Skip to main content
Top
Gepubliceerd in: Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders 11/2023

Open Access 17-08-2022 | Original Paper

Parental Imitations and Expansions of Child Language Predict Later Language Outcomes of Autistic Preschoolers

Auteurs: Jodie Smith, Rhylee Sulek, Kailia Van Der Wert, Olivia Cincotta-Lee, Cherie C. Green, Catherine A. Bent, Lacey Chetcuti, Kristelle Hudry

Gepubliceerd in: Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders | Uitgave 11/2023

Abstract

Both the amount and responsiveness of adult language input contribute to the language development of autistic and non-autistic children. From parent–child interaction footage, we measured the amount of adult language input, overall parent responsiveness, and six discrete parent responsive behaviours (imitations, expansions, open-ended questions, yes/no questions, comments and acknowledgements) to explore which types of responsiveness predicted autistic preschoolers’ language five months later, after controlling for adult language input. We found expansions and particularly imitations to be more important for later language than overall responsiveness. This study emphasises the need to capture what exactly about parent language input influences child language acquisition, and adds to the evidence that imitating and expanding early language might be particularly beneficial for autistic preschoolers.
Opmerkingen

Publisher's Note

Springer Nature remains neutral with regard to jurisdictional claims in published maps and institutional affiliations.
Acquisition of oral language is an important developmental milestone. In non-autistic cohorts, language is not only fundamental for communicating with others but a prerequisite for the advancement of other skills such as emergent literacy (Zubrick et al., 2015), attention and prosocial behaviour (Bretherton et al., 2013). There is likewise growing evidence that early language skills are predictive of social, academic, adaptive, and vocational outcomes in autistic children (Durkin et al., 2011; Gillespie-Lynch et al., 2012; Howlin et al., 2000). Building awareness of specific factors that robustly contribute to early language acquisition is important if we are to optimise developmental outcomes for children on the autism spectrum.

Adult–Child Interaction and Language Development

Language learning takes place in social contexts beginning with interactions between parents and infants (Bruner, 1975; Vygotsky, 1962). Studies of the general population highlight that both the quantity (i.e., overall amount) and quality (i.e., syntactic and semantic diversity) of adult language input during early interactions shape children’s early language trajectories (Rowe, 2012; Smith et al., 2021). There is likewise compelling evidence of similar associations for parental input supporting the language acquisition of autistic children (Naigles, 2013). As an illustration, Fusaroli et al. (2019) discovered that autistic preschoolers had longer utterances, together with larger, richer vocabularies, when they had been exposed to longer parent utterances earlier in childhood. Swanson et al. (2019) similarly found that exposure to more adult words at ages 9 and 15 months was associated with better subsequent language at age 24 months for both infants at higher- and lower-likelihood of developing autism.
Another unique predictor of child language outcomes in non-autistic cohorts is adult responsiveness (Eshel et al., 2006)—that is, the extent to which adults provide responses that are timely, semantically contingent, and developmentally appropriate in relation to what a child has said or done (Tamis-LeMonda et al., 2001). As autistic children can have difficulties with joint attention and shifting social attention (Adamson et al., 2019; Mo et al., 2019), adult responsiveness may be particularly important for facilitating language for young autistic children (Hudry et al., 2013; McDuffie & Yoder, 2010; Siller & Sigman, 2002) since reduced attentional demands should theoretically increase their capacity for word learning (Hoff, 2006). Notably, since quality and quantity language measures are largely consistent across the literature and easily replicable—i.e., often operationalised as amount/diversity of adult words and mean length of utterances (MLU)—comparing findings here is relatively straightforward. In contrast, cross-study comparison of findings in relation to adult responsiveness is complicated by opaque and inconsistent measurement.

Global vs. Discrete Measurement of Parent Responsiveness

Parent responsiveness is typically operationalised through coding parent–child interaction footage using measures that are either global (i.e., rating/Likert scales) or discrete (i.e., capturing “moment-to-moment” responsiveness) (Barnett et al., 2022; Morawska et al., 2014). In a recent systematic review and meta-analysis, Edmunds et al. (2019) found a significant positive effect of parent responsiveness on communication outcomes for children with, or at higher likelihood of, autism. This relationship did not differ according to the use of global versus discrete measurement, although there was more variability when global ratings were used. This indicates that while both measurement types can meaningfully capture parent responsiveness, discrete coding may offer a more nuanced picture. Moreover, from an application perspective, global responsiveness measures are often subjective impacting measurement precision (Morawska et al., 2014). Further, global measures comprise multiple behaviours making it difficult to identify which specific aspects of the interaction are facilitating language acquisition (Hirsh-Pasek et al., 2015).
Prior attempts to capture ‘moment-to-moment’ micro-level responsiveness have lacked specificity and/or informative detail; in particular, the coding of individual parent responsive utterances but failure to further subcategorise (i.e., whether a parent’s response is in the form of an imitation, a question or a comment). Such lack of specificity could partly explain inconsistencies—and indeed null findings—in the extant literature here. For instance, Flippin and Watson (2015) measured composite responsive parent verbal utterances (including comments, requests, and directives) provided by mothers and fathers of young autistic children. They found that fathers’ verbal responsiveness was significantly related to overall child language skills, while mothers’ verbal responsiveness was unrelated to child language. Choi et al. (2020) similarly totaled contingent verbal responses (i.e., occurrences of verbal and non-verbal infant communicative behaviours that were followed by a parent response) in dyads with and without a family history of autism, along with parents’ MLU. Whilst MLU was associated with subsequent child language abilities, there was a surprising null effect for contingent responses in their study.

