Introduction
Poor sleep, characterized by insufficient sleep duration and reduced sleep quality, is an increasing health problem among adolescents (Inchley et al.,
2020), and can negatively impact mental health (Jamieson et al.,
2020), physical health (Miller et al.,
2018), and cognition (Tarokh et al.,
2016). It has been suggested that sleep in adolescence is affected by biological factors, contextual factors, as well as psychosocial factors (Becker et al.,
2015). Regarding those psychosocial factors or determinants, behavior change theories postulate that knowledge, attitude, self-efficacy, perceived norms, perceived barriers, and perceived social support influence whether humans engage in the behavior or not (Bartholomew Eldredge et al.,
2016). However, little research has investigated whether these psychosocial determinants are associated with adolescents’ sleep behavior. This study aims to explore whether changes in knowledge, attitude, self-efficacy, perceived norm, perceived barriers and perceived social support are associated with changes in sleep parameters over a 1-year period using secondary data from Flemish adolescents.
Adolescence is a period in which healthy sleep is particularly important, but might also be particularly disrupted due to a range of different factors, such as a shift in circadian rhythm, detachment from parents, social interests that favor later bedtimes, and increased social media use (Blake et al.,
2019). Healthy sleep comprises sleep duration, i.e., the total time spent asleep, and sleep quality. In this study, sleep quality includes the ease of going to bed, the ease of falling asleep and reinitiating sleep, and the ease of returning to wakefulness (Sufrinko et al.,
2015). Moreover, daytime sleepiness (Drake et al.,
2003) and Sleep Onset Latencies (SOL, Roenneberg et al.,
2015) are indicators of sleep quality. A biopsychosocial contextual model of adolescent sleep (Becker et al.,
2015) suggests that the biological, contextual, and psychosocial changes one undergoes during adolescence influence (healthy) sleep. Psychosocial factors have previously been suggested to be the most changeable factors, and are therefore most often targeted by public health interventions (Crutzen et al.,
2017; Jansen et al.,
2015). The Theory of Planned Behavior (TPB, Ajzen,
1991), the Reasoned Action Approach (RAA, Fishbein & Ajzen,
2011), and the Attitude-Social influence-self-Efficacy model (ASE, De Vries et al. (
1988)), identify knowledge, attitude, self-efficacy and perceived norms as psychosocial determinants of intending to perform a behavior or not. Actual performance of this behavior may then be hindered by perceived barriers, or facilitated by perceived support (Eldredge et al.,
2016). Specifically, knowledge is defined as the understanding that one has of a key concept, attitude is an individual’s positive or negative evaluation of performing a particular behavior, self-efficacy the subjective probability that one is able to perform a behavior, and perceived norms the beliefs about whether one’s environment approves or disapproves from a behavior (Ajzen,
1991).
Previous research suggested that the abovementioned theories are appropriate for understanding, and intervening, in a variety of health behaviors in adolescence including nutrition-related behaviors (Riebl et al.,
2015), drinking behavior (Sciglimpaglia et al. (
2020)), cyberbullying (Heirman & Walrave,
2012), intimate partner violence (Nardi-Rodríguez et al. (
2019)), condom use (Gomes and Nunes (
2018)), and safe sex intentions (Armitage & Talibudeen,
2010). Moreover, some developmental specificities such as an increasing need for autonomy, peer norms becoming more important, and increased detachment from parents (Sawyer et al.,
2012) might further underscore the importance to investigate psychosocial determinants such as self-efficacy or perceived parental and peer norms regarding healthy sleep during adolescence.
As poor sleep behavior in adolescence is likely to carry over to adult life (Dregan & Armstrong,
2010), it is important to gain insight into the psychosocial determinants that help to explain this behavior already in adolescence. Despite this necessity, little research has investigated the psychosocial determinants identified by behavior change theories in relation to adolescents’ sleep behavior. Previous studies were qualitative (Gruber et al.,
2017; Vandendriessche et al.
