Skip to main content
Top
Gepubliceerd in: Psychological Research 4/2023

Open Access 22-07-2022 | Original Article

On the emergence of the in–out effect across trials: two items do the trick

Auteurs: Sascha Topolinski, Lea Boecker, Charlotte S. Löffler, Beatriz Gusmão, Moritz Ingendahl

Gepubliceerd in: Psychological Research | Uitgave 4/2023

Abstract

Individuals prefer letter strings whose consonantal articulation spots move from the front of the mouth to the back (e.g., BAKA, inward) over those with a reversed consonant order (e.g., KABA, outward), the so-called in–out effect. The present research explores whether individuals hold an internal standard or scheme of consonant order that triggers this effect. If this were the case, the in–out effect should already occur in one-trial between-subjects designs. If not, the in–out effect should emerge over the course of trials in within-subjects designs. In Experiments 1a–e (1b–e preregistered; total N = 2973; German, English, and Portuguese samples) employing a one-trial between-subjects design, no in–out effect was found. In Experiment 2 (N = 253), employing within-subjects designs with either 1, 5, 10, 30, or 50 trials per consonant order category (inward vs. outward), the in–out effect was absent in the first trial, but already surfaced for the first 2 trials, reached significance within the first 10 trials and a solid plateau within the first 20 trials. Of the four theoretical explanations, the present evidence favors the fluency/frequency and letter-position accounts and is at odds with the eating-related embodiment and easy-first accounts.
Opmerkingen

Publisher's Note

Springer Nature remains neutral with regard to jurisdictional claims in published maps and institutional affiliations.

On the emergence of the in–out effect across trials: two items do the trick

Consonants are produced at different spots in the mouth, varying from the front of the mouth to the back (Titze, 1994). People prefer letter strings in which the articulation spots of the consonants move from the front of the mouth to the back (e.g., PAK, first lips then back tongue; inward) over letter strings in which the consonantal articulation spots move from the back of the mouth to the front (e.g., KAP, first back tongue then lips; outward). This so-called in–out effect (Topolinski et al., 2014) biases a wide range of judgments and even behavior, such as basic word liking (Topolinski et al., 2014), memory for words (Lindau & Topolinski, 2018b), attitudes towards persons bearing inward or outward strings as names (Garrido et al., 2019; Silva & Topolinski, 2018), consumer attitudes towards products bearing in/out strings as brands (Godinho & Garrido, ; Ingendahl & Vogel, 2022a; Pathak et al., 2021; Rossi et al., 2017; Topolinski, 2017a, 2017b; Topolinski & Boecker, 2016b; Topolinski et al., 2015), and even food consumption (Rossi et al., 2017).
Thus far, the in–out effect has been documented in German, English, French, Portuguese, Ukrainian, Turkish, and Japanese native speakers (Godinho & Garrido, 2016; Godinho et al., 2019a, 2019b; Körner & Rummer, 2021; Motoki & Pathak, 2022; Rossi et al., 2017; Topolinski et al., 2014) and occurs for even highly complicated string structures (e.g., FOLOKOLOF vs. KOLOFOLOK, Topolinski & Bakhtiari, 2016) and under sub-optimal presentation conditions, such as very brief exposure (Gerten & Topolinski, 2020), aural presentation (Topolinski & Boecker, 2016a), and even concurrent oral interference (Lindau & Topolinski, 2018a).
Its driving mechanisms are hotly debated (for a succinct review, see Ingendahl et al., 2022a). The present paper contributes to this debate by exploring whether the in–out effect is based on an internal standard of consonant order. To investigate this, we exploit the difference in judgmental formation in between-subject and within-subject designs. In this way, we can test different mechanisms that have been put forward to explain the in–out effect.

