Herbert, A. M. (1998). The spatial region of integration for visual symmetry detection. Proceedings of the Royal Society B: Biological Sciences, 265, 659–664.PubMedCrossRef Dakin, S. C., & Herbert, A. M. (1998). The spatial region of integration for visual symmetry detection. Proceedings of the Royal Society B: Biological Sciences, 265, 659–664.PubMedCrossRef
go back to reference Eimer, M., & Schlaghecken, F. (1998). Effects of masked stimuli on motor activation: behavioral and electrophysiological evidence. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 24, 1737–1747.PubMed Eimer, M., & Schlaghecken, F. (1998). Effects of masked stimuli on motor activation: behavioral and electrophysiological evidence. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 24, 1737–1747.PubMed
go back to reference Fisher, C. B., & Bornstein, M. H. (1982). Identification of symmetry: effects of stimulus orientation and head position. Perception & Psychophysics, 32, 443–448. Fisher, C. B., & Bornstein, M. H. (1982). Identification of symmetry: effects of stimulus orientation and head position. Perception & Psychophysics, 32, 443–448.
go back to reference Houtkamp, R., & Roelfsema, P. R. (2010). Parallel and serial grouping of image elements in visual perception. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 36, 1443–1459. doi:10.1037/a0020248.PubMed Houtkamp, R., & Roelfsema, P. R. (2010). Parallel and serial grouping of image elements in visual perception. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 36, 1443–1459. doi:10.​1037/​a0020248.PubMed
go back to reference Jacobsen, T., & Höfel, L. (2003). Descriptive and evaluative judgment processes: Behavioral and electrophysiological indices of processing symmetry and aesthetics. Cognitive, Affective, & Behavioral Neuroscience, 3, 289–299. doi:10.3758/CABN.3.4.289.CrossRef Jacobsen, T., & Höfel, L. (2003). Descriptive and evaluative judgment processes: Behavioral and electrophysiological indices of processing symmetry and aesthetics. Cognitive, Affective, & Behavioral Neuroscience, 3, 289–299. doi:10.​3758/​CABN.​3.​4.​289.CrossRef
go back to reference Julesz, B. (2006). Foundations of cyclopean perception. Cambridge: MIT Press. (Original published in 1971). Julesz, B. (2006). Foundations of cyclopean perception. Cambridge: MIT Press. (Original published in 1971).
go back to reference Klotz, W., Heumann, M., Ansorge, U., & Neumann, O. (2007). Electrophysiological activation by masked primes: Independence of prime-related and target-related activities. Advances in Cognitive Psychology, 3, 449–465.PubMedCentralCrossRef Klotz, W., Heumann, M., Ansorge, U., & Neumann, O. (2007). Electrophysiological activation by masked primes: Independence of prime-related and target-related activities. Advances in Cognitive Psychology, 3, 449–465.PubMedCentralCrossRef
go back to reference Koffka, K. (1935). Principles of Gestalt psychology. London: Routledge & Kegan Paul. Koffka, K. (1935). Principles of Gestalt psychology. London: Routledge & Kegan Paul.
go back to reference Kovács, I., & Julesz, B. (1993). A closed curve is much more than an incomplete one: Effect of closure in figure-ground segmentation. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America, 90, 7495–7497.PubMedCentralPubMedCrossRef Kovács, I., & Julesz, B. (1993). A closed curve is much more than an incomplete one: Effect of closure in figure-ground segmentation. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America, 90, 7495–7497.PubMedCentralPubMedCrossRef
go back to reference Lachmann, T., & van Leeuwen, C. (2008). Goodness is central: Task-invariance of perceptual organization in a dual-task setting. Japanese Psychological Research, 50, 193–203. Lachmann, T., & van Leeuwen, C. (2008). Goodness is central: Task-invariance of perceptual organization in a dual-task setting. Japanese Psychological Research, 50, 193–203.
go back to reference Leuthold, H., & Kopp, B. (1998). Mechanisms of priming by masked stimuli: Inferences from event-related brain potentials. Psychological Science, 9, 263–269.CrossRef Leuthold, H., & Kopp, B. (1998). Mechanisms of priming by masked stimuli: Inferences from event-related brain potentials. Psychological Science, 9, 263–269.CrossRef
go back to reference Machilsen, B., Pauwels, M., & Wagemans, J. (2009). The role of vertical mirror symmetry in visual shape detection. Journal of Vision, 9(12), 11, 1–11. doi:10.1167/9.12.11. Machilsen, B., Pauwels, M., & Wagemans, J. (2009). The role of vertical mirror symmetry in visual shape detection. Journal of Vision, 9(12), 11, 1–11. doi:10.​1167/​9.​12.​11.