2022) or only focused on subsets of psychosocial determinants and had a cross-sectional design (Cassoff et al. (
2014b); Bonnar et al.,
2015; Short et al. (
2020)). However, behavior change theories postulate that psychosocial determinants are interrelated, suggesting that they should be investigated together. Qualitative studies indicated that adolescents themselves considered knowledge about sleep, attitude toward sleep and going to bed on time, self-efficacy regarding going to bed on time, perceived parental and peer norms, and perceived barriers and support to going to bed on time to be important factors influencing their sleep behavior. These studies, however, advocated for the use of quantitative methods in large samples to confirm their findings (Gruber et al.,
2017; Vandendriessche et al.,
2022). Studies using quantitative methods, but only focusing on a subset of psychosocial determinants, found that attitude and parental social support might be important psychosocial determinants of sleep (Cassoff et al. (
2014b); Bonnar et al.,
2015; Short et al. (
2020)). Another study indicated that emotional and behavioral difficulties were longitudinally related to increased sleep problems in adolescents (Kortesoja et al.,
2020). Although this study did not specifically focus on the psychosocial determinants identified by behavior change theories, experiencing emotional and behavioral difficulties might be seen as an important perceived barrier toward healthy sleep.
Results
The following variables were found to be related to drop-out: biological sex, SES as assessed with parental educational level, sleep duration on school days, sleep quality, and attitude toward sleep. Boys, participants with lower educated parents, lower sleep duration on school days, lower sleep quality, and less positive attitude toward sleep had higher chances of dropping out at T1. No statistically significant differences were observed for age, place of birth, sleep duration on school days, and daytime sleepiness. Associations between the different sleep parameters were investigated as well, with correlations ranging from −0.05 for SOL on school days at T0 and sleep duration on free days at T1, to 0.67 for SOL on school days at T1 and SOL on free days at T1. Moreover, associations between psychosocial determinants ranged from <0.001 for norm knowledge at T1 and perceived barriers at T0, to 0.58 for bedtime rules on school days at T1 and bedtime rules on school days at T0. Lastly, associations between sleep parameters and psychosocial determinants ranged from <0.001 for norm knowledge at T1 and sleep duration on free days at T0, to 0.31 for general sleep quality at T1 and self-efficacy at T1. Correlation tables can be found in Appendix
3.
Associations between changes in psychosocial determinants and changes in sleep duration and quality, including total variance explained (R
2) are provided in Tables
3–
5. Unstandardized B coefficients show the increase of sleep duration and sleep quality for every one-unit increase of the determinant. Standardized beta coefficients show the relative strength of the association of each psychosocial determinant to the sleep parameters. CIBER results (Appendix
2) were mostly in line with results from regression analyses.
Table 3
Results of linear models: Associations of changes in psychosocial determinants with changes in sleep duration
Knowledge | −0.10 | 0.00 | −0.29 | 0.08 | 0.30 | −0.01 | 0.03 | −0.30 | 0.27 | 0.93 |
Norm-knowledge | 0.13 | 0.05 | 0.00 | 0.25 | 0.04 | −0.08 | 0.04 | −0.27 | 0.10 | 0.37 |
Attitudes | 0.20 | 0.07 | 0.11 | 0.29 | <0.001 | 0.06 | 0.02 | −0.08 | 0.19 | 0.41 |