Discrete Responsive Behaviours

Studies in the general population literature that have further categorised discrete parental responsive behaviours into more specific subcomponents (e.g., imitations and expansions of child language, responsive comments etc.) have yielded more consistent findings than those that have relied on composite measures. For example, an association between parent use of both responsive imitations (i.e., Child: “Car”; Adult: “Car”) and expansions (i.e., Child: “Car”; Adult: “Red car”), and concurrent and subsequent child language is now accepted as fairly robust (Levickis et al., 2014; Smith et al., 2018a, 2018b; Tamis-LeMonda et al., 2001). In the context of early childhood autism, however, few studies have taken such a nuanced view of parent responsiveness and none have compared the relative predictive value of overall and discrete parent responsiveness; yet, gathering a more nuanced, fine-tuned understanding of adult responsiveness during interactions with young autistic children may be especially important here. There are likely subtle perturbations—whether from inherent child social communication difficulties or attendant parent behaviours—influencing parent–child interactions with young autistic children (Wan et al., 2013). Consequently, there may be unforeseen generalisability issues with using previously published checklists and measures developed in non-autistic populations within neurodiverse samples.
Of all discrete responsive behaviours, we might expect that behaviours that include copying back child language (i.e., imitations and expansions) may be more valuable for autistic children than is the case in neurotypical development (Field, 2017). This hypothesis is based on evidence suggesting that being imitated can support increased social behaviours for autistic children, including increased vocalisations (Field et al., 2001), imitations (Field et al., 2008) and communicative gaze (Sanefuji & Ohgami, 2011). These responsive parent behaviours that explicitly echo and/or extend on what a child has said (i.e., imitations and expansions) also shape and encourage early words by promptly reinforcing the accurate adult target word (Tamis-LeMonda et al., 2001) and support communicative intent by providing immediate feedback to the child that verbal sounds have meaning (Bruner, 1975; Rowland & Fried-Oken, 2010). As for other responsive behaviours—such as responsive questions (i.e., Child: Holds a car; Parent: “What’s that?” [WH question] or “Is that a car?” [yes/no question]) and comments/acknowledgements (i.e., Child: Holds a car; Parent: “That’s a car!” [comment] or “Wow!” [acknowledgement])—it is plausible that these may have less influence on early language acquisition because they do not immediately reflect back or build upon a child’s own words/sounds. This may be especially true for autistic children who may benefit most from the strengthening of intentional communication afforded by immediacy and attentional contingency.
There is less consistency in the literature with regard to the effect of parent responsive behaviours on children’s language skills in the context of autism, compared to in the general population. For example, expansions have sometimes been related to concurrent and subsequent child language (McDuffie & Yoder, 2010; Naigles, 2013) and sometimes not (Haebig et al., 2013). Surprisingly, imitations have been found to be unrelated to later language in two previous studies of parents and young autistic children (Haebig et al., 2013; McDuffie & Yoder, 2010). And, responsive commenting has been found to predict change in child expressive vocabulary in one study (McDuffie & Yoder, 2010) but was unrelated to later language for more verbally fluent children in a subsequent study (Haebig et al., 2013). We know little about the role of responsive questions and acknowledgements on later language outcomes in autistic cohorts. Small samples sizes and few dedicated studies may contribute to the inconsistency we see in the autistic literature versus that on non-autistic children, necessitating replication studies and larger cohorts.

Current Study

Drawing on data from a cohort of parents and their autistic preschoolers, we sought to investigate the relative predictive value of six discrete parent responsive behaviours for later child language. We broadly hypothesised that:
1.
Discrete responsive behaviours would account for more variance in receptive and expressive child language outcomes than overall parent responsiveness, and;
 
2.
The six discrete behaviours would contribute differentially, with behaviours that explicitly echo and/or extend on what a child has said (i.e., expansions and imitations) accounting for more variance than those that do not (i.e., WH questions, yes/no questions, comments and acknowledgements).
 

Method

Design and Procedure

This study utilised data collected from a larger longitudinal study of autistic children and their families conducted between 2017 and 2019. Ethics approval for the study was obtained from La Trobe University Human Research Ethics Committee; HREC #16-136. Children were eligible for the study if aged 17–43 months, diagnosed with autism and with a non-verbal developmental age-equivalence ≥ 12 months. Parents also needed to speak sufficient English to understand written and spoken information to partake in this research. Autism diagnoses were confirmed through administration of the Autism Diagnostic Observation Schedule (ADOS-2; Lord et al., 2012) by a research-reliable assessor. Measures of child developmental skills (including language) and parent–child interaction (PCI) footage were collected during research assessments completed at baseline and five-month follow-up. Where two parents attended assessments, the self-designated primary caregiver participated in the PCI video.

Participants

Table 1 provides baseline characteristics for the 53 parent–child dyads comprised in this study. Most children were male with an average age under three years. Mean verbal and non-verbal developmental skills were below chronological age expectations. Participating parents were largely well educated, biological mothers. A substantial minority subgroup of parents reported low-income status (n = 3 missing), and 24 families self-described as culturally and linguistically diverse (with varied home languages including Mandarin, Cantonese, Tamil and others). Codes for baseline parent–child interaction were available for 49 parents (n = 4 missing; reasons for missing videos included declined consent to be filmed and predominant use of a language other than English in videos).
Table 1
Baseline characteristics of participating parents and children
Characteristics
N
M/n (SD/%)
Range
Child
   
 Age (months)
53
33.87 (6.92)
17–43
 Sex (male)
53
43 (81.13)
 
Mullen Scales of Early Learning Developmental Quotient
53
  
 Non-verbal
 
71.56 (23.55)
31–133
 Verbal
 
58.20 (32.70)
12–134
MacArthur–Bates communicative development
   
 Receptive vocabulary
43
245.05 (179.41)
0–669
 Expressive vocabulary
43
141.49 (184.49)
0–630
Autism diagnostic observation schedule CSS
53
7.06 (1.68)
3–10
Primary parent
   
 Mothers
53
45 (84.91)
 
 Age (years)
45
36 (4.56)
26–48
 Education level
42
  
  Primary
 
1 (2.38)
 
  Secondary
 
6 (14.29)
 
  Tertiary
 
19 (45.24)
 
  Postgraduate
 
16 (38.10)
 
Family
   
 Culturally and linguistically diverse
45
24 (53.33)
 
 Low income status
45
16 (35.56)
 
Primary home languages other than English (top 6)
   
 Mandarin
47
6 (12.77)
 
 Tamil
47
4 (8.51)
 
 Japanese
47
1 (2.13)
 
 Urdu
47
1 (2.13)
 
 Vietnamese
47
1 (2.13)
 
 Sinhala
47
1 (2.13)
 