Judgment formation in between- and within-subject designs

In a between-subjects design (with a single trial being the most extreme case), individuals are exposed to only one manifestation of the experimental conditions or a single stimulus (cf., Keren, 2014). In such designs, individuals have only the stimulus features and their accompanying processing dynamics to inform their judgment. These cues must deviate strongly enough from some internal standard participants maintain in their mind to affect judgments (Kahneman & Miller, 1986; Macedo & Cardoso, 2019; Mussweiler, 2003). Also, this standard itself must be strong enough to trigger signals when being violated (e.g., Bless & Burger, 2016; Gerten & Topolinski, 2019; Rumelhart & Ortony, 1977). In a within-subjects design, however, individuals are exposed to several manifestations of the experimental conditions and several stimuli (cf., Keren, 2014). In such designs, in addition to the basic features of each given stimulus individuals have the relative differences between the various stimuli to calibrate their judgments along the trials (cf., calibration in serial evaluative judgments, Fasold et al., 2012; Unkelbach & Memmert, 2014; Unkelbach et al., 2012).
This difference between within- and between-subjects designs can be used to explore the presence and strength of an internal standard. If a certain effect occurs in a one-shot between-subjects design, there must be a strong internal standard. For instance, the word LOVE should be preferred over the word HATE already in a one-trial between-subjects design. Each of these words triggers an affective response deviating from our usual neutral affective baseline, a strong experiential internal standard. To use a psychological manipulation closer to the in–out effect, the letter string CODROT should be preferred over the letter string OTCDRO (both being anagrams of DOCTOR, Topolinski et al., 2016) also in a between-subjects design because the latter starkly defies the strong internal standards we hold about word syllable structure, vowel-consonant alternation, and thus resulting pronounceability (cf., Laham et al., 2012; Newman et al., 2014; Song & Schwarz, 2009).
In contrast, there are stimulus(-related) features for which individuals hold no absolute strong standard but rather establish the standard according to the relative level of and the differences in manifestations between the stimuli they encounter. One example that has been researched in detail in recent years is the ease with which a stimulus can be processed (processing fluency, e.g., Reber et al., 2004, Topolinski & Strack, 2009a, 2009b). Manipulations of processing fluency usually employ within-subjects designs in which the effect unfolds over the course of trials, since individuals hold no strong internal standard of absolute fluency but rather develop this standard given the stimulus ecology they encounter (see Dechêne et al., 2009; Garcia-Marques et al., 2019; Hansen et al., 2008; Wänke & Hansen, 2015; but also see exceptions regarding the truth-effect and repetition reviewed by Garcia-Marquez et al. 2019).1
From these considerations, it follows that if a certain effect occurs in a one-trial between-subjects design, the target stimulus must deviate strongly enough from a pre-existing strong internal standard or scheme. Applied to the in–out effect, if consonantal articulation direction influences liking even in a one-trial between-subjects design, individuals need to hold an internal standard of how consonantal articulation places follow each other in words. Also, this standard must be strong enough to elicit affective responses when stimuli violate it. The qualification that the deviation of the stimulus from that internal standard must also be strong enough to be effective can be ignored since there cannot be a stronger deviation from a sequence than its complete reversal (inward vs. outward).
This logic of testing the in–out effect in a one-trial between-subjects design informs our theorizing on its underlying mechanisms since the different explanations of the in–out effect put forward differ regarding the strength of such an internal standard or scheme of consonant order. Let us consult each of these explanations and their respective predictions.

The internal standard of consonant order in different theories of the in–out effect

First, it has to be emphasized that an internal standard or scheme of consonant order is not a noetic rule or conscious belief individuals are aware of (Ingendahl et al., 2022a,2022b,2022c,2022d). When being probed, participants never show insight into the in–out manipulation; when being debriefed, participants never mention consonant order as a cue they based their judgments on. It is rather a phonotactic (cf., e.g., Onishi et al., 2002) or mere motor constraint (MacDonald & Weiss, 2022) in our schemata of language representation and influences the encoding of verbal material on a very basic, fast, and automatic level. Also, the present work does not deal with the question of whether individuals become aware of the experimental manipulation in within-subjects designs or not and whether this may produce an effect (for a discussion, see Keren, 2014).
The eating-related embodied account (Godinho et al., 2019a,2019b; Maschmann et al., 2020; Topolinski & Boecker, 2016b; Topolinski et al., 2017) states that the oral motor sequences during subvocalization of consonants resemble eating (inward words) or expectoration (outward words) mouth movements that are positively or negatively associated. These overlearned, instinctive, phylogenetically primeval, and ontologically very early motor sequence representations should be a very strong internal standard. Also, they represent absolute rather than relative or graded representations: An inward motor trajectory is associated with appetitive food consumption movements, an outward trajectory is associated with aversive expectoration movements, and there is rarely a mixed or graded behavior (e.g., sucking one’s own sputum). Thus, this account would predict in–out effects for one-trial between-subjects designs, it should be only a matter of statistical power.
The processing fluency and language frequency account (Bakhtiari et al., 2016; Godinho & Garrido, 2021; Ingendahl et al., 2021, 2022a,2022b,2022c,2022d; Körner et al., 2019) states that inward relative to outward words are easier to articulate (cf., Silva et al., 2017; Song & Schwarz, 2009; Topolinski & Strack, 2010), because their consonantal sequence structure imitates real words in natural language more closely. Indeed, in corpus analyses it was found that inward consonant sequences are slightly more frequent than outward sequences in real language (Bakhtiari et al., 2016). However, since real words rarely feature pure inward or outward sequences of consonants, but rather language shows a slight bias toward inward sequences across many words at best (Bakhtiari et al., 2016), this internal language-based standard should be rather weak and subtle. Thus, a fluency/frequency account would predict the absence of an in–out effect in one-trial between-subjects designs. Rather, it would predict that the effect would unfold over the course of several trials involving both inward and outward words, which makes the deviations from that subtle internal standard salient eventually.
A more specialized version of a fluency account focusing directly on motor constraints is easy first in language acquisition (Ingendahl et al., 2022a; MacDonald & Weiss, 2022; Topolinski et al., 2022), stating that from ontogenetically early speech production on, front consonants are produced more often and are thus motorically easier to pronounce than back consonants, and individuals prefer processing easy before harder motor components. Since this well-established labial-coronal effect emerges very early in language acquisition and seems to be linguistically universal (for a recent review, see Aoyama & Davis, 2016), it should maintain a very strong internal standard during word processing. Thus, this account would predict in–out effects in one-trial between-subjects designs.
Finally, the letter-position account (Ingendahl & Vogel, 2022b; Maschmann et al., 2020) states that front consonants occur more often in the beginning than at the end of real words in natural language and readers pay more attention to word beginnings than the rest of a word (for discussions to what degree reader focus on word beginnings and endings, respectively, see Ingendahl & Vogel, 2022b; Maschmann et al., 2020). Similar to the fluency/frequency account, this would yield a rather subtle internal standard. Consequently, this account would predict in–out effects only in within-subject designs.