Perceived advantages | 0.08 | 0.04 | 0.00 | 0.17 | 0.06 | 0.22 | 0.04 | 0.08 | 0.35 | 0.001 |
Self-efficacy | 0.05 | 0.01 | −0.04 | 0.14 | 0.28 | 0.04 | 0.01 | −0.10 | 0.18 | 0.55 |
Modelling peers | 0.10 | 0.05 | 0.02 | 0.18 | 0.01 | −0.00 | 0.01 | −0.12 | 0.12 | 0.97 |
Perceived norm peers | −0.08 | −0.04 | −0.15 | 0.01 | 0.05 | 0.05 | 0.00 | −0.07 | 0.17 | 0.40 |
Modelling parents | −0.004 | 0.01 | −0.09 | 0.08 | 0.92 | 0.04 | 0.01 | −0.08 | 0.17 | 0.50 |
Perceived norm parents | −0.04 | −0.02 | −0.13 | 0.05 | 0.39 | 0.04 | 0.03 | −0.09 | 0.18 | 0.54 |
Perceived norm parents, related to adolescent behavior | −0.04 | 0.01 | −0.13 | 0.04 | 0.32 | 0.04 | 0.00 | −0.08 | 0.17 | 0.50 |
Perceived barriers | −0.07 | −0.04 | −0.18 | 0.05 | 0.25 | −0.08 | −0.04 | −0.25 | 0.09 | 0.37 |
Perceived parental support (encouragement) | −0.03 | −0.00 | −0.10 | 0.04 | 0.37 | −0.04 | 0.02 | −0.14 | 0.05 | 0.38 |
Perceived parental support (bedtime rules school days) | 0.14 | 0.09 | 0.09 | 0.18 | <0.001 | – | – | – | – | – |
Perceived parental support (bedtime rules free days) | – | – | – | – | – | 0.23 | 0.08 | 0.13 | 0.32 | <0.001 |
Table 4
Results of linear models: Associations of changes in psychosocial determinants with changes in sleep quality (general sleep quality and daytime sleepiness)
Knowledge | −0.28 | −0.01 | −1.14 | 0.57 | 0.52 | 0.75 | 0.04 | −0.24 | 1.75 | 0.14 |
Norm-knowledge | −0.29 | −0.01 | −0.84 | 0.26 | 0.30 | −0.57 | −0.01 | −1.20 | 0.06 | 0.07 |
Attitudes | 0.47 | 0.06 | 0.07 | 0.87 | 0.02 | 0.44 | −0.01 | −0.02 | 0.90 | 0.06.13 |
Perceived advantages | 0.31 | 0.06 | −0.08 | 0.71 | 0.12 | 0.08 | −0.01 | −0.37 | 0.52 | 0.74 |
Self-efficacy | 0.66 | 0.06 | 0.25 | 1.07 | 0.001 | −0.78 | −0.04 | −1.25 | −0.31 | 0.001 |
Modelling peers | 0.09 | 0.02 | −0.26 | 0.46 | 0.59 | −0.37 | −0.04 | −0.79 | 0.05 | 0.08 |
Perceived norm peers | −0.14 | 0.02 | −0.50 | 0.21 | 0.44 | 0.12 | 0.00 | −0.29 | 0.54 | 0.55 |
Modelling parents | 0.28 | 0.05 | −0.10 | 0.66 | 0.15 | −0.53 | −0.02 | −0.97 | 0.09 | 0.02 |
Perceived norm parents | −0.14 | 0.00 | −0.50 | 0.22 | 0.44 | 0.20 | −0.02 | −0.26 | 0.68 | 0.38 |
Perceived norm parents, related to adolescent behavior | 0.24 | 0.00 | −0.15 | 0.63 | 0.22 | −0.37 | −0.03 | −0.81 | 0.08 | 0.11 |
Perceived barriers | −2.62 | −0.18 | −3.14 | −2.11 | <0.001 | 1.47 | 0.10 | 0.90 | 2.06 | <0.001 |
Perceived parental support (encouragement) | −0.13 | −0.01 | −0.44 | 0.17 | 0.38 | −0.07 | 0.01 | −0.42 | 0.29 | 0.70 |
Perceived parental support (bedtime rules school days) | −0.13 | −0.02 | −0.39 | 0.11 | 0.38 | −0.06 | 0.00 | −0.34 | 0.23 | 0.69 |
Perceived parental support (bedtime rules free days) | −0.14 | −0.01 | −0.44 | 0.16 | 0.26 | −0.06 | −0.02 | −0.40 | 0.28 | 0.69 |
Table 5
Results of linear models: Associations of changes in psychosocial determinants with changes in sleep quality (sleep onset latencies (SOL) on school days and free days)
Knowledge | 0.04 | 0.05 | −0.03 | 0.11 | 0.26 | 0.03 | 0.03 | −0.05 | 0.11 | 0.45 |
Norm-knowledge | −0.02 | −0.03 | −0.07 | 0.02 | 0.29 | −0.00 | −0.02 | −0.05 | 0.04 | 0.86 |
Attitudes | −0.01 | −0.00 | −0.05 | 0.02 | 0.49 | −0.01 | −0.01 | −0.04 | 0.03 | 0.64 |
Perceived advantages | 0.01 | −0.02 | −0.02 | 0.04 | 0.61 | 0.01 | 0.00 | −0.02 | 0.05 | 0.54 |
Self-efficacy | −0.01 | −0.02 | −0.04 | 0.03 | 0.77 | 0.03 | 0.01 | −0.01 | 0.06 | 0.16 |
Modelling peers | −0.01 | 0.01 | −0.04 | 0.02 | 0.46 | −0.01 | 0.01 | −0.05 | 0.02 | 0.39 |
Perceived norm peers | 0.00 | 0.01 | −0.03 | 0.03 | 0.91 | −0.01 | −0.01 | −0.04 | 0.02 | 0.51 |
Modelling parents | −0.01 | 0.01 | −0.04 | 0.02 | 0.58 | −0.02 | −0.02 | −0.05 | 0.02 | 0.37 |
Perceived norm parents | 0.03 | 0.03 | −0.01 | 0.06 | 0.14 | 0.03 | 0.03 | 0.00 | 0.07 | 0.07 |