Measures

Predictors: Parent Responsive Behaviours

Parent–child interaction sampling was in the context of ten minutes of free-play with an identical set of age-appropriate toys provided to each parent (i.e., books, blocks, pretend play materials, a spinning top etc.). Videos were transcribed and coded using Systematic Analysis of Language Transcripts (SALT) software following pre-prescribed coding conventions (Miller et al., 2011). Basic SALT measures for both parent and child (i.e., total words and MLU) were retained for inclusion as potential covariates in the current analysis (see below). Videos and transcripts were then reviewed and all parent utterances that were timely (within < 5 s following a child behaviour or utterance), semantically contingent, and developmentally appropriate (Tamis-LeMonda et al., 2001) were coded as responsive. Proportionate responsiveness was computed for analysis as the number of responsive utterances divided by total number of parent utterances. Videos and transcripts were reviewed again to further sub-code all responsive utterances into exhaustive and mutually-exclusive categories of imitations, expansions, WH questions, yes/no questions, comments or acknowledgements. Table 2 presents detailed descriptions and summary descriptive statistics of responsive behaviours. Coded procedures were based on past research (see Levickis et al., 2014; Smith et al., 2019).
Table 2
Detailed descriptions of baseline parental responsiveness measures (predictors) and follow-up child language (outcomes)
Variable
Type
Definition
Sample (n)
M (SD), range
Predictor
Responsiveness (proportionate)
Proportion of parent utterances that were responsive to child language or behaviour. Calculated by dividing responsive utterances by overall number of utterances
49
.48 (.17), .11–.82
Predictor
Imitations
Parent repeated the child’s preceding verbal or non-verbal vocalisation or verbalisation exactly or with a reduction of words (e.g., Child: “Car”; Parent: “Car”)
49
5.82 (7.20), 0–35
Predictor
Expansions
Parent repeated one or all of the child’s preceding words/sounds and added language (e.g., Child: “Car”; Parent: “It is a car!”)
49
4.04 (5.23), 0–19
Predictor
WH questionsa
Parent asked a “wh” question (e.g., “what,” “when,” “who”), following a child act or behaviour (e.g., Child picks up a car; Parent: “What’s that?”). ‘How’ questions included here too
49
4.27 (4.19), 0–23
Predictor
Yes/No questionsa
Parent asked a question requiring a binary (yes or no) answer following a child act or behaviour (e.g., Child picks up a car; Parent: “Is that a car?”)
49
7.45 (7.04), 0–28
Predictor
Commentsa
Parent commented on a child’s action or behaviour (e.g., Child plays with a car; Parent: “That’s a car!”). No response required from the child. Excluded acknowledgements/exclamations
49
45.27 (22.31), 5–91
Predictor
Acknowledgementsa
Exclamation or acknowledgement of a child’s action or behaviour (e.g., Child crashed the car; Parent: “Uh-oh!”)b. No response required from the child
49
22.20 (12.86), 3–61
Outcome
Mullen Scales of Early Learning (MSEL)
Examiner assessed measure of child development. Expressive and Receptive Language Subscales retained with age-equivalent scores (in months) used for analysis
40
Expressivec: 24.83 (12.87), 3–45
Receptivec: 25.17 (14.34). 2–55
Outcome
Vineland Adaptive Behavior Scales (VABS)
Parent-report measure of child adaptive functioning collected via parent interview. Expressive and Receptive Language Subscales retained with age-equivalent scores (in months) used for analysis
50
Expressivec: 25.80 (12.73), 5–60
Receptivec: 26.12 (14.67). 3–66
Outcome
MacArthur-Bates communicative development (MCDI)
Parent-report measure of the number of words a child understands and uses collected via parent questionnaire. Cumulative expressive and receptive vocabulary scores used for analysis
44
Expressive: 257.75 (235.66), 0–669
Receptive: 369.91(194.89). 0–675
aAs coding was mutually exclusive, if parent utterance also imitated or expanded child utterance, imitation/expansion took precedence; bcommon responses included: yeah, no, wee, wow, oh-dear, mm, okay, hooray; c age-equivalence scores (in months)
Interrater reliability was evaluated by having 20% of randomly-selected videos coded by a second trained researcher. We follow Syed and Nelson (2015)’s recommendation of reporting multiple indices of reliability; here, Intraclass Correlation Coefficients (ICC) and Cohen’s kappa statistics. Reliability was good-to-excellent for proportionate responsiveness (κ = .79, ICC > .99), imitations (κ = .70, ICC = .93), expansions (κ = .65, ICC = .97), WH questions (κ = 1.00, ICC = 1.00), comments (κ = .75, ICC = 1.00) and acknowledgements (κ = .69, ICC = .98), and acceptable for yes/no questions (κ = .56, ICC = .97).

Outcomes: Child Language

Available standardised measures of child language included Expressive and Receptive subscales of the Mullen Scales of Early Learning (MSEL; Mullen, 1995) and Vineland Adaptive Behavior Scales 2nd Edition (VABS-II; Sparrow et al., 2005), as well as vocabulary data from the MacArthur-Bates Communicative Development Inventories (MCDI; Fenson et al., 2007). The MSEL Expressive and Receptive Language subscales have acceptable internal consistency (α > .85) and good construct validity against other measures of cognitive ability (r = .74) (Swineford et al., 2015). The VABS-II has good inter-interviewer reliability for the Communication Domain (r = .76) and has been validated for use with autistic children (Sparrow et al., 2005). The MCDI also has good reliability and predictive validity for later language in autistic children (Luyster et al., 2007). To avoid floor effects in our data, we used MSEL and VABS-II Expressive and Receptive subscale age equivalent scores in our analysis. For MCDI data, we retained raw counts of expressive and receptive vocabulary.

Covariates

We considered several child and parent factors as potential covariates for inclusion in analyses. Due to the small sample size, a preliminary correlation analysis was conducted to determine the associations between child language outcomes and predictor measures coded from PCI footage, and each of child autism severity (ADOS-2 calibrated severity scores, range 1–10; Lord et al., 2012), cognitive ability (MSEL Developmental Quotient [DQ]; Mullen, 1995) and adaptive behavior (Adaptive Behavior Composite [ABC]; Sparrow et al., 2005). Our SALT measures of child language from interaction videos (i.e., linguistic opportunities for parent responsiveness)—namely child total words (CTW), child different words (CDW) and child mean length of utterance (MLU)—were highly intercorrelated (r = .686 to .850). For parsimony, of all child measures, we only retained CTW (M = 57.73, SD = 71.76, Range: 0–256) as a covariate because it showed the strongest associations with other child measures: cognitive ability (r = .686) and adaptive behaviour (r = .531). Parent MLU (M = 2.78 SD = 87, Range: 1.34–5.01) was included as a second covariate to control for the quality of parent linguistic input (Fusaroli et al., 2019; Swanson et al., 2019).

Data Analysis

We note that study data collection coincided with local community lockdowns related to COVID-19, resulting in a loss of MSEL (i.e., direct assessment) data for 24.5% (n = 13) of the sample. Across the remaining variables of interest, missing data ranged between 5.7 and 17%, and were at random χ2(24) = 34.80, p = .71. All analyses have been conducted with available data only.
A series of Pearson’s correlations were run to identify relationships between our measures. Significant correlates were then entered into a series of hierarchical multiple regressions to determine the additional predictive value of parent responsiveness (entered at Step 2; either proportionate parent responsiveness or discrete parent responsiveness behaviours) for child receptive and expressive language outcomes, beyond the effect of covariates (entered at Step 1; CTW, parent MLU and home language). Two PCI videos were partly in a language other than English. A sensitivity analysis conducted to see if the final results changed when these were included/excluded yielded substantively identical results, so data are reported for all coded videos (n = 49).

