Skip to main content
Top
Gepubliceerd in: Psychological Research 4/2023

16-08-2022 | Original Article

Perceptual grouping in complex rhythmic patterns

Auteurs: Lars Dietmar Hestermann, Johan Wagemans, Ralf T. Krampe

Gepubliceerd in: Psychological Research | Uitgave 4/2023

Log in om toegang te krijgen
share
DELEN

Deel dit onderdeel of sectie (kopieer de link)

  • Optie A:
    Klik op de rechtermuisknop op de link en selecteer de optie “linkadres kopiëren”
  • Optie B:
    Deel de link per e-mail

Abstract

Perception of simple temporal patterns has been shown to rely on accentuations in terms of intensity, pitch, or timbre, but also on grouping according to runs of the same events (intervals between successive sounds or light flashes) or significant gaps between them (Garner in The processing of information and structure. Lawrence Erlbaum, 1974; Preusser et al. in Am J Psychol 83(2):151–170 in 1970; Royer and Garner in Percept Psychophys 1(1):41–47, 1966; Royer and Garner in Percept Psychophys 7(2):115–120, 1970; Yu et al. in Atten Percept Psychophys 77(8):2728–2739, 2015). Here we investigate whether the run and gap principles can also account for participants' perceived start of complex rhythmic patterns. We also investigated the role of participants' musical training. Sixteen novices and 16 amateur musicians listened to rhythmic patterns and indicated perceived starting points by a single tap with a drumstick on electronic pads. Auditory patterns contained prominent gaps, runs, or a combination of the two for target intervals. We systematically varied task complexity in terms of the target durations of intervals constituting the patterns and overall tempos. Overall, run and gap principles proved to be useful grouping principles accounting for a large proportion (59.2%) of the selected starting positions underlining the universal relevance of these principles. Grouping principles were not as successful in predicting the perceived start of a rhythmic pattern compared to previous studies. Results indicate that additional grouping principles must be at play. Predictive power of the grouping principles varied depending on the structure of rhythmic patterns. For rhythmic patterns including longer intervals (i.e., longer gaps) the gap principle alone or in combination with the run principle showed the strongest predictive power. Novices and amateur musicians were similar in their usage of grouping principles suggesting that the underlying principles might be equally at the dispositions of performers and listeners.
Literatuur
go back to reference Bouwer, F. L., Burgoyne, J. A., Odijk, D., Honing, H., & Grahn, J. A. (2018). What makes a rhythm complex? The influence of musical training and accent type on beat perception. PLoS ONE, 13(1), e0190322.PubMedCentralCrossRefPubMed Bouwer, F. L., Burgoyne, J. A., Odijk, D., Honing, H., & Grahn, J. A. (2018). What makes a rhythm complex? The influence of musical training and accent type on beat perception. PLoS ONE, 13(1), e0190322.PubMedCentralCrossRefPubMed
go back to reference Bregman, A. S. (1990). Auditory scene analysis: The perceptual organization of sound. The MIT Press.CrossRef Bregman, A. S. (1990). Auditory scene analysis: The perceptual organization of sound. The MIT Press.CrossRef
go back to reference Brown, S. W. (2006). Timing and executive function: Bidirectional interference between concurrent temporal production and randomization tasks. Memory & Cognition, 34(7), 1464–1471.CrossRef Brown, S. W. (2006). Timing and executive function: Bidirectional interference between concurrent temporal production and randomization tasks. Memory & Cognition, 34(7), 1464–1471.CrossRef
go back to reference Collyer, C. E., Boatright-horowitz, S. S., & Hooper, S. (1997). A motor timing experiment implemented using a musical instrument digital interface (MIDI) approach. Behavior Research Methods, Instruments, & Computers, 29(3), 346–352.CrossRef Collyer, C. E., Boatright-horowitz, S. S., & Hooper, S. (1997). A motor timing experiment implemented using a musical instrument digital interface (MIDI) approach. Behavior Research Methods, Instruments, & Computers, 29(3), 346–352.CrossRef
go back to reference Collyer, C. E., Broadbent, H. A., & Church, R. M. (1992). Categorical time production: Evidence for discrete timing in motor control. Perception & Psychophysics, 51(2), 134–144.CrossRef Collyer, C. E., Broadbent, H. A., & Church, R. M. (1992). Categorical time production: Evidence for discrete timing in motor control. Perception & Psychophysics, 51(2), 134–144.CrossRef