Perceived norm parents, related to adolescent behavior | 0.01 | 0.00 | −0.02 | 0.05 | 0.43 | 0.00 | −0.01 | −0.03 | 0.03 | 0.98 |
Perceived barriers | 0.11 | 0.09 | 0.07 | 0.15 | <0.001 | 0.08 | 0.08 | 0.04 | 0.13 | <0.001 |
Perceived parental support (encouragement) | −0.00 | −0.00 | −0.03 | 0.02 | 0.68 | −0.01 | −0.00 | −0.04 | 0.01 | 0.24 |
Perceived parental support (bedtime rules school days) | −0.00 | 0.00 | −0.02 | 0.02 | 0.86 | – | – | – | – | – |
Perceived parental support (bedtime rules free days) | – | – | – | – | – | 0.01 | 0.02 | −0.01 | 0.03 | 0.40 |
Associations of Change in Psychosocial Determinants with Change in Sleep Duration
Bedtime rules (i.e., perceived parental support), attitude toward healthy sleep, perceived advantages of healthy sleep, knowledge of the norm to sleep at least 8 h per night, and perceived peer behavior were found to be significantly associated with sleep duration on school days and/or on free days. Specifically, a one-unit increase in bedtime rules was associated with an increase in sleep duration of 8 min and 4 s on school days and 13 min and 8 s on free days (B = 0.14, β = 0.09, p < 0.001; B = 0.23, β = 0.08, p < 0.001). Moreover, a one-unit increase in positive attitude toward healthy sleep was associated with an increase in sleep duration of 12 min on school days (B = 0.20, β = 0.07, p < 0.001). Relatedly, a one-unit increase in the perceived advantages of healthy sleep was significantly associated with an increase in sleep duration of 13 min and 2 s on free days (B = 0.22, β = 0.04, p = 0.001). A one-unit increase in knowledge of the norm of sleeping 8 h per night was associated with an increase in sleep duration of 7 min and 8 s (B = 0.13, β = 0.05, p = 0.04) on school days. Lastly, a one-unit increase in modeling by peers was associated with increases in sleep duration of 6 min on school days (B = 0.10, β = 0.05, p = 0.01), while a one-unit increase in peer norms was associated with a decrease of 4 min and 8 s at school days (B = −0.08, β = −0.04, p = 0.05). Changes in the remaining psychosocial determinants were not significantly associated with changes in sleep duration.
Associations of Change in Psychological Determinants with Change in Sleep Quality
Perceived barriers toward going to bed on time, attitude toward healthy sleep, self-efficacy toward engaging in healthy sleep behavior, and parental modeling were found to be significantly associated with sleep quality parameters. Specifically, a one-unit increase in perceived barriers was associated with a decrease of 2.62 on a scale of 60 for general sleep quality (B = −2.62, β = −0.18, p < 0.001) and an increase of 1.47 on a scale of 40 for daytime sleepiness (B = 1.47, β = 0.10, p < 0.001). Further, a one-unit increase in perceived barriers was associated with an increase in Sleep Onset Latencies of 6 min and 6 s on school days and 4 min and 8 s on free days (B = 0.11, β = 0.09, p < 0.001; B = 0.09, β = 0.08, p < 0.001). One-unit increases in self-efficacy were associated with an increase of 0.66 on a scale of 60 for general sleep quality (B = 0.66, β = 0.06, p = 0.001), and a decrease of 0.78 on a scale of 40 for daytime sleepiness (B = −0.78, β = −0.04, p = 0.001). Further, one-unit increases in positive attitude toward healthy sleep were associated with increases of 0.47 on a scale of 60 for general sleep quality (B = 0.47, β = 0.06, p = 0.02). One-unit increases in parental modeling were associated with a decrease of 0.53 on a scale of 40 for daytime sleepiness (B = −0.53, β = −0.02, p = 0.02). Changes in the remaining psychosocial determinants were not significantly associated with changes in sleep quality.