go back to reference Marino, A. C., & Scholl, B. J. (2005). The role of closure in defining the “objects” of object-based attention. Perception & Psychophysics, 67, 1140–1149. doi:10.3758/BF03193547.CrossRef Marino, A. C., & Scholl, B. J. (2005). The role of closure in defining the “objects” of object-based attention. Perception & Psychophysics, 67, 1140–1149. doi:10.​3758/​BF03193547.CrossRef
go back to reference Norcia, A. M., Candy, T. R., Pettet, M. W., Vildavski, V. Y., & Tyler, C. W. (2002). Temporal dynamics of the human response to symmetry. Journal of Vision, 2, 132–139. doi:10.1167/2.2.1.PubMedCrossRef Norcia, A. M., Candy, T. R., Pettet, M. W., Vildavski, V. Y., & Tyler, C. W. (2002). Temporal dynamics of the human response to symmetry. Journal of Vision, 2, 132–139. doi:10.​1167/​2.​2.​1.PubMedCrossRef
go back to reference Palmer, S. E., & Ghose, T. (2008). Extremal edges: A powerful cue to depth perception and figure-ground organization. Psychological Science, 19, 77–84.PubMedCrossRef Palmer, S. E., & Ghose, T. (2008). Extremal edges: A powerful cue to depth perception and figure-ground organization. Psychological Science, 19, 77–84.PubMedCrossRef
go back to reference Palmer, S. E., & Hemenway, K. (1978). Orientation and symmetry: Effects of multiple, rotational, and near symmetries. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 4, 691–702. doi:10.1037/0096-1523.4.4.691.PubMed Palmer, S. E., & Hemenway, K. (1978). Orientation and symmetry: Effects of multiple, rotational, and near symmetries. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 4, 691–702. doi:10.​1037/​0096-1523.​4.​4.​691.PubMed
go back to reference Roelfsema, P. R. (2006). Cortical algorithms for perceptual grouping. Annual Review of Neuroscience, 29, 203–227.PubMedCrossRef Roelfsema, P. R. (2006). Cortical algorithms for perceptual grouping. Annual Review of Neuroscience, 29, 203–227.PubMedCrossRef
go back to reference Sasaki, Y., Vanduffel, W., Knutsen, T., Tyler, C., & Tootell, R. (2005). Symmetry activates extrastriate visual cortex in human and nonhuman primates. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences (USA), 102, 3159–3163. doi:10.1073/pnas.0500319102.CrossRef Sasaki, Y., Vanduffel, W., Knutsen, T., Tyler, C., & Tootell, R. (2005). Symmetry activates extrastriate visual cortex in human and nonhuman primates. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences (USA), 102, 3159–3163. doi:10.​1073/​pnas.​0500319102.CrossRef
go back to reference Schmidt, T., & Schmidt, F. (2009). Processing of natural images is feedforward: A simple behavioral test. Attention, Perception, & Psychophysics, 71, 594–606.CrossRef Schmidt, T., & Schmidt, F. (2009). Processing of natural images is feedforward: A simple behavioral test. Attention, Perception, & Psychophysics, 71, 594–606.CrossRef
go back to reference Schmidt, F., & Schmidt, T. (submitted). Grouping principles in direct competition. Schmidt, F., & Schmidt, T. (submitted). Grouping principles in direct competition.
go back to reference Schumann, F. (1900). Beiträge zur Analyse der Gesichtswahrnehmungen. Erste Abhandlung. Einige Beobachtungen über die Zusammenfassung von Gesichtseindrücken zu Einheiten. Zeitschrift für Psychologie und Physiologie der Sinnesorgane, 23, 1–32. Schumann, F. (1900). Beiträge zur Analyse der Gesichtswahrnehmungen. Erste Abhandlung. Einige Beobachtungen über die Zusammenfassung von Gesichtseindrücken zu Einheiten. Zeitschrift für Psychologie und Physiologie der Sinnesorgane, 23, 1–32.