go back to reference Collyer, C. E., Broadbent, H. A., & Church, R. M. (1994). Preferred rates of repetitive tapping and categorical time production. Perception & Psychophysics, 55(4), 443–453.CrossRef Collyer, C. E., Broadbent, H. A., & Church, R. M. (1994). Preferred rates of repetitive tapping and categorical time production. Perception & Psychophysics, 55(4), 443–453.CrossRef
go back to reference Drake, C. (1998). Psychological processes involved in the temporal organization of complex auditory sequences: universal and acquired processes. Music Perception; Berkeley, California, 16(1), 11–26.CrossRef Drake, C. (1998). Psychological processes involved in the temporal organization of complex auditory sequences: universal and acquired processes. Music Perception; Berkeley, California, 16(1), 11–26.CrossRef
go back to reference Essens, P. J. (1986). Hierarchical organization of temporal patterns. Perception & Psychophysics, 40(2), 69–73.CrossRef Essens, P. J. (1986). Hierarchical organization of temporal patterns. Perception & Psychophysics, 40(2), 69–73.CrossRef
go back to reference Fraisse, P. (1946). II.—Mouvements rythmiques et arythmiques. L’année Psychologique, 47(1), 11–27.CrossRef Fraisse, P. (1946). II.—Mouvements rythmiques et arythmiques. L’année Psychologique, 47(1), 11–27.CrossRef
go back to reference Fraisse, P. (1974). Psychologie du rythme. PUF. Fraisse, P. (1974). Psychologie du rythme. PUF.
go back to reference Fraisse, P. (1982). 6—Rhythm and tempo. In D. Deutsch (Ed.), Psychology of music (pp. 149–180). Academic Press.CrossRef Fraisse, P. (1982). 6—Rhythm and tempo. In D. Deutsch (Ed.), Psychology of music (pp. 149–180). Academic Press.CrossRef
go back to reference Garner, W. R. (1962). Uncertainty and structure as psychological concepts. Wiley. Garner, W. R. (1962). Uncertainty and structure as psychological concepts. Wiley.
go back to reference Garner, W. R. (1974). The processing of information and structure. Lawrence Erlbaum. Garner, W. R. (1974). The processing of information and structure. Lawrence Erlbaum.
go back to reference Garner, W. R., & Clement, D. E. (1963). Goodness of pattern and pattern uncertainty. Journal of Verbal Learning and Verbal Behavior, 2(5), 446–452.CrossRef Garner, W. R., & Clement, D. E. (1963). Goodness of pattern and pattern uncertainty. Journal of Verbal Learning and Verbal Behavior, 2(5), 446–452.CrossRef
go back to reference Garner, W. R., & Gottwald, R. L. (1967). Some perceptual factors in the learning of sequential patterns of binary events. Journal of Verbal Learning and Verbal Behavior, 6(4), 582–589.CrossRef Garner, W. R., & Gottwald, R. L. (1967). Some perceptual factors in the learning of sequential patterns of binary events. Journal of Verbal Learning and Verbal Behavior, 6(4), 582–589.CrossRef
go back to reference Garner, W. R., & Gottwald, R. L. (1968). The perception and learning of temporal patterns. The Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology, 20(2), 97–109.CrossRefPubMed Garner, W. R., & Gottwald, R. L. (1968). The perception and learning of temporal patterns. The Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology, 20(2), 97–109.CrossRefPubMed
go back to reference Gazzaniga, M. S., Ivry, R. B., & Mangun, G. R. (2019). Cognitive neuroscience: The biology of the mind (5th ed.). W.W. Norton & Company. Gazzaniga, M. S., Ivry, R. B., & Mangun, G. R. (2019). Cognitive neuroscience: The biology of the mind (5th ed.). W.W. Norton & Company.
go back to reference Grondin, S. (2010). Timing and time perception: A review of recent behavioral and neuroscience findings and theoretical directions. Attention, Perception, & Psychophysics, 72(3), 561–582.CrossRef Grondin, S. (2010). Timing and time perception: A review of recent behavioral and neuroscience findings and theoretical directions. Attention, Perception, & Psychophysics, 72(3), 561–582.CrossRef
go back to reference Grondin, S., Meilleur-Wells, G., & Lachance, R. (1999). When to start explicit counting in a time-intervals discrimination task: A critical point in the timing process of humans. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 25, 993–1004. Grondin, S., Meilleur-Wells, G., & Lachance, R. (1999). When to start explicit counting in a time-intervals discrimination task: A critical point in the timing process of humans. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 25, 993–1004.