Sensitivity Analyses
Sensitivity analyses were run for all models without imputation, Moreover, mixed linear models were included as sensitivity analyses for those models which did not suffer from singularity. Results were similar to original results (see Appendix
4).
Discussion
While deepening knowledge on the most important changeable factors of adolescent sleep is important to understand adolescent sleep and eventually develop healthy sleep interventions, little research has investigated the psychosocial determinants identified by leading behavior change theories in relation to adolescent sleep. The current study explored whether changes in psychosocial determinants (i.e., knowledge, attitudes, perceived norms, self-efficacy, perceived barriers, and perceived social support), were associated with changes in adolescent sleep duration and sleep quality parameters. Changes in perceived parental social support (i.e., having bedtime rules), positive attitudes toward and perceived advantages of healthy sleep, perceived peer behavior, and norm knowledge were associated with changes in sleep duration, with parental social support and attitude having the strongest association. Changes in perceived barriers, self-efficacy, attitude, and perceived parental behavior were associated with changes in sleep quality parameters, with perceived barriers having the strongest association. The current results confirm the hypothesis that perceived social support, attitude, and perceived barriers are important psychosocial factors related to adolescent sleep and are in line with previous research (Cassoff et al. (
2014b); Bonnar et al. (
2015); Short et al. (
2020); Kortesoja et al., (
2020)). However, as is suggested by leading behavior change theories, other psychosocial determinants also seem to play a role.
Bedtime rules (i.e., perceived support from parents), had the strongest association with sleep duration, with increases of 8.4 min on weekdays and 13.8 min on weekends. This indicates that the structure a family can offer is important for adolescents’ sleep health, despite their increased need for independence and autonomy, including detachment from parents (Spear & Kulbok,
2004). While effect sizes are small, this finding is still promising, considering that increases of 15 min in sleep duration have been found to be clinically relevant (Perfect et al.,
2016). These results are in line with previous research, in which structured home environments and family support were found to improve healthy sleep in children and adolescents (Bally & van Grieken (
2020); Leonard & Khurana,
2022). Interestingly, encouragement of healthy sleep behavior was not found to be associated with sleep parameters, indicating that tangible bedtime rules might have a greater impact on adolescents’ sleep than simple encouragement. A significant association between increased levels of parental modeling and decreased levels of daytime sleepiness suggests that parents might be important figures when it comes to aspects of adolescent sleep quality, as well. While the current findings underscore the importance of parents for adolescent healthy sleep, it is unclear how this applies to adolescents who are not supported by their parents or do not have a parent or guardian figure in their lives. In that sense, the current findings might not be generalizable to a less privileged sample.
While parental influence might gradually decrease during adolescence, more time is spent with peers and the norms of peer networks become increasingly important (Ryan,
2000; Wang et al.,
2018). This is also reflected in the current results, as positive peer modeling and perceived peer sleep norms were found to be significantly associated with sleep duration on school days. Nowadays, social media use allows adolescents to closely monitor when peers are online, until when they send messages, and consequently when they go to bed, which might further explain these results. Moreover, previous studies indicated that adolescents were embarrassed to tell peers they would prefer sleeping to chatting (Vandendriessche et al.,
2022), underscoring the importance of peer norms in relation to bedtimes. Sleep quality was, however, not associated with perceived peer behavior. Due to the reasons stated above, sleep duration and bedtimes might be more salient or observable to peers than how well adolescents sleep, e.g., how long it takes them to fall asleep, and how many times they wake up during the night.
Increased levels of positive attitudes and relatedly perceived advantages of healthy sleep behavior were found to be associated with improved levels of sleep duration on school days and free days, as well as with general sleep quality. While attitudes were less strongly associated with sleep duration than bedtime rules, the increase of 12 min is quite mentionable. These results confirm the hypothesis that attitudes are important psychosocial determinants of adolescent sleep. Moreover, this is in line with previous research showing that attitudes were strong predictors for other health behaviors in adolescence, namely nutrition behavior (Riebl et al.,
2015) and cyberbullying (Heirman & Walrave,
2012). The current findings suggest the need to improve adolescents’ positive attitudes toward healthy sleep and going to bed on time to improve healthy sleep. Considering that adolescents have been shown to prioritize other activities over sleep (Vandendriessche et al.,
2022), this might be challenging. However, behavior change techniques like the direct experience of rewarding outcomes (Maibach & Cotton,
1995), (e.g., by keeping a sleep-mood diary) or repeated exposure to a stimulus (Zajonc,
2001), (e.g., by using prompts in a mobile application) have been shown to bring about attitude change. Interestingly, an increased level of knowledge about sleep, which has been proposed to be the first step toward developing a positive attitude (Cain et al.,
2011), was not found to be associated with improved levels of sleep parameters, except for norm knowledge and sleep duration on school days. This is in line with previous studies showing that knowledge about short-term negative consequences of poor sleep did not translate into healthier sleep behavior among adolescents (Cassoff et al.,
2014a; Gruber et al.,
2017). Moreover, a cross-sectional study in college students found attitude, but not knowledge, to be associated with longer sleep duration and improved sleep quality (Peach et al.,
2018).
An increased level of perceived barriers toward going to bed on time was most strongly associated with decreased levels of general sleep quality, increased levels of daytime sleepiness, and increased SOLs. Especially the observed increases in SOLs (6.6 min on school days and 4.8 min on free days) that were associated with increases in perceived barriers are quite large when considering the mean SOL in the current sample (26 min on school days and 20 min on free days at T0, and 22 min on school days and 18 min on free days at T1). A larger variety of barriers was assessed in this study: time constraints (hobbies in the evening, no time for relaxation), perceived sleep difficulties (falling asleep, wake after sleep onset, waking too early), school stress (including homework in the evening), worries, fear of missing out (related to TV programs as well as social media conversations), and perceiving sleeping early as boring. Some of these perceived barriers have already been investigated. For example, the biopsychosocial contextual model of adolescent sleep suggests that mental health and academic factors belong to the main psychosocial factors influencing adolescent sleep (Becker et al.,
2015). Moreover, previous longitudinal research has shown that emotional and behavioral difficulties predicted current and future sleep problems in adolescence (Kortesoja et al.,
2020). Lastly, especially social-media related screen use has been shown to negatively affect adolescent sleep (Mireku et al.,
2019). Future studies should further investigate which of these barriers are most strongly associated with sleep parameters. Interestingly, perceived barriers were found to be associated with all sleep quality parameters, but not with sleep duration. An explanation might be that some of the perceived barriers were about sleep difficulties, which are similar to some parameters of sleep quality. For example, the perceived barrier of not being able to directly fall asleep is reflected in SOL, and the perceived barrier of waking up during the night is reflected in general sleep quality. However, correlations between the psychosocial determinant perceived barriers and SOL on school days and free days (r
T0 = 0.23, r
T1 = 0.15, and r
T0 = 0.16, r
T1 = 0.12, respectively) and general sleep quality (r
T0 = −0.51, r
T1 = −0.34) were small to moderate (Akoglu,
2018). Altogether, the current findings illustrate that psychosocial factors impeding adolescent sleep quality might be related to psychosocial wellbeing and certain emotional states, and underscore the need to look at adolescent sleep in relation to mental health.
Previous research has found positive psychosocial factors like increased self-efficacy to be a buffer for negative psychosocial factors such as stress (Mikkelsen et al.,
2020). This is reflected in the current findings, as an increased level of self-efficacy was found to be significantly associated with improved levels of sleep quality parameters, although the associations were rather weak. Considering that self-efficacy and perceived barriers were both associated with sleep quality parameters, we might speculate that increased levels of self-efficacy might be a buffer to overcome perceived barriers and aid healthy sleep in adolescence. As adolescents reported low levels of self-efficacy when it came to improving their sleep behavior in previous studies (Vandendriessche et al.,
2022), there seems to be room for improvement, indicating that future interventions aiming to improve adolescent sleep could focus on increasing self-efficacy. Especially in the developmental context of adolescence, self-efficacy might be a valuable target, as the belief to be able to do things might relate to adolescents’ increased need for autonomy.