go back to reference Seydell-Greenwald, A., & Schmidt, T. (2012). Rapid activation of motor responses by illusory contours. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 38, 1168–1182.PubMed Seydell-Greenwald, A., & Schmidt, T. (2012). Rapid activation of motor responses by illusory contours. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 38, 1168–1182.PubMed
go back to reference Tapia, E., Breitmeyer, B. G., & Shooner, C. R. (2010). Role of task-directed attention in nonconscious and conscious response priming by form and color. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 36, 74–87.PubMed Tapia, E., Breitmeyer, B. G., & Shooner, C. R. (2010). Role of task-directed attention in nonconscious and conscious response priming by form and color. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 36, 74–87.PubMed
go back to reference Van der Helm, P. A., & Leeuwenberg, E. L. J. (1996). Goodness of visual regularities: A nontransformational approach. Psychological Review, 103, 429–456.PubMedCrossRef Van der Helm, P. A., & Leeuwenberg, E. L. J. (1996). Goodness of visual regularities: A nontransformational approach. Psychological Review, 103, 429–456.PubMedCrossRef
go back to reference Van der Helm, P. A., & Leeuwenberg, E. L. J. (1999). A better approach to goodness: Reply to Wagemans (1999). Psychological Review, 106, 622–630.CrossRef Van der Helm, P. A., & Leeuwenberg, E. L. J. (1999). A better approach to goodness: Reply to Wagemans (1999). Psychological Review, 106, 622–630.CrossRef
go back to reference VanRullen, R., & Thorpe, S. J. (2001). Is it a bird? Is it a plane? Ultra-rapid visual categorization of natural and artifactual objects. Perception, 30, 655–668. doi:10.1068/p3029.PubMedCrossRef VanRullen, R., & Thorpe, S. J. (2001). Is it a bird? Is it a plane? Ultra-rapid visual categorization of natural and artifactual objects. Perception, 30, 655–668. doi:10.​1068/​p3029.PubMedCrossRef
go back to reference Vath, N., & Schmidt, T. (2007). Tracing sequential waves of rapid visuomotor activation in lateralized readiness potentials. Neuroscience, 145, 197–208.PubMedCrossRef Vath, N., & Schmidt, T. (2007). Tracing sequential waves of rapid visuomotor activation in lateralized readiness potentials. Neuroscience, 145, 197–208.PubMedCrossRef
go back to reference Vorberg, D., Mattler, U., Heinecke, A., Schmidt, T., & Schwarzbach, J. (2003). Different time courses for visual perception and action priming. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences USA, 100, 6275–6280.CrossRef Vorberg, D., Mattler, U., Heinecke, A., Schmidt, T., & Schwarzbach, J. (2003). Different time courses for visual perception and action priming. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences USA, 100, 6275–6280.CrossRef
go back to reference Wagemans, J., Van Gool, L., & d’Ydewalle, G. (1991). Detection of symmetry in tachistoscopically presented dot patterns: Effects of multiple axes and skewing. Perception & Psychophysics, 50, 413–427. doi:10.3758/BF03205058.CrossRef Wagemans, J., Van Gool, L., & d’Ydewalle, G. (1991). Detection of symmetry in tachistoscopically presented dot patterns: Effects of multiple axes and skewing. Perception & Psychophysics, 50, 413–427. doi:10.​3758/​BF03205058.CrossRef
go back to reference Wenderoth, P. (2000). The differential effects of simultaneous and successive cueing on the detection of bilateral symmetry in dot patterns. The Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology Section A: Human Experimental Psychology, 53, 165–190. Wenderoth, P. (2000). The differential effects of simultaneous and successive cueing on the detection of bilateral symmetry in dot patterns. The Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology Section A: Human Experimental Psychology, 53, 165–190.
go back to reference Wenderoth, P., & Welsh, S. (1998). Effects of pattern orientation and number of symmetry axes on the detection of mirror symmetry in dot and solid patterns. Perception, 27, 965–976. doi:10.1068/p270965.PubMedCrossRef Wenderoth, P., & Welsh, S. (1998). Effects of pattern orientation and number of symmetry axes on the detection of mirror symmetry in dot and solid patterns. Perception, 27, 965–976. doi:10.​1068/​p270965.PubMedCrossRef
go back to reference Wertheimer, M. (1923). Untersuchungen zur Lehre von der Gestalt, II. Psychologische Forschung, 4, 301–350.CrossRef Wertheimer, M. (1923). Untersuchungen zur Lehre von der Gestalt, II. Psychologische Forschung, 4, 301–350.CrossRef
go back to reference Zipser, K., Lamme, V. A. F., & Schiller, P. H. (1996). Contextual modulation in primary visual cortex. The Journal of Neuroscience, 16, 7376–7389.PubMed Zipser, K., Lamme, V. A. F., & Schiller, P. H. (1996). Contextual modulation in primary visual cortex. The Journal of Neuroscience, 16, 7376–7389.PubMed
Metagegevens
Titel
Rapid processing of closure and viewpoint-invariant symmetry: behavioral criteria for feedforward processing
Auteurs
Filipp Schmidt
Thomas Schmidt
Publicatiedatum
01-01-2014
Uitgeverij
Springer Berlin Heidelberg
Gepubliceerd in
Psychological Research / Uitgave 1/2014
Print ISSN: 0340-0727
Elektronisch ISSN: 1430-2772
DOI
https://doi.org/10.1007/s00426-013-0478-8

Andere artikelen Uitgave 1/2014

Psychological Research 1/2014 Naar de uitgave