go back to reference Handel, S. (1989). Listening: An introduction to the perception of auditory events. MIT Press. Handel, S. (1989). Listening: An introduction to the perception of auditory events. MIT Press.
go back to reference Handel, S. (1992). The differentiation of rhythmic structure. Perception & Psychophysics, 52(5), 497–507.CrossRef Handel, S. (1992). The differentiation of rhythmic structure. Perception & Psychophysics, 52(5), 497–507.CrossRef
go back to reference Handel, S. (1993). The effect of tempo and tone duration on rhythm discrimination. Perception & Psychophysics, 54(3), 370–382.CrossRef Handel, S. (1993). The effect of tempo and tone duration on rhythm discrimination. Perception & Psychophysics, 54(3), 370–382.CrossRef
go back to reference Handel, S. (1998). The interplay between metric and figural rhythmic organization. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 24(5), 1546–1561. Handel, S. (1998). The interplay between metric and figural rhythmic organization. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 24(5), 1546–1561.
go back to reference Hestermann, L. D., Wagemans, J., & Krampe, R. T. (2019). Task-set control, chunking, and hierarchical timing in rhythm production. Psychological Research Psychologische Forschung, 83(8), 1685–1702.CrossRefPubMed Hestermann, L. D., Wagemans, J., & Krampe, R. T. (2019). Task-set control, chunking, and hierarchical timing in rhythm production. Psychological Research Psychologische Forschung, 83(8), 1685–1702.CrossRefPubMed
go back to reference Ivry, R. B., & Hazeltine, R. E. (1995). Perception and production of temporal intervals across a range of durations: Evidence for a common timing mechanism. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 21(1), 3–18.PubMed Ivry, R. B., & Hazeltine, R. E. (1995). Perception and production of temporal intervals across a range of durations: Evidence for a common timing mechanism. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 21(1), 3–18.PubMed
go back to reference Jackendoff, R., & Lerdahl, F. (1983). A generative theory of tonal music. MIT Press. Jackendoff, R., & Lerdahl, F. (1983). A generative theory of tonal music. MIT Press.
go back to reference Keele, S. W., & Hawkins, H. L. (1982). Explorations of individual differences relevant to high level skill. Journal of Motor Behavior, 14(1), 3–23.CrossRefPubMed Keele, S. W., & Hawkins, H. L. (1982). Explorations of individual differences relevant to high level skill. Journal of Motor Behavior, 14(1), 3–23.CrossRefPubMed
go back to reference Keller, P. (1999). Attending in complex musical interactions: The adaptive dual role of meter. Australian Journal of Psychology, 51(3), 166–175.CrossRef Keller, P. (1999). Attending in complex musical interactions: The adaptive dual role of meter. Australian Journal of Psychology, 51(3), 166–175.CrossRef
go back to reference Krampe, R. T., Doumas, M., Lavrysen, A., & Rapp, M. (2010). The costs of taking it slowly: Fast and slow movement timing in older age. Psychology & Aging, 25(4), 980–990.CrossRef Krampe, R. T., Doumas, M., Lavrysen, A., & Rapp, M. (2010). The costs of taking it slowly: Fast and slow movement timing in older age. Psychology & Aging, 25(4), 980–990.CrossRef
go back to reference Krampe, R. T., Engbert, R., & Kliegl, R. (2002). the effects of expertise and age on rhythm production: Adaptations to timing and sequencing constraints. Brain and Cognition, 48(1), 179–194.CrossRefPubMed Krampe, R. T., Engbert, R., & Kliegl, R. (2002). the effects of expertise and age on rhythm production: Adaptations to timing and sequencing constraints. Brain and Cognition, 48(1), 179–194.CrossRefPubMed
go back to reference Krampe, R. T., & Ericsson, K. A. (1996). Maintaining excellence: Deliberate practice and elite performance in young and older pianists. Journal of Experimental Psychology. General, 125(4), 331–359.CrossRefPubMed Krampe, R. T., & Ericsson, K. A. (1996). Maintaining excellence: Deliberate practice and elite performance in young and older pianists. Journal of Experimental Psychology. General, 125(4), 331–359.CrossRefPubMed
go back to reference Krampe, R. T., Kliegl, R., Mayr, U., Engbert, R., & Vorberg, D. (2000). The fast and the slow of skilled bimanual rhythm production: Parallel versus integrated timing. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 26(1), 206–233.PubMed Krampe, R. T., Kliegl, R., Mayr, U., Engbert, R., & Vorberg, D. (2000). The fast and the slow of skilled bimanual rhythm production: Parallel versus integrated timing. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 26(1), 206–233.PubMed