Results

Table 3 shows a pattern of moderate-to-strong significant correlations between all child language outcome measures and the covariates parent MLU and CTW. Proportionate responsiveness was moderately strongly correlated with MSEL receptive language (r = .439) and weakly so with VABS-II expressive language (r = .439). All child language outcomes were moderately correlated with imitations (r = .315–555), and moderate-to strongly correlated with expansions (r = .421–.799). Significant, weak-to-moderate correlations were found between WH questions and VABS-II receptive (r = .378) and expressive language (r = .451) and MCDI expressive vocabulary (r = .309), with a weak correlation found between yes/no questions and MSEL expressive language (r = .340).
Table 3
Full correlation matrix of covariates, predictors and outcomes
 
PMLU
CTW
ACK
COM
YN
WH
IMI
EXP
Prop Resp
MSEL RL
MSEL EL
VABS RL
VABS EL
MCDI RL
MCDI EL
Income
.452**
.224
.278
.219
.100
.204
.125
.295
.073
.127
.142
.259
.373*
.291
.323*
n
42
42
42
42
42
42
42
42
42
34
34
43
43
40
40
PMLU
 
.652**
.170
.279
.235
.237
.135
.585**
.164
.578**
.674**
.482**
.703**
.743**
.694**
n
 
49
49
49
49
49
49
49
49
38
38
47
47
41
41
CTW
  
− .094
− .109
.187
.304*
.319*
.798**
.178
.748**
.725**
.463**
.721**
.744**
.782**
n
  
49
49
49
49
49
49
49
38
38
47
47
41
41
ACK
   
.589**
.479**
.476**
− .101
.058
.480**
− .046
− .077
.054
.060
− .163
− .081
n
   
49
49
49
49
49
49
38
38
47
47
41
41
COM
    
.544**
.277
.292*
.140
.618**
.211
.126
.149
.134
− .020
− .024
n
    
49
49
49
49
49
38
38
47
47
41
41
YN
     
.643**
.283*
.334*
.626**
.292
.340*
.259
.268
.124
.198
n
     
49
49
49
49
38
38
47
47
41
41
WH
      
.207
.508**
.595**
.201
.315
.378**
.451**
.206
.309*
n
      
49
49
49
38
38
47
47
41
41
IMI
       
.484**
.315*
.447**
.407*
.555**
.470**
.395*
.405**
n
       
49
49
38
38
47
47
41
41
EXP
        
.421**
.732**
.635**
.598**
.799**
.688**
.724**
n
        
49
38
38
47
47
41
41
Prop Resp.
         
.439**
.285
.200
.303*
.197
.246
n
         
38
38
47
47
41
41
MSEL RL
          
.831**
.551**
.780**
.813**
.827**
n
          
40
39
39
36
36
MSEL EL
           
.585**
.854**
.841**
.911**
n
           
39
39
36
36
VABS RL
            
.814**
.698**
.659**
n
            
50
44
44
VABS EL
             
.857**
.887**
n
             
44
44
MCDI RL
              
.926**
n
              
44
PMLU parent mean length of utterance, CTW child total words, ACK acknowledgments, COM comments, YN yes/no questions, WH WH questions, IMI imitations, EXP expansions, Prop Resp proportionate responsiveness, MSEL RL Mullen Scales of Early Learning receptive language age equivalence, MSEL EL Mullen Scales of Early Learning expressive language age equivalence, VABS RL Vineland Adaptive Behaviour Scales receptive language age equivalence, VABS EL Vineland Adaptive Behaviour Scales expressive language age equivalence, MCDI RL MacArthur-Bates communicative development inventories receptive vocabulary, MCDI EL MacArthur-Bates communicative development inventories expressive vocabulary
*p < .05, **p < .001

Prediction of Child Language Outcomes

Tables 4, 5, and 6 present results of hierarchical multiple regressions. Where both types of responsiveness (i.e., proportionate and discrete behaviours) were related to child language, the predictive value of each of these for a given outcome measure was evaluated in separate regression models (as these were non-independent so could not be included within the same model).
Table 4
Hierarchical multiple regression for directly assessed expressive and receptive language measures (MSEL)
Receptive language
Expressive language
 
β
R2
F
ΔR2
ΔF
 
β
R2
F
ΔR2
ΔF
Model 1
           
 Step 1
 
.60
14.38**
      
  PMLU
.205
          
  CTW
.648**
          
  Home language
.086
          
 Step 2
 
.69
15.90**
.10
8.83*
      
  PMLU
.130
          
  CTW
.658**
          
  Home language
.107
          
  Proportionate responsiveness
.321*
          
Model 2
           
 Step 1
 
.60
14.38**
Step 1
 
.64
17.14**
  PMLU
.205
    
 PMLU
.370*
    
  CTW
.648**
    
 CTW
.514**
    
  Home language
.086
    
 Home language
− .001
    
 Step 2
 
.68
11.51**
.08
3.49*
Step 2
 
.68
9.38**
.05
1.22
  PMLU
.143
    
 PMLU
.349*
    
  CTW
.432*
    
 CTW
.413*
    
  Home language
.023
    
 Home language
− .042
    
  Imitations
.059
    
 Imitations
.172
    
  Expansions
.344
    
 Expansions
.076
    
      
 Yes/No Qs
.042
    
*p < .05, **p < .001
Table 5
Hierarchical multiple regression for parent reported expressive and receptive language measures (VABS)
Receptive language
Expressive language
 
β
R2
F
ΔR2
ΔF
 
β
R2
F
ΔR2
ΔF
Model 1
     
Step 1
 
.57
16.45**
      
 PMLU
.425*
    
      
 CTW
.418*
    
      
 Home Language
.020
    
      
Step 2
 
.60
13.65**
.03
2.82
      
 PMLU
.420*
    
      
 CTW
.416*
    
      
 Home Language
.077
    
      
 Proportionate Responsiveness
.186
    
Model 2
           
 Step 1
 
.27
4.46*
Step 1
 
.57
16.45**
  PMLU
.398*
    
 PMLU
.425*
    
  CTW
.201
    
 CTW
.418*
    
  Home language
.199
    
 Home Language
.020
    
 Step 2
 
.52
6.13**
.25
6.00*
Step 2
 
.76
17.92*
.19
8.89**
  PMLU
.404*
    
 PMLU
.394*
    
  CTW
− .114
    
 CTW
.086
    
  Home language
.113
    
 Home Language
− .036
    
  Imitations
.406*
    
 Imitations
.225*
    
 Expansions
.191
    
 Expansions
.335*
    
 ‘Wh’ Questions
.137
    
 ‘Wh’ Questions
.123
    
*p < .05, **p < .001
Table 6
Hierarchical multiple regression for parent reported expressive and receptive vocabulary (MCDI)
Receptive vocabulary
Expressive vocabulary
 
β
R2
F
ΔR2
ΔF
 
β
R2
F
ΔR2
ΔF
Step 1
 
.66
21.50**
Step 1
 
.66
22.04**
 PMLU
.469*
    
 PMLU
.346*
    
 CTW
.425*
    
 CTW
.555**
    
 Home language
.030
    
 Home language
.063
    
Step 2
 
.74
17.84**
.08
4.92*
Step 2
 
.73
14.42**
.08
2.97*
 PMLU
.510**
    
 PMLU
.384*
    
 CTW
.244*
    
 CTW
.366*
    
 Home language
− .048
    
 Home language
.004
    
 Imitations
.281*
    
 Imitations
.237*
    
 Expansions
.084
    
 Expansions
.098
    
      
 ‘Wh’ Questions
.062
    
*p < .05, **p < .001

Predictors of Assessed Child Language

In the prediction of child MSEL receptive language, covariates entered at Step 1 (CTW, parent MLU and home language) contributed significantly to the model, explaining 59.8% of the variance, although only CTW made a significant unique contribution (β = .658, p < .001). The addition of proportionate responsiveness at Step 2 added significant predictive value, explaining a further 9.6% of the variance, and with both proportionate responsiveness (β = .321, p = .006) and the covariate CTW (β = .658, p < .001) significant unique predictors of MSEL receptive language; R2 = .69, F(4, 28) = 15.90, p < .001. In the alternate model, with discrete responsiveness behaviours (specifically, imitations and expansions) instead added at Step 2, these explained a significant further 8.3% of variance in scores; R2 = .68, F(5, 27) = 11.51, p < .001. Here, however, only the covariate CTW (β = .432, p = .013), but neither discrete predictor, imitations (β = .059, p = ns) or expansions (β = .344, p = ns), was a significant unique predictor.
In the prediction of MSEL expressive language, covariates (CTW, parent MLU and home language) entered at Step 1 again contributed significantly to the model, explaining 61.1% of the variance, with both CTW (β = .514, p = .001) and MLU (β = .370, p = .020) showing unique contributions. The addition of discrete responsiveness behaviours (here, imitations, expansions and yes/no questions) at Step 2 explained a further (but non-significant) 4.5% of the variance in MSEL expressive language scores, with covariates CTW (β = .413, p = .023) and parent MLU (β = .349, p = .032) as significant, unique predictors in the final model; R2 = .68, F(6, 26) = 9.38, p < .001. As proportionate responsiveness was not found to be associated with MSEL expressive language in preliminary correlation analyses, this model was not tested.

Predictors of Parent-Reported Child Communication Skills

In the prediction of VABS receptive communication, covariates, CTW, parent MLU and home language entered at Step 1 explained 27.2% of the variance, with only MLU conferring a significant unique contribution (β = .398, p = .045). The addition of discrete responsiveness behaviours (here, imitations, expansions and WH questions) at Step 2 explained a further 25.4% of the variance in VABS receptive communication, with imitations (β = .406, p = .007), but not expansions (β = .191, p = ns) or WH questions (β = .137, p = ns), emerging as a significant unique predictor, along with one covariate, parent MLU (β = .404, p = .021); R2 = .52, F(6, 34) = 6.13, p < .001. No model was tested with proportionate parent responsiveness entered at Step 2.
In the prediction of VABS-II expressive communication, the combination of covariates, CTW, parent MLU and home language, entered at Step 1 explained 57.1% of the variance, with CTW (β = .418, p = .005) and MLU (β = .425, p = .006) making a significant unique contribution. The addition of proportionate responsiveness at Step 2 explained a further, non-significant, 3.1% of the variance in VABS-II expressive communication, with both covariates remaining as significant, unique predictors with similar coefficients; R2 = .60, F(4, 36) = 13.65, p < .001. In the alternative model with the addition of discrete behaviours (here, imitations, expansions and WH questions) at Step 2, these explained a further significant 18.8% of the variance in VABS-II expressive communication scores; R2 = .76, F(6, 34) = 17.92, p < .001. Here, both imitations (β = .225, p = .031) and expansions (β = .335, p = .036) carried significant unique predictive value, along with one covariate, parent MLU (β = .394, p = .002).

Predictors of Parent-Reported Vocabulary Knowledge

In the prediction of MCDI receptive vocabulary, the combination of covariates (CTW, parent MLU and home language) entered at Step 1 explained 65.5% of the variance, with CTW (β = .425, p = .004) and MLU (β = .469, p = .002) making a significant unique contribution. The addition of discrete responsiveness behaviours (here, imitations and expansions) at Step 2 explained a further, significant 8.1% of the variance in MCDI receptive vocabulary language, although only imitations was a significant unique predictor (β = .281, p = .016), alongside two covariates, parent MLU and CTW; R2 = .74, F(5, 32) = 17.84, p < .001. No model was tested with proportionate parent responsiveness entered at Step 2.
Finally, in the prediction of MCDI expressive vocabulary, the combination of covariates (CTW, parent MLU and home language) entered at Step 1 explained 66.0% of the variance, with CTW (β = .555, p < .001) and MLU (β = .346, p = .019) making a significant unique contribution. The addition of discrete responsiveness behaviours (here, imitations, expansions and WH questions) at Step 2 explained a further 7.6% of the variance in scores, although again, only imitations (β = .237, p = .042) carried significant unique predictive value, alongside two covariates, parent MLU and CTW; R2 = .74, F(6, 31) = 14.42, p < .001. No model was tested with proportionate parent responsiveness entered at Step 2.

Discussion

This study took a more nuanced approach to exploring the contribution of parent responsiveness to child language outcomes compared to previous work in the autism literature. Building upon previous research, we considered the comparative contribution of six discrete responsive behaviours (expansions, imitations, WH questions, yes/no questions, comments and acknowledgements) and parents’ relative responsiveness to later child language in a diverse sample of parents and their autistic preschoolers. As hypothesised, relative responsiveness was less predictive of later language skills than discrete responsive behaviours, predicting only one language outcome (i.e., directly-assessed receptive language). Also, as hypothesised, expansions—but especially imitations—of child language were valuable for later language. Whereas responsive questions, comments and acknowledgments did not predict any language outcomes, imitations contributed consistently to parent-report measures of both expressive and receptive language. Expansions predicted parent-reported expressive language skills. This study emphasises the need to capture what it is about parent language (i.e., discrete aspects) that influences child language acquisition and adds to the evidence that, alongside providing quality language input, the simple act of verbal imitation might be particularly important for autistic children’s language development.

Relative Responsiveness and MLU

Relative parent responsiveness (i.e., responsive utterances divided by all parent utterances) only predicted later directly-assessed child receptive language skills; it was unrelated to the remaining five language outcomes. This finding adds to the ambiguity reported in other studies exploring summed responsive parent behaviours (both proportion and frequency) in relation to language skills in young autistic children (Choi et al., 2020; Flippin & Watson, 2015). Moreover, our findings suggest that whilst overall parent responsiveness plays a role in later language—in this instance understanding of language—by not exploring constituent parts of responsiveness, we are missing potential important associations, such as those discussed below.
Although we used parent MLU as a covariate here it should be noted that of all variables entered into regression models (including early child language), MLU remained the most consistent predictor (five of six) of child language outcomes in final regression models. Our findings support those of Choi et al. (2020) regarding the importance of parent MLU for later language abilities of autistic children. Our findings also emphasise that, in order to understand the relative importance of different aspects of the language learning environment more fully for autistic children, we must capture both what parents say to their young autistic children as well as how they say it.

Imitations and Expansions

After controlling for earlier child words and parent MLU, we found that expansions of early language only predicted parent-report expressive communication in our sample, not parent-report vocabulary or directly-assessed receptive and expressive language. Findings both support and diverge from past literature. For example, parent expansions of child utterances did predict spoken vocabulary (measured the same way as in our study) in a small sample of minimally verbal autistic children (< 10 spontaneous words; McDuffie & Yoder, 2010). Parent MLU was not controlled for by McDuffie and Yoder (2010) which may contribute to discrepant findings—especially since we found it so consistently related to later child language here. Yet, Naigles (2013) did control for maternal MLU in their smaller, but more verbal sample, and found expansions positively related to subsequent receptive language scores. Expansions have likewise been strongly predictive of later language in non-autistic samples (Levickis et al., 2014; Taumoepeau, 2016).
Our sample was heterogenous language-wise, with baseline expressive MCDI vocabulary scores ranging from 0 to 630. Since past research has found divergent associations based on baseline language levels of children (Haebig et al., 2013), our equivocal findings may have been influenced by our language diverse sample. If we had the sample size to compare minimally verbal vs. more verbal children, for example, our patterns of results may have been clearer. Moreover, expansions were strongly correlated with our covariate child total words in the present sample (r = .798; note however that the assumption of multicollinearity for the regression analysis was not violated), which often carried significant unique predictive value at Step 2 in the regression models. This association is unsurprising since parents require child language to expand upon. However, it is plausible that this association limited our capacity to observe a unique statistical contribution of expansions to our outcomes. So, while it appears that expansions play a role in early language skills of autistic children, larger samples that permit sample stratification to identify contingent trends is required.
Parent imitations of verbal and non-verbal utterances predicted four of our child outcomes, namely parent-report expressive and receptive communication (i.e., per the VABS-II) and expressive and receptive vocabulary (i.e., per the MCDI). It was interesting that discrete responsive behaviours were predictive of parental report, but not directly assessed child language. Perhaps parent-report measures capture more nuance in what the child is capable of across different and familiar contexts over a short follow-up period in a way that direct assessments do not. Imitations of early words and sounds (measured in the same way as in the current study) have likewise been predictive of concurrent and subsequent language in non-autistic infants (Smith et al., 2018a, 2018b). Interestingly, however, McDuffie and Yoder (2010) found imitations of language were not uniquely predictive of spoken vocabulary (after controlling for child verbal utterances) in their sample of minimally verbal autistic children.
It was unclear whether McDuffie and Yoder (2010) measured imitations of both linguistic and pre-linguistic sounds. In our study, 19 children (38.8%) used < 10 spontaneous words in the parent–child interaction, but only three children (6.1%) had < 10 utterances of any type (i.e., whether comprising words or non-word vocalisations). Capturing whether a parent echoes linguistic and pre-linguistic sounds (as we did here) may be especially important in understanding the developmental trajectories of minimally verbal autistic children, who may struggle with communicative intent (i.e., when the child becomes aware of their role as a communicator able to send purposeful signals to a partner; Rowland & Fried-Oken, 2010). Imitations provide the child with immediate feedback that their verbal sounds have meaning to another person so may foster intentional communication (Bruner, 1975). Imitations likewise shape and strengthen early words by promptly reinforcing the adult language target (Tamis-LeMonda et al., 2001).

Other Responsive Behaviours

It was surprising not to find any predictive value of responsive comments on child language given that this has been found by others, including in minimally verbal autistic children (Haebig et al., 2013; McDuffie & Yoder, 2010). Yet, there is inconsistency in the extant literature here. Whilst responsive comments were not predictive of later language for more verbally fluent (described as ≥ 5 words or phrases) young autistic children (Haebig et al., 2013), simple labelling has actually been found to be negatively associated with later language in a group of non-autistic, slow-to-talk toddlers (Levickis et al., 2014). We similarly found no associations between either type of responsive questions and later language outcomes. In non-autistic samples, parents’ use of responsive open-ended questions at 12 months did not predict later language (Smith et al., 2019), but when used at 24 months they did (Levickis et al., 2014); yet, simpler responsive yes/no questions used at 12 months were predictive of later language (Smith et al., 2019).
Regarding the aforementioned negative association between simple labelling and later language, Levickis et al. (2014) suggested that labelling did not provide the rich language input needed to promote language acquisition over the toddlerhood period. Similarly, different question types may likewise be more appropriate for specific periods of linguistic development. So, whilst responsiveness appears helpful for language acquisition, potentially specific behaviours still need to be tailored to the child’s individual level to effectively scaffold language learning (i.e., working in the Zone of Proximal Development; Vygotsky, 1962). Parents need to be aware of their child’s individual level of language proficiency to understand the input required to move them to the next linguistic level. Due to the heterogeneity of our sample, some autistic children may have benefitted from responsive commenting; others from the encouragement of child participation, choice-making and problem-solving afforded by question-asking (Taylor et al., 2009). What appears to be clear is that—for our heterogenous group of autistic children—parent responsive behaviours that explicitly met children at their language level (i.e., imitations and expansions), whatever their level, appeared important for all children.

Limitations and Future Directions

Given our sample size, we were not able to explore the types of parent responsiveness which might be most critical for shaping early language acquisition in autistic children with more versus fewer baseline language abilities. Our sample size likewise precluded us from investigating whether findings differed depending on whether families were from Culturally and Linguistically Diverse (CaLD) versus non-CaLD backgrounds too. However, since most research, including autism research, is conducted with white, middle class cohorts (Rad et al., 2018), our diverse sample is a strength here. Further, our loss of data due to COVID-19 restrictions likely reduced our ability to detect differences in our analyses of directly-assessed child language. Finally, parent responsiveness was assessed based on a one-off, 10-min free play interaction between one parent and their child, using a standard set of materials. While these materials were selected to elicit a variety of interactions/play scenarios, and therefore capture a range of responsive behaviours, it is possible that capturing parent–child interactions across a variety of contexts might have yielded different results, especially when comparing more goal-orientated tasks (i.e., dressing, bath time, mealtimes) to free-play activities.
Moving forward, having standardised approaches to data collection in this field, for example creating a consistent approach to capture and assess parent responsiveness, could begin to address the challenges of comparing data across traditionally small samples in community-based research settings. Enhanced interoperability across data sets might address these limitations and our capacity to answer specific research questions. To explore generalisability of our findings, future research should also study different play scenarios, especially during more routine, naturalistic contexts across different cultural groups.

Conclusions

Focusing on discrete behaviours may further our understanding of specific mechanisms underlying how autistic children acquire language in the context of everyday interactions with their regular social partners. Generalised responsiveness is encouraged as a way to promote broad social-communication skill acquisition in child-centered parent programmes for autistic children (i.e., Green et al., 2010) and infants at higher likelihood of developing autism (i.e., Whitehouse et al., 2019, 2021). Discrete parent responsive behaviours are also typically encouraged as a way to promote specific language development for autistic children (see Bruno et al., 2016 for more information). Our findings may be valuable for indicating the relative importance of different, teachable responsive behaviours that parents can readily incorporate within everyday routines to support language outcomes for their young autistic children, including the simple act of repeating back and building upon autistic children’s early words and sounds, whether or not these are yet used intentionally.

Acknowledgments

Thanks to families for their participation in this study. Thanks also to Zoe Lazaridis, Daniel Berends, Megan Clark, Megan Grant, Katherine Natoli, Erin O’Connor, Rachael Rankin, Veronica Rose and Victorian ASELCC staff who supported with data collection, technical support and/or recruitment.

Declarations

Conflict of interest

The authors have no relevant financial or non-financial interest to disclose.

Ethical Approval

Appropriate ethical approval was obtained for this study (La Trobe University Human Research Ethics Committee; HREC #16-136).
Written informed consent was gained from all participants.
Open AccessThis article is licensed under a Creative Commons Attribution 4.0 International License, which permits use, sharing, adaptation, distribution and reproduction in any medium or format, as long as you give appropriate credit to the original author(s) and the source, provide a link to the Creative Commons licence, and indicate if changes were made. The images or other third party material in this article are included in the article's Creative Commons licence, unless indicated otherwise in a credit line to the material. If material is not included in the article's Creative Commons licence and your intended use is not permitted by statutory regulation or exceeds the permitted use, you will need to obtain permission directly from the copyright holder. To view a copy of this licence, visit http://​creativecommons.​org/​licenses/​by/​4.​0/​.

Publisher's Note

Springer Nature remains neutral with regard to jurisdictional claims in published maps and institutional affiliations.

Onze productaanbevelingen

BSL Psychologie Totaal

Met BSL Psychologie Totaal blijf je als professional steeds op de hoogte van de nieuwste ontwikkelingen binnen jouw vak. Met het online abonnement heb je toegang tot een groot aantal boeken, protocollen, vaktijdschriften en e-learnings op het gebied van psychologie en psychiatrie. Zo kun je op je gemak en wanneer het jou het beste uitkomt verdiepen in jouw vakgebied.

BSL Academy Accare GGZ collective

Literatuur
go back to reference Bruner, J. (1975). The ontogenesis of speech acts. Journal of Child Language, 2, 1–19.CrossRef Bruner, J. (1975). The ontogenesis of speech acts. Journal of Child Language, 2, 1–19.CrossRef
go back to reference Bruno, K. A., Gulati, K. L., & Mody, M. (2016). Speech and language assessment and treatment for autism spectrum disorder. In C. J. McDougle (Ed.), Autism Spectrum Disorder (pp. 321–338). Oxford University Press.CrossRef Bruno, K. A., Gulati, K. L., & Mody, M. (2016). Speech and language assessment and treatment for autism spectrum disorder. In C. J. McDougle (Ed.), Autism Spectrum Disorder (pp. 321–338). Oxford University Press.CrossRef
go back to reference Fenson, L., Marchman, V. A., Thal, D. J., Dale, P. S., Reznick, J. S., & Bates, E. (2007). MacArthur-bates communicative development inventories (2nd ed.). Brookes Publishing Co. Fenson, L., Marchman, V. A., Thal, D. J., Dale, P. S., Reznick, J. S., & Bates, E. (2007). MacArthur-bates communicative development inventories (2nd ed.). Brookes Publishing Co.
go back to reference Green, J., Charman, T., McConachie, H., Aldred, C., Slonims, V., Howlin, P., Le Couteur, A., Leadbitter, K., Hudry, K., Byford, S., Barrett, B., Temple, K., Macdonald, W., & Pickles, A. (2010). Parent-mediated communication-focused treatment in children with autism (PACT): A randomised controlled trial. The Lancet, 375(9732), 2152–2160. https://doi.org/10.1016/s0140-6736(10)60587-9CrossRef Green, J., Charman, T., McConachie, H., Aldred, C., Slonims, V., Howlin, P., Le Couteur, A., Leadbitter, K., Hudry, K., Byford, S., Barrett, B., Temple, K., Macdonald, W., & Pickles, A. (2010). Parent-mediated communication-focused treatment in children with autism (PACT): A randomised controlled trial. The Lancet, 375(9732), 2152–2160. https://​doi.​org/​10.​1016/​s0140-6736(10)60587-9CrossRef
go back to reference Howlin, P., Mawhood, L., & Rutter, M. (2000). Autism and developmental receptive language disorder—A follow-up comparison in early adult life. II: Social, behavioural, and psychiatric outcomes. Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry, 41(5), 561–578.CrossRefPubMed Howlin, P., Mawhood, L., & Rutter, M. (2000). Autism and developmental receptive language disorder—A follow-up comparison in early adult life. II: Social, behavioural, and psychiatric outcomes. Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry, 41(5), 561–578.CrossRefPubMed
go back to reference Lord, C., Rutter, M., DiLavore, P. C., Risi, S., Gotham, K., & Bishop, S. (2012). Autism diagnostic observation schedule (2nd ed.). Western Psychological Services. Lord, C., Rutter, M., DiLavore, P. C., Risi, S., Gotham, K., & Bishop, S. (2012). Autism diagnostic observation schedule (2nd ed.). Western Psychological Services.
go back to reference McDuffie, A. S., & Yoder, P. (2010). Types of parent verbal responsiveness that predict language in young children with autism spectrum disorder. Journal of Speech, Language and Hearing Research, 53(4), 1026–1039.CrossRef McDuffie, A. S., & Yoder, P. (2010). Types of parent verbal responsiveness that predict language in young children with autism spectrum disorder. Journal of Speech, Language and Hearing Research, 53(4), 1026–1039.CrossRef
go back to reference Miller, J., Andriacchi, K., & Nockerts, A. (2011). Assessing language production using SALT software: A clinician’s guide to language sample analysis. SALT Software LLC. Miller, J., Andriacchi, K., & Nockerts, A. (2011). Assessing language production using SALT software: A clinician’s guide to language sample analysis. SALT Software LLC.
go back to reference Mullen, E. M. (1995). Mullen scales of early learning (AGS). American Guidance Service Inc. Mullen, E. M. (1995). Mullen scales of early learning (AGS). American Guidance Service Inc.
go back to reference Rowland, C., & Fried-Oken, M. (2010). Communication Matrix: A clinical and research assessment tool targeting children with severe communication disorders. Journal of Paediatric Rehabilitation Medicine: An Interdisciplinary Approach, 3, 319–329.CrossRef Rowland, C., & Fried-Oken, M. (2010). Communication Matrix: A clinical and research assessment tool targeting children with severe communication disorders. Journal of Paediatric Rehabilitation Medicine: An Interdisciplinary Approach, 3, 319–329.CrossRef
go back to reference Siller, M., & Sigman, M. (2002). The behaviors of parents of children with autism predict the subsequent development of their children’s communication. Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders, 32(2), 77–89.CrossRefPubMed Siller, M., & Sigman, M. (2002). The behaviors of parents of children with autism predict the subsequent development of their children’s communication. Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders, 32(2), 77–89.CrossRefPubMed
go back to reference Smith, J., Levickis, P., Eadie, P., Bretherton, L., Conway, L., & Goldfeld, S. (2019). Associations between early maternal behaviours and child language at 36 months in a cohort experiencing adversity. International Journal of Language & Communication Disorders, 54(1), 110–122. https://doi.org/10.1111/1460-6984.12435CrossRef Smith, J., Levickis, P., Eadie, P., Bretherton, L., Conway, L., & Goldfeld, S. (2019). Associations between early maternal behaviours and child language at 36 months in a cohort experiencing adversity. International Journal of Language & Communication Disorders, 54(1), 110–122. https://​doi.​org/​10.​1111/​1460-6984.​12435CrossRef
go back to reference Sparrow, S., Cicchetti, D., & Balla, D. (2005). The Vineland adaptive behavior scales, 2nd edition (VABS-II). NCS Pearson Inc. Sparrow, S., Cicchetti, D., & Balla, D. (2005). The Vineland adaptive behavior scales, 2nd edition (VABS-II). NCS Pearson Inc.
go back to reference Swanson, M. R., Donovan, K., Paterson, S., Wolff, J. J., Parish-Morris, J., Meera, S. S., Watson, L. R., Estes, A. M., Marrus, N., Elison, J. T., Shen, M. D., McNeilly, H. B., MacIntyre, L., Zwaigenbaum, L., St. John, T., Botteron, K., Dager, S., & Piven, J. (2019). Early language exposure supports later language skills in infants with and without autism. Autism Research. https://doi.org/10.1002/aur.2163CrossRefPubMedPubMedCentral Swanson, M. R., Donovan, K., Paterson, S., Wolff, J. J., Parish-Morris, J., Meera, S. S., Watson, L. R., Estes, A. M., Marrus, N., Elison, J. T., Shen, M. D., McNeilly, H. B., MacIntyre, L., Zwaigenbaum, L., St. John, T., Botteron, K., Dager, S., & Piven, J. (2019). Early language exposure supports later language skills in infants with and without autism. Autism Research. https://​doi.​org/​10.​1002/​aur.​2163CrossRefPubMedPubMedCentral
go back to reference Tamis-LeMonda, C. S., Bornstein, M. H., & Baumwell, L. (2001). Maternal responsiveness and children’s achievement of language milestones. Child Development, 72(3), 748–767.CrossRefPubMed Tamis-LeMonda, C. S., Bornstein, M. H., & Baumwell, L. (2001). Maternal responsiveness and children’s achievement of language milestones. Child Development, 72(3), 748–767.CrossRefPubMed
go back to reference Taylor, N., Donovan, W., Miles, S., & Leavitt, L. (2009). Maternal control strategies, maternal language usage and children’s language usage at two years. Journal of Child Language, 36(2), 381–404.CrossRefPubMed Taylor, N., Donovan, W., Miles, S., & Leavitt, L. (2009). Maternal control strategies, maternal language usage and children’s language usage at two years. Journal of Child Language, 36(2), 381–404.CrossRefPubMed
go back to reference Whitehouse, A., Varcin, K. J., Alvares, G. A., Barbaro, J., Bent, C., Boutrus, M., Chetcuti, L., Cooper, M. N., Clark, A., Davidson, E., Dimov, S., Dissanayake, C., Doyle, J., Grant, M., Iacono, T., Maybery, M., Pillar, S., Renton, M., Rowbottam, C., … Hudry, K. (2019). Pre-emptive intervention versus treatment as usual for infants showing early behavioural risk signs of autism spectrum disorder: A single-blind, randomised controlled trial. The Lancet Child & Adolescent Health, 3(9), 605–615. https://doi.org/10.1016/s2352-4642(19)30184-1CrossRef Whitehouse, A., Varcin, K. J., Alvares, G. A., Barbaro, J., Bent, C., Boutrus, M., Chetcuti, L., Cooper, M. N., Clark, A., Davidson, E., Dimov, S., Dissanayake, C., Doyle, J., Grant, M., Iacono, T., Maybery, M., Pillar, S., Renton, M., Rowbottam, C., … Hudry, K. (2019). Pre-emptive intervention versus treatment as usual for infants showing early behavioural risk signs of autism spectrum disorder: A single-blind, randomised controlled trial. The Lancet Child & Adolescent Health, 3(9), 605–615. https://​doi.​org/​10.​1016/​s2352-4642(19)30184-1CrossRef
go back to reference Whitehouse, A., Varcin, K. J., Pillar, S., Billingham, W., Alvares, G. A., Barbaro, J., Bent, C. A., Blenkley, D., Boutrus, M., Chee, A., Chetcuti, L., Clark, A., Davidson, E., Dimov, S., Dissanayake, C., Doyle, J., Grant, M., Green, C. C., Harrap, M., … Hudry, K. (2021). Effect of preemptive intervention on developmental outcomes among infants showing early signs of autism: A randomized clinical trial of outcomes to diagnosis. JAMA Pediatrics, 175(11), e213298. https://doi.org/10.1001/jamapediatrics.2021.3298CrossRefPubMedPubMedCentral Whitehouse, A., Varcin, K. J., Pillar, S., Billingham, W., Alvares, G. A., Barbaro, J., Bent, C. A., Blenkley, D., Boutrus, M., Chee, A., Chetcuti, L., Clark, A., Davidson, E., Dimov, S., Dissanayake, C., Doyle, J., Grant, M., Green, C. C., Harrap, M., … Hudry, K. (2021). Effect of preemptive intervention on developmental outcomes among infants showing early signs of autism: A randomized clinical trial of outcomes to diagnosis. JAMA Pediatrics, 175(11), e213298. https://​doi.​org/​10.​1001/​jamapediatrics.​2021.​3298CrossRefPubMedPubMedCentral
Metagegevens
Titel
Parental Imitations and Expansions of Child Language Predict Later Language Outcomes of Autistic Preschoolers
Auteurs
Jodie Smith
Rhylee Sulek
Kailia Van Der Wert
Olivia Cincotta-Lee
Cherie C. Green
Catherine A. Bent
Lacey Chetcuti
Kristelle Hudry
Publicatiedatum
17-08-2022
Uitgeverij
Springer US
Gepubliceerd in
Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders / Uitgave 11/2023
Print ISSN: 0162-3257
Elektronisch ISSN: 1573-3432
DOI
https://doi.org/10.1007/s10803-022-05706-9

Andere artikelen Uitgave 11/2023

Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders 11/2023 Naar de uitgave

S:I: .Developmental Approach and Targeted Treatment of Sensory Alterations

A Randomized Controlled Trial for Audiovisual Multisensory Perception in Autistic Youth