Altogether, the discussed findings suggest that psychosocial determinants identified by several leading behavior change theories can be applied to adolescent sleep. Specifically, perceived parental support (i.e., bedtime rules) and adolescents’ attitudes might be relevant to adolescent sleep duration, while perceived barriers and self-efficacy might be relevant for sleep quality. The current study does not allow any firm explanations on why these psychosocial determinants were differentially related to sleep duration and sleep quality parameters, which underlines the complexity of adolescent sleep. This complexity is also reflected in findings of other studies which indicated that some sleep parameters, such as sleep duration and efficiency, are explained by genetic factors, while others, such as sleep midpoint variability, are explained to a greater extent by environmental factors (Breitenstein et al.,
2021). Sleep parameters which are more explained by environmental factors might be more appropriate targets for interventions. Especially for these sleep parameters, it might be interesting to further investigate the most strongly associated psychosocial determinants. However, it should also be noted that sleep duration and sleep quality are interrelated, which is reflected in them sharing genetic factors (Breitenstein et al.,
2021). This, in turn, might make it difficult for sleep interventions to specifically target either sleep duration or quality.
While the observed increases in sleep duration are noteworthy, beta coefficients indicate that the observed associations were rather weak. A possible explanation might be that intention was not assessed in the current study. Behavior change theories postulate that intention to engage in a behavior is an important step between the psychosocial determinants of the behavior and actual performance of the behavior (Ajzen,
1991; Fishbein & Ajzen,
2011; de Vries et al. (
1988)). Applied to sleep, adolescents might intend to go to bed on time but not do so, which could be reflected in shorter sleep durations; or they might intend to improve sleep hygiene behavior but not do so, which could be reflected in reduced sleep quality. This is also referred to as the intention-behavior gap (Sheeran & Webb,
2016). It remains the question whether stronger associations would be observed when including changes in adolescents’ intention to perform healthy sleep behavior in the analyses. Theories propose that perceived barriers can hinder the performance of actual behavior even if the intention to perform that behavior is present, while perceived support can facilitate the performance of actual behavior whilst the intention to perform that behavior is present. Therefore, perceived barriers and perceived support influence the pathway from intention to actual behavior, which might explain why these psychosocial determinants were most strongly related with sleep parameters in the current study.
This study had both strengths and limitations. Strengths are its longitudinal two-wave panel design and the large sample size. Moreover, CIBER (Crutzen et al.,
2017) was used as an additional visual approach to establish determinant relevance of several psychosocial determinants simultaneously, and confirmed results from linear regression analyses. However, there are some limitations that should be addressed as well. First, even though validated measures were used, sleep was measured using self-report. Self-report might represent time spent in bed, instead of real time spent asleep. This might have biased results for sleep duration especially, and calls for objective measures of sleep. Nevertheless, recent findings indicate that objective and subjective measures might assess different sleep parameters (Breitenstein et al.,
2021), suggesting that future studies could combine objective and subjective measures to assess sleep. Secondly, the measure of sleep duration was calculated using the midpoint method to recode the answer options, which is not an exact measure. Third, both psychosocial determinants and sleep parameters were assessed using a questionnaire, which might have led to common method variance problems. Another limitation relates to the assessment of psychosocial determinants. While the questionnaire that was co-created with a group of adolescents was tested for reliability and validity in a small sample, no psychometric study exists to confirm its robustness. As reliability estimates are quite low, future research should further investigate how to best assess psychosocial determinants of sleep in adolescents. Lastly, the items concerning perceived barriers were summed to have an overall score on the level of barriers adolescents perceive, however, future research could examine them separately. This might be especially important for choosing the most appropriate behavior change techniques when developing future healthy sleep interventions. For example, positive or negative arousal states might be addressed by emotion-regulation techniques, while fear of missing out related to social media use might be better addressed by specifying action plans to reduce screen use before bedtime (Gollwitzer & Sheeran,
2006). Finally, it should be noted that the current results are limited to the context of Flemish Belgium and might not be generalizable to other age- or sociocultural groups.
Publisher’s note Springer Nature remains neutral with regard to jurisdictional claims in published maps and institutional affiliations.