go back to reference Krampe, R. T., Mayr, U., & Kliegl, R. (2005). Timing, sequencing, and executive control in repetitive movement production. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 31(3), 379–397.PubMed Krampe, R. T., Mayr, U., & Kliegl, R. (2005). Timing, sequencing, and executive control in repetitive movement production. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 31(3), 379–397.PubMed
go back to reference Kraus, N., & Chandrasekaran, B. (2010). Music training for the development of auditory skills. Nature Reviews Neuroscience, 11(8), 599–605.CrossRefPubMed Kraus, N., & Chandrasekaran, B. (2010). Music training for the development of auditory skills. Nature Reviews Neuroscience, 11(8), 599–605.CrossRefPubMed
go back to reference Kubovy, M. (1988). Should we resist the seductiveness of the space:time: Vision:audition analogy? Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 14, 318–320. Kubovy, M. (1988). Should we resist the seductiveness of the space:time: Vision:audition analogy? Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 14, 318–320.
go back to reference Kubovy, M., Holcombe, A. O., & Wagemans, J. (1998). On the lawfulness of grouping by proximity. Cognitive Psychology, 35(1), 71–98.CrossRefPubMed Kubovy, M., Holcombe, A. O., & Wagemans, J. (1998). On the lawfulness of grouping by proximity. Cognitive Psychology, 35(1), 71–98.CrossRefPubMed
go back to reference Kubovy, M., & van den Berg, M. (2008). The whole is equal to the sum of its parts: A probabilistic model of grouping by proximity and similarity in regular patterns. Psychological Review, 115(1), 131–154.CrossRefPubMed Kubovy, M., & van den Berg, M. (2008). The whole is equal to the sum of its parts: A probabilistic model of grouping by proximity and similarity in regular patterns. Psychological Review, 115(1), 131–154.CrossRefPubMed
go back to reference Kubovy, M., & Wagemans, J. (1995). Grouping by proximity and multistability in dot lattices: A quantitative Gestalt theory. Psychological Science, 6(4), 225–234.CrossRef Kubovy, M., & Wagemans, J. (1995). Grouping by proximity and multistability in dot lattices: A quantitative Gestalt theory. Psychological Science, 6(4), 225–234.CrossRef
go back to reference Large, E. W., & Palmer, C. (2002). Perceiving temporal regularity in music. Cognitive Science, 26(1), 1–37.CrossRef Large, E. W., & Palmer, C. (2002). Perceiving temporal regularity in music. Cognitive Science, 26(1), 1–37.CrossRef
go back to reference Lerdahl, F., & Jackendoff, R. (1996). A generative theory of tonal music (repr). MIT.CrossRef Lerdahl, F., & Jackendoff, R. (1996). A generative theory of tonal music (repr). MIT.CrossRef
go back to reference Palmer, C., & Krumhansl, C. L. (1990). Mental representations for musical meter. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 16(4), 728–741.PubMed Palmer, C., & Krumhansl, C. L. (1990). Mental representations for musical meter. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 16(4), 728–741.PubMed
go back to reference Povel, D.-J., & Essens, P. (1985). Perception of temporal patterns. Music Perception: An Interdisciplinary Journal, 2(4), 411–440.CrossRef Povel, D.-J., & Essens, P. (1985). Perception of temporal patterns. Music Perception: An Interdisciplinary Journal, 2(4), 411–440.CrossRef
go back to reference Preusser, D., Garner, W. R., & Gottwald, R. L. (1970). Perceptual organization of two-element temporal patterns as a function of their component one-element patterns. The American Journal of Psychology, 83(2), 151–170.CrossRefPubMed Preusser, D., Garner, W. R., & Gottwald, R. L. (1970). Perceptual organization of two-element temporal patterns as a function of their component one-element patterns. The American Journal of Psychology, 83(2), 151–170.CrossRefPubMed
go back to reference Rammsayer, T., & Altenmüller, E. (2006). Temporal information processing in musicians and nonmusicians. Music Perception: an Interdisciplinary Journal, 24(1), 37–48.CrossRef Rammsayer, T., & Altenmüller, E. (2006). Temporal information processing in musicians and nonmusicians. Music Perception: an Interdisciplinary Journal, 24(1), 37–48.CrossRef