Present research

The aim of the present work is to shed light on the underlying mechanism of the in–out effect. Gauging in–out effects in between-subjects designs and comparing their effect size to within-subject designs yields highly informative results that do not ultimately decide between the different explanations of the in–out effect, but still provide support and counterevidence for the respective theories (cf., e.g., Fawcett, 2013, for a similar consideration to explore the production effect). While the eating-related embodiment and easy-first account would predict between-subjects in–out effects, the fluency/frequency and letter-position accounts would not.
In Experiments 1a–e, we realized between-subjects replications of earlier within-subjects designs of the in–out effect in three different languages. In Experiment 2, we tested the in–out effect for different numbers of items in a within-subjects design and gauged its development over the course of the initial trials.

A priori power-analyses, transparency, and openness

Assuming a small-to-moderate effect size of d = 0.3 (requiring N = 352 to detect a possible in–out effect with a power of 0.80), Experiment 1a was conducted with the aim to collect at least 350 participants but as many participants as possible given a certain logistic time frame (availability of the laboratory). Eventually, N = 437 participants were collected yielding d = 0.28 (Nrequired|0.80 power = 404). For the online setup in Experiment 1b a pilot study (N = 49, 29 male, 18 female, 2 diverse; mean age 10, SD = 10) was conducted with the same method as the eventual experiment. This yielded inward (n = 21) = 4.86 (SD = 2.29), outward (n = 28) = 4.14 (SD = 1.78), d = 0.36, 95% CI [− 0.22 to 0.92]. This extrapolates to Nrequired|0.80 power = 246, Nrequired|0.95 power = 404. Thus, we aimed to assure a power of 0.95 and collected at least N = 400 in Experiments 1b–e.
For the within-between interaction in Experiment 2, we powered (0.80) for a small to medium effect (f = 0.15), leading to a minimal sample size of N = 140 (28 per condition). Due to uncertainty about the actual effect size, we oversampled up to 250 participants.
We report how we determined our sample size, all data exclusions (if any), all manipulations, and all measures in the studies—and we report all studies we conducted for this setup. Data were analyzed using SPSS, Bayesian tests were conducted in JASP 0.16.1 with default settings.

Experiments 1a–e

One-trial between-subjects designs in German, English, and Portuguese

First, we explored the possibility of an in–out effect involving only one trial per participant. The only previous publication we are aware of that employed a between-subjects design of the in–out effect was Rossi et al. (2017). However, those authors used a stimulus pool of only 16 in/out letter strings and sampled 3 of them for each participant in the in- and the out-group, respectively.
We used stimulus material that has yielded reliable in–out effects in earlier within-subjects designs adapted to German (Experiments 1a, b), English (Experiment 1c), and Portuguese (Experiment 1d) phonation, respectively. Since the in–out effect is assumed to be a universal phenomenon (Topolinski et al., 2014) and has indeed been replicated in various languages, even outside the Indo-European language family and in non-WEIRD samples (Godinho et al., 2019a,2019b; Motoki & Pathak, 2022), the present test involved three different languages to attest its cross-language generalizability.
In addition, we report within-subjects replications with the respective materials used in Experiments 1a–c. To rule out that in a one-shot between-subjects design a missing in–out effect is due to the circumstance that participants were not prepared, thus inattentive, or not familiarized with the liking rating for that one stimulus, we implemented a warming-up phase with some irrelevant non-verbal filler items in Experiments 1b–d.
Moreover, in Experiment 1e we employed a manipulation of pronounceability of letter strings (cf., Laham et al., 2012; Newman et al., 2014; Song & Schwarz, 2009) in addition to in/out strings. An independent group of participants was presented with one either easy-to-pronounce letter string (e.g., BATREK) or one hard-to-pronounce letter string (e.g., EAKRTB; Silva et al., 2017; Topolinski et al., 2016; Zürn & Topolinski, 2017). Given our assumption that pronounceability has a relatively strong internal standard (see introduction), a manipulation of pronounceability should also work in a between-subjects design (note that this manipulation is usually employed within-subjects).
Experiment 1a was a laboratory study from 2017 with German participants whose demographics and raw data were lost in a rushed evacuation of the laboratory during the Covid outbreak in the spring of 2020. Still, we report this data set to avoid file drawer issues.

Methods

Samples

In Experiment 1a, N = 437 (demographics were lost in the laboratory evacuation during the Covid outbreak in spring 2020) individuals recruited from the campus of the University of Cologne, Germany, took part in an onsite laboratory experiment for candy as a reward. In Experiment 1b, N = 397 (190 female, 196 male, 11 diverse; mean age 28, SD = 8) German native speakers were recruited via Prolific Academic (https://​www.​prolific.​co) in an online setting and participated for a reward of ~ 0.35£. In Experiment 1c, N = 549 (316 female, 228 male, 2 diverse, 3 prefer not to say; mean age 43, SD = 14) English native speakers were recruited via Prolific Academic for an online survey for an 8€/h reward. In Experiment 1d, N = 490 (237 female, 248 male, 5 diverse; mean age 26, SD = 8) Portuguese native speakers were recruited via Prolific Academic for an online survey for 0.23£ reward. In Experiment 1e N = 1100 (651 female, 426 male, 21 diverse, 2 prefer not to say; mean age 38, SD = 13) English native speakers were recruited via Prolific Academic for an online survey for an 8€/h reward.

Materials

In–out stimuli

In Experiments 1a and 1b, we used the stimulus pool adjusted to German phonation provided in Topolinski and Boecker (2016a; Experiment 1), which yielded reliable effects in within-subjects implementations (e.g., Silva & Topolinski, 2018). These stimuli consist of two-syllable vocal-consonant-vocal-consonant (VCVC) letter strings each featuring a front (B, M, P) and a back consonant (G, K, R) either in the inward direction (front-back) or the outward direction (back-front). All possible combinations of the respective three front and back consonants were realized and all possible filler vowel combinations (vowels A, E, I, O, U).2 The resulting stimuli were N = 68 inward and N = 72 outward stimuli. In Experiment 1c, we used the stimulus pool adjusted to English phonation provided in Topolinski et al., (2014, Experiment 6) that used front (B, F, M, P), middle (D, L, N, S, T), and back (K) consonants and random vowels to construe VCVCVC, VCVCVCV, CVCVC, and CVCVCV letter strings. The resulting pool contained N = 125 inward and N = 157 outward stimuli. In Experiment 1d, we used the stimulus pool adjusted to Portuguese phonation provided by Godinho and Garrido (2016) involving front (P, B, F, V), middle (T, N, D), and back (C, G) consonants and random vowels in CVCVCV and VCVCVC structures. The pool contained N = 138 items of each inward and outward stimuli, respectively. In all of these pools, meaningful words were discarded, of course.

Filler stimuli for warming-up phase

In Experiments 1b-d, we used a Chinese ideograph (taken from Payne et al., 2005), a foto of a woman’s face with a neutral expression (taken from Goeleven et al., 2008), a polygon (source unknown), and a dot pattern (taken from Hansen & Topolinski, 2011).

Pronounceability stimuli

In Experiment 1e, we used the stimulus pool of easy vs. hard-to-pronounce letter strings published in Topolinski et al. (2016). These consisted of anagrams of real German words (e.g., BETRAG, amount) that were rendered to be either easy (BATREG) or hard to pronounce (EAKRTB). The stimuli ranged from 6 to 11 letters and amounted to 50 per condition. These stimuli have shown reliable and strong effects in within-subjects designs on anagram solvability ratings (Topolinski et al., 2016), the trustworthiness of eBay usernames (Silva et al., 2017), and money transfer in the trust game (Zürn & Topolinski, 2017).

Procedure

In all experiments, participants were informed that they would be presented stimuli they should evaluate for future research on a Likert scale ranging from (13) I do not like it at all to (10) I like it a lot. In the onsite laboratory Experiment 1a, they were instructed that they would be shown a single nonsense word to read and evaluate. In the online Experiments 1b-1e, they were instructed that they would be shown several stimuli (the nature of those stimuli, e.g., being either visual or verbal, was not specified). As a warming-up phase before the crucial evaluation of the in/outward target word, they evaluated the abovementioned visual filler stimuli in the fixed order they are mentioned in the materials section above (these ratings were not recorded). We implemented this warming-phase to make sure participants paid full attention to the crucial letter string trial in this online setup.
In the crucial target trial in each experiment, one random stimulus from the inward and outward pool was randomly selected per participant. In addition, in Experiment 1e a group of participants rated their liking of an easy or hard-to-pronounce letter string. The tasks took less than 5 min.
Materials and data are available online at https://​osf.​io/​a5n6t/​?​view_​only=​7306671bef694d4f​a5d69197cf698855​. The preregistrations for Experiments 1b-e can be found at https://​aspredicted.​org/​ZLL_​YXC (1b), https://​aspredicted.​org/​PTK_​2DN (1c), https://​aspredicted.​org/​N85_​6RN (1e). We have complied with APA ethical standards in the treatment of our sample.

Results

As can be seen in Table 1, the evidence for a preference of inward over outward letter strings was mixed across the different experiments. A joint ANOVA including all experiments found no in–out effect, F(1, 2412) = 0.13, p = 0.715, ηp2 < 0.01, a main effect of experiment, F(4, 2412) = 22.93, p < 0.001, ηp2 = 0.04, and no interaction, F(4, 2412) = 2.01, p = 0.091, ηp2 < 0.01. The main effect of experiment was constituted by the fact that the liking ratings were overall higher in the onsite laboratory Experiment 1a than in the other experiments. In essence, there was no difference in liking between inward (M = 3.97, SD = 1.97) and outward letter strings (M = 4.03, SD = 2.00), t(2420) = 0.72, p = 0.474, d = − 0.03, 95% CI (d) (− 0.11, 0.05), BF10 = 0.059.
Table 1
Statistics for Experiments 1a–e
Experiment
Articulation Direction
df
t
p
d
95% CI (d)
BF10
Inward
Outward
1a
4.79 (1.30) N = 217
4.44 (1.23) N = 220
435
2.90
0.004
0.28
0.09, 0.47
5.98
1b
3.91 (1.83) N = 200
3.88 (1.91) N = 197
395
0.12
0.908
0.12
− 0.19, 0.21
0.11
1c
3.89 (2.26) N = 276
4.23 (2.22) N = 273
547
− 1.78
0.076
− 0.15
− 0.32, 0.02
0.44
1d
3.33 (1.80) N = 254
3.47 (2.02) N = 236
488
− 0.79
0.431
− 0.07
− 0.25, 0.11
0.14
1e
4.03 (2.13) N = 274
4.08 (2.20) N = 275
547
− 0.02
0.814
− 0.02
− 0.19, 0.15
0.10
All means were based on a 10-point rating scale, except for Experiment 1c and 1e with an 11-point scale. Standard deviations appear in parentheses
N  number of participants, df  degrees of freedom, d   Cohen’s d, BF10  Bayes Factor for a Bayesian t test in JASP. The values for Experiment 1e only pertain to the two groups who received in/out stimuli in that experiment. The results for the groups receiving (dys)fluent stimuli are reported in the text
In contrast, in Experiment 1e, regarding the additional groups being presented with one easy- or one hard-to-pronounce letter string, participants marginally preferred easy (M = 3.54, SD = 2.14) over hard letter strings (M = 3.20, SD = 2.30), t(549) = 1.86, p = 0.063, d = 0.16, 95% CI (d) (− 0.01, 0.33). Also, participants overall preferred inward/outward (M = 4.05, SD = 2.17) over easy/hard letter strings (M = 3.37, SD = 2.14), F(1, 1096) = 27.95, p < 0.001, ηp2 = 0.03.
To attest that the stimuli used in the present between-subjects designs are effective, we report a within-subjects online study with the German stimulus pool from Experiments 1a–b on German native speakers (N = 16). This yet unpublished data set was a pilot study for the baseline of another, conceptually unrelated project (judgmental correction of the in–out effect, Topolinski & Löffler, 2022), randomly sampling 30 inward and 30 outwards words from the Topolinski and Boecker (2016a, Experiment 1) stimulus pool per participant. In this data set, participants liked inward letter strings (M = 4.64, SE = 0.27) more than outward letter strings (M = 4.30, SE = 0.24), t(15) = 3.09, p = 0.008, dz = 0.77, 95% CI (dz) (0.20–1.32).
Moreover, we report a preregistered (https://​osf.​io/​r72ed/​?​view_​only=​3c3c2dce81814b17​94fa1691ae71b4ed​) unpublished within-subjects online study with the English stimulus pool from Experiment 1c (from Topolinski et al., 2014, Experiment 6) that sampled 12 inward and 12 outward words per participant. In this data set, participants liked inward letter strings (M = 4.03, SE = 0.14) more than outward letter strings (M = 3.90, SE = 0.13), t(252) = 2.97, p = 0.003, dz = 0.19, 95% CI (dz) (0.06–0.31).

Discussion

We went to great lengths to document an in–out effect that involves only one trial, testing 2422 participants in three languages. We did not establish such evidence, although the stimuli used in the present experiments were carefully crafted according to the respective phonation rules in German, English, and Portuguese and had produced reliable in–out effects in within-subjects designs in previous publications, and proved reliable in additional within-subjects replications reported here (for Experiments 1a–c).
Furthermore, according to our knowledge being the first to implement a pronounceability manipulation in a one-shot between-subjects design, we did find a marginally significant effect (Experiment 1e). We argue that this was the case because pronounceability bears an internal standard stronger than consonant order, and participants do not need several stimuli to calibrate their liking but use their internal standard of word structure in the single trial right away. Of course, given the very low item reliability of this single measurement, the effect was very small. Also, the items used in that experiment derive from German words, and thus the psycholinguistic properties of those stimuli aside from pronounceability (e.g., letter frequencies) were unfamiliar to the English sample. This is reflected in the fact that the participants in Experiment 1e preferred in/out letter strings generally over (dis)fluent letter strings (the former being specially crafted for English phonation). In turn, the very fact that there was a solid between-subjects difference in the liking of in/out vs. (dis)fluent letter strings supports the idea that such effects are possible in one-shot between-subjects designs.
The only two differences between Experiment 1a and the other experiments, namely being an onsite laboratory experiment and doing without the warming-up evaluation phase, can hardly explain why Experiment 1a found an in–out effect and the others (online and with a warming-up phase) did not. First, the additional two studies attesting a within-subjects in–out effect were also online studies; and second, it is implausible that the presentation of four neutral stimuli in advance should block the in–out effect.

Experiment 2

Different numbers of items in a within-subjects design

We employed a within-subjects manipulation of the in–out effect with varying numbers of items per in/out category to explore (a) how the in–out effect depends on the number of items, and (b) how the in–out effect unfolds over time. In past publications, this number of items (sampled from usually much larger stimulus pools) ranged from 10 (Topolinski et al., 2014, first experiments) to up to 60 items per category per participant (Topolinski & Boecker, 2016a, 2016b, Experiment 2b). Here, we manipulated between-subjects whether 1, 5, 10, 30, or 50 items per in/out category were presented in a within-subjects design. In addition, independent of the between-subjects condition of how many items a participant received overall, we analyzed only the first 1, 2, 3, up to 10 trials in the whole data set to see how the in–out effect unfolds over time (for a similar approach focusing on the initial and last trials in a truth effect experiment, see Garcia-Marquez et al., 2019).

Methods

Sample

N = 253 (demographics were lost in the laboratory evacuation during the Covid outbreak in spring 2020) individuals were recruited from a campus of the University of Cologne, Germany, and participated in this onsite laboratory experiment for €3 as a reward (the task was always bundled with other, conceptually irrelevant tasks, see procedure section).

Materials

The 2-syllables stimulus pool for German phonation from Topolinski and Boecker (2016a; Experiment 1) was used (as in Experiments 1a, b).

Design

We employed a 2 (articulation direction: inward, outward; within-subjects) X 5 (number of items per within-condition: 1 item, 5 items, 10 items, 30 items, 50 items; between-subjects) design.

Procedure

Since the different between-subjects conditions differed in length, the present tasks were always bundled with other, conceptually irrelevant tasks (Gerten & Topolinski, 2021; Gerten et al., 2022; Zürn et al., 2021) in variable length and flexible combination to always end up with an experimental session of approx. 15–20 min. This was done to rule out that possible differences in the present conditions were due to differing participant rewards and thus motivation. In the present task, participants were informed that they would be presented with nonsense words and should report their spontaneous liking for each stimulus typing in a number ranging from (0) I do not like it at all to (10) I like it a lot. Then, participants received either 1, 5, 10, 30, or 50 inward and outward words, respectively, in random order re-randomized anew for each participant to render their liking ratings. The tasks took 1 to 7 min.
Data are available online at https://​osf.​io/​a5n6t/​?​view_​only=​7306671bef694d4f​a5d69197cf698855​. We have complied with APA ethical standards in the treatment of our sample.

Results and discussion

Mistyped responses (14 of 9670, 0.1%) were discarded (the experimental driving software DirectRT allowed not only to type in numbers but also letters). Two participants in the 1-item per category condition typed in invalid responses in both trials and were discarded. One participant from the 1-item per category condition typed in an invalid response in one trial and was thus discarded from the analyses in Table 2, but was included in the later exploratory analyses in Fig. 3 (see below).
Table 2
Statistics for the experimental between-subjects conditions in Experiment 2
Number of items per in–out condition
Articulation Direction
df
t
p
dz
95% CI (dz)
BF10
Inward
Outward
1 item (N = 47)
4.28 (0.35)
3.72 (0.28)
46
1.48
0.145
0.22
-0.07, 0.50
0.44
5 items (N = 49)
4.63 (0.24)
4.20 (0.22)
48
2.39
0.021
0.34
0.05, 0.63
2.01
10 items (N = 54)
5.18 (0.19)
4.60 (0.20)
53
4.61
 < 0.001
0.63
0.33, 0.92
770.34
30 items (N = 50)
4.60 (0.17)
4.30 (0.18)
49
5.11
 < 0.001
0.72
0.41, 1.03
 > 1000
50 items (N = 50)
4.56 (0.20)
4.15 (0.19)
49
4.76
 < 0.001
0.67
0.36, 0.96
 > 1000
All means were based on a 10-point rating scale. Standard errors appear in parentheses
N   number of participants, df  degrees of freedom, dz  Cohen’s effect size for within-subjects t tests, BF10  Bayes Factor for a Bayesian t test in JASP
First, we tested whether the number of items presented in each between-subjects condition affects the in–out effect, which should result in an interaction of articulation direction and item number. A 2 (articulation direction: inward, outward; within-subjects) X 5 (number of items per within-condition: 1 item, 5 items, 10 items, 30 items, 50 items; between-subjects) ANOVA over the aggregated liking ratings found a main effect of articulation direction, F(1, 245) = 28.07, p < 0.001, ηp2 = 0.10; a conceptually irrelevant main effect for number of items, F(4, 248) = 2.47, p = 0.045; and no interaction, F(4, 245) = 0.36, p = 0.834. Across groups, inward letter strings (M = 4.66, SE = 0.11) were liked more than outward letter strings (M = 4.20, SE = 0.10), t(249) = 5.34, p < 0.001, dz = 0.34, 95% CI (dz) (0.21–0.47).
Despite the absent interaction, we plotted the respective descriptive and t tests per between-subjects condition in Table 2 for illustrative reasons. When only 1 item per category was presented, the in–out effect emerged with an already non-neglectable but insignificant effect size of dz = 0.22, increased in effect size approaching significance (mind Bonferroni-correction for multiple testings) when 5 items per category were presented and reached a solid plateau of highly significant effects for 10 to 50 items per category. This pattern is not surprising for mere statistical but also for psychological reasons: Of course, the more items are sampled, the higher the reliability of the measurement and thus the effect size (e.g., Bellemare et al., 2014; Erlebacher, 1977; Judd et al., 2012; Lakens, 2013). However, note that in the present data there is no pattern of an ever increasing effect size with increasing number of items: with 10 items per category the effect size reaches a plateau.
The more interesting aspect for the present exploration is the immediate onset of the in–out effect already in the 1-item per category condition. Figure 1 displays the differences of the means (inward minus outward) per number-of-items condition. These measures illustrate how much the ratings deflect contingent on in/out items and thus map the sensitivity (in a psychological, not statistical meaning) to the in/out manipulation. Independent from the decreasing error margins along with the conditions (which is due to the increasing measurement reliability mentioned above), the raw mean of the difference between inward and outward trials is already relatively high in the 1-item condition and does not differ between the groups (thus the missing interaction in the ANOVA).

Analyses on a trial-level

Next, we analyzed how the in–out effect unfolds over the course of trials, irrespective of the between-subjects condition of how many items a participant received overall. For instance, for a participant both in the 1-item and 50-items per category group, the very first trial of the task works psychologically like a one-trial between-subjects design (Experiments 1a-e). Also, this analysis allows for including much more participants than the above analyses that split the sample into groups. The resulting in–out effect sizes are shown in Fig. 2.4
Perfectly mirroring the pattern from the main analysis (that, however, involved fewer participants) and the evidence from Experiments 1a-e, the first trial does not show a noteworthy in–out effect (d = 0.12). Then, already with the second trial, the effect size doubled (d = 0.27) and was, in this analysis, significant, p = 0.003. This evidence is conceptually similar to the 1-item per category (i.e., 2 items altogether) group in Table 2, which had a similar effect size but was not significant due to the smaller sample size. Finally, the in–out effect soon settles on highly significant (all ps < 0.001) effects well above d = 0.30 within the first ten trials. This evidence is conceptually similar to the 5-item per category (i.e., 10 items altogether) group in Table 2 that had a similar effect size but only marginal significance. Again, this pattern is conflated with increasing reliability due to the increasing number of items involved, however, there is no monotonous increase.
To map the sensitivity to the in–out manipulation per trial independent of item number/measurement reliability, Fig. 3 displays the differences of the means (inward minus outward) separately for each of the first ten trials (which of course can only be done via between-subjects tests). Here again, the liking ratings already show a sensitivity to the in–out manipulation starting with the second trial.

The trick of the second trial

For exploratory reasons, we analyzed the liking ratings across all participants for the first and the second trial, respectively, contingent on which in–out category they were presented in the given trial. Since in–out was randomly sampled, there were four groups: participants who received first an inward and then again an inward item, first an inward and then an outward item, first an outward and then an inward item, and first an outward and then again an outward item. The means are displayed in Fig. 4.
The interaction consonantal order (inward vs. outward) X trial number (first trial vs. second trial) was significant, F(3, 247) = 4.88, p = 0.003, ηp2 = 0.06. The likings ratings did not differ between the groups in the first trial (white bars in Fig. 4), F(3, 248) = 0.36, p = 0.784, but in the second trial (grey bars in Fig. 4), F(3, 248) = 5.45, p = 0.001, ηp2 = 0.06. For participants receiving first an inward and then again an inward item, liking ratings increased from the first to the second trial, t(42) = 2.62, p = 0.012, dz = 0.40, 95% CI (dz) (0.09, 0.80). Thus, although inward order did not affect the first liking rating, it added positive valence to the liking in the second trial. For participants receiving first an inward and then an outward item, liking ratings dropped only descriptively across trials, t(71) = 1.65, p = 0.100, dz = 0.20, 95% CI (dz) (− 0.04, 0.43). Thus, a contrastive detrimental effect of outward on liking was not observed. For participants receiving first an outward and then an inward item, liking ratings increased across trials (despite Bonferroni correction for multiple testing), t(78) = 2.65, p = 0.010, dz = 0.30, 95% CI (dz) (0.07, 0.52). Thus, a contrastive beneficial effect of inward on liking was observed. For participants receiving first an outward and then again an outward item, liking ratings did not differ across trials, t(56) = 0.65, p = 0.521, dz = 0.09, 95% CI (dz) (− 0.18, 0.35). Thus, outward consonant order did not add negative valence to the liking in the second trial. In sum, effects of the in–out manipulation were absent in the first, but emerged already in the second trial, particularly a beneficial additive and contrastive impact of inward on likings, while outward had a lesser pronounced detrimental effect on preferences.

General discussion

We explored the strength of the internal standard or schema individuals hold about consonantal order in random letter strings by comparing the in–out effect sizes in between- and within-subjects designs. If individuals carry a strong internal standard of inward and outward consonantal order and the respective affective connotations, as the eating-related embodiment (Topolinski et al., 2014) and the easy-first (MacDonald & Weiss, 2022) account suggest, then the in–out effect would already occur in a one-trial between-subjects design, similar to other effects with strong internal comparison standards. Testing over 2400 participants in three different languages, we did not find such an effect (Experiments 1a-e). This null-finding is unlikely due to lacking power since it was corroborated by Bayes analyses that attested strong evidence against H1, BF10 = 0.059. Also, as a whole, Experiments 1a-e had a post-doc power of (1−β) = 0.998 to detect a small effect size d = 0.2, and still (1−β) = 0.958 to detect an effect size of d = 0.15. In contrast, a manipulation of pronounceability of letter strings, which assumably involves a stronger internal standard of word structure, produced a marginal between-subjects effect (Experiment 1e).
In Experiment 2, employing within-subject replications of the in–out effect with varying item numbers, we again found no in–out effect for the first trial (which equals a one-trial between-subjects instantiation psychologically). However, already in the second trial, the in–out effect began to emerge with highly significant effects in the analyses involving larger samples. Particularly inward items increased liking, but outward items did not significantly decrease liking. Across the further course of the liking task, the in–out effect very soon reached a plateau of solid significant effect sizes within the first few trials (see Fig. 2).
This pattern resembles the unfolding of any fluency effect over the course of an experiment, where no absolute standards to compare the stimulus with are available, but participants develop an internal standard given the relative differences of the stimuli they are being presented with (cf., Dechêne et al., 2009; Garcia-Marques et al., 2019; Hansen et al., 2008; Unkelbach et al., 2012; Wänke & Hansen, 2015). Most importantly, this pattern would be predicted by both the fluency/frequency (e.g., Bakhtiari et al., 2016; Godinho & Garrido, 2021; Ingendahl et al., 2021, 2022a, 2022b, 2022c, 2022d; Körner et al., 2019) and the letter-position (Ingendahl & Vogel, 2022b; Maschmann et al., 2020) accounts of the in–out effect.
Yet, the present analyses also showed that the in–out effect emerges surprisingly fast, already in the second trial. Particularly the finding that a second inward item presented after an initial inward item increased likings points to the direction of a possible absolute judgmental standard instead of relative judgmental calibration. This should be scrutinized in the future with larger sample sizes.
As a limitation, it is also necessary to consider the role of measurement reliability in the present findings. It is almost trivial that the in–out effect is smaller for a single measurement than for multiple trials. Yet, a mere reliability explanation would predict a monotonously asymptotic increase of the in–out effect with an increasing number of trials (up to the “true” effect size). This is not what we observe in Experiment 2 (see Fig. 2), where the in–out effect is strongest already for the three first trials and even slightly decreases afterwards. Also, the results of Experiment 1e show that even a single measure in a pure between-design can detect systematic preferences between word stimuli—namely between the in/out and (dis)fluent letter strings.
As another limitation, the present study is not a decisive test of the competing theories about the driving mechanisms of the in–out effect, but it provides further evidence to the ongoing conceptual debate (Ingendahl et al., 2022b). It renders an eating-related embodiment (see Maschmann et al., 2020) and an easy-first account (see Topolinski et al., 2022) even more unlikely and puts the in–out effect into the conceptual vicinity of usual fluency effects and their dependence on several sequential stimulus presentations (e.g., Bakhtiari et al., 2016; Dechêne et al., 2009).
Concluding, a fluency account of the in–out effect seems a promising explanation. However, recent research has shown that central predictions of a fluency account, a part of the presence of an internal standard, could not be supported experimentally (Ingendahl et al., 2022a,2022b,2022c,2022d). For instance, in mediation analyses, the impact of consonant order on liking is only partially mediated by simultaneously assessed subjective fluency (Bakhtiari et al., 2016), and the in–out effect is not modulated by experimental inductions of fluency using training (Ingendahl et al., 2021). Thus, further research is needed to determine the actual driving force behind the in–out effect.

Implications for other psycholinguistic effects and marketing

Beyond the examination of the in–out effect, the present logic of comparing between-subjects and within-subjects designs to gauge the role of an internal judgmental standard in a certain effect can be useful in theory development for other phenomena as well. Generally, this logic can be used to test whether a certain effect is based on fluency or not (cf., Dechêne et al., 2009; Garcia-Marques et al., 2019; Hansen et al., 2008; Wänke & Hansen, 2015). Moreover, this logic can be used to test the involvement of an internal standard in any phenomenon.
As one example, regarding sound symbolism, for instance, consider the well-known bouba/kiki effect (Köhler, 1970), where a spiky visual form is associated with a sharp-edged phonetic name sound and a round form with a round sound (for recent examinations, see, e.g., Barton & Halberstadt, 2018; Cuskley et al., 2017). Usually, this effect is tested in a one-shot between-subjects design (for a recent large-scale replication across languages using such a design, see Ćwiek et al., 2022). In contrast, the sound symbolism phenomenon of size-pitch association (small objects/high front vowels, large objects/low back vowels; Sapir, 1929) is usually tested in within-subjects designs (e.g., Klink, 2000). This might imply that the bouba/kiki-effect is driven by a stronger internal judgmental standard than usual sound symbolism effects, which should be tested in future studies.
The same applies to other psycholinguistics effects that are usually tested in within-subjects designs, such as vowel-affect (Rummer et al., 2014), voicedness-gender (Slepian & Galinsky, 2016), or sound-taste associations (Motoki et al., 2020). Particularly for their feasibility as a marketing technique (Topolinski, 2017a, 2017b), effects involving an internal standard evoking an effect already in a one-shot test are more promising as marketing tool as they are likely to elicit attitudes immediately during a single exposure to a single brand name.

Declarations

Conflict of interest

We have no conflict of interest to disclose.
Open AccessThis article is licensed under a Creative Commons Attribution 4.0 International License, which permits use, sharing, adaptation, distribution and reproduction in any medium or format, as long as you give appropriate credit to the original author(s) and the source, provide a link to the Creative Commons licence, and indicate if changes were made. The images or other third party material in this article are included in the article's Creative Commons licence, unless indicated otherwise in a credit line to the material. If material is not included in the article's Creative Commons licence and your intended use is not permitted by statutory regulation or exceeds the permitted use, you will need to obtain permission directly from the copyright holder. To view a copy of this licence, visit http://​creativecommons.​org/​licenses/​by/​4.​0/​.

Publisher's Note

Springer Nature remains neutral with regard to jurisdictional claims in published maps and institutional affiliations.

Onze productaanbevelingen

BSL Psychologie Totaal

Met BSL Psychologie Totaal blijf je als professional steeds op de hoogte van de nieuwste ontwikkelingen binnen jouw vak. Met het online abonnement heb je toegang tot een groot aantal boeken, protocollen, vaktijdschriften en e-learnings op het gebied van psychologie en psychiatrie. Zo kun je