go back to reference Repp, B. H. (1999). Detecting deviations from metronomic timing in music: Effects of perceptual structure on the mental timekeeper. Perception & Psychophysics, 61(3), 529–548.CrossRef Repp, B. H. (1999). Detecting deviations from metronomic timing in music: Effects of perceptual structure on the mental timekeeper. Perception & Psychophysics, 61(3), 529–548.CrossRef
go back to reference Repp, B. H. (2001). Phase correction, phase resetting, and phase shifts after subliminal timing perturbations in sensorimotor synchronization. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 27(3), 600–621.PubMed Repp, B. H. (2001). Phase correction, phase resetting, and phase shifts after subliminal timing perturbations in sensorimotor synchronization. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 27(3), 600–621.PubMed
go back to reference Repp, B. H. (2005). Sensorimotor synchronization: A review of the tapping literature. Psychonomic Bulletin & Review, 12(6), 969–992.CrossRef Repp, B. H. (2005). Sensorimotor synchronization: A review of the tapping literature. Psychonomic Bulletin & Review, 12(6), 969–992.CrossRef
go back to reference Repp, B. H., & Su, Y.-H. (2013). Sensorimotor synchronization: A review of recent research (2006–2012). Psychonomic Bulletin & Review, 20(3), 403–452.CrossRef Repp, B. H., & Su, Y.-H. (2013). Sensorimotor synchronization: A review of recent research (2006–2012). Psychonomic Bulletin & Review, 20(3), 403–452.CrossRef
go back to reference Royer, F. L., & Garner, W. R. (1966). Response uncertainty and perceptual difficulty of auditory temporal patterns. Perception & Psychophysics, 1(1), 41–47.CrossRef Royer, F. L., & Garner, W. R. (1966). Response uncertainty and perceptual difficulty of auditory temporal patterns. Perception & Psychophysics, 1(1), 41–47.CrossRef
go back to reference Royer, F. L., & Garner, W. R. (1970). Perceptual organization of nine-element auditory temporal patterns. Perception & Psychophysics, 7(2), 115–120.CrossRef Royer, F. L., & Garner, W. R. (1970). Perceptual organization of nine-element auditory temporal patterns. Perception & Psychophysics, 7(2), 115–120.CrossRef
go back to reference Sloboda, J. A. (1986). The musical mind: The cognitive psychology of music (reprint edition). Oxford University Press.CrossRef Sloboda, J. A. (1986). The musical mind: The cognitive psychology of music (reprint edition). Oxford University Press.CrossRef
go back to reference Ten Hoopen, G. (1992). Temporal processing of fast auditory patterns. In: Paper presented at the 2nd international conference on music perception and cognition, Los Angeles. Ten Hoopen, G. (1992). Temporal processing of fast auditory patterns. In: Paper presented at the 2nd international conference on music perception and cognition, Los Angeles.
go back to reference Wagemans, J. (2018). Perceptual organization. In J. T. Wixted & J. Serences (Eds.), In: The Stevens’ handbook of experimental psychology and cognitive neuroscience. Sensation, perception and attention (Vol. 2, pp. 803–872). Wiley. Wagemans, J. (2018). Perceptual organization. In J. T. Wixted & J. Serences (Eds.), In: The Stevens’ handbook of experimental psychology and cognitive neuroscience. Sensation, perception and attention (Vol. 2, pp. 803–872). Wiley.
go back to reference Yu, M., Getz, L., & Kubovy, M. (2015). Perceiving the initial note: Quantitative models of how listeners parse cyclical auditory patterns. Attention, Perception, & Psychophysics, 77(8), 2728–2739.CrossRef Yu, M., Getz, L., & Kubovy, M. (2015). Perceiving the initial note: Quantitative models of how listeners parse cyclical auditory patterns. Attention, Perception, & Psychophysics, 77(8), 2728–2739.CrossRef
go back to reference Zatorre, R. J., Chen, J. L., & Penhune, V. B. (2007). When the brain plays music: Auditory–motor interactions in music perception and production. Nature Reviews Neuroscience, 8(7), 547–558.CrossRefPubMed Zatorre, R. J., Chen, J. L., & Penhune, V. B. (2007). When the brain plays music: Auditory–motor interactions in music perception and production. Nature Reviews Neuroscience, 8(7), 547–558.CrossRefPubMed
Metagegevens
Titel
Perceptual grouping in complex rhythmic patterns
Auteurs
Lars Dietmar Hestermann
Johan Wagemans
Ralf T. Krampe
Publicatiedatum
16-08-2022
Uitgeverij
Springer Berlin Heidelberg
Gepubliceerd in
Psychological Research / Uitgave 4/2023
Print ISSN: 0340-0727
Elektronisch ISSN: 1430-2772
DOI
https://doi.org/10.1007/s00426-022-01717-4

Andere artikelen Uitgave 4/2023

Psychological Research 4/2023 Naar de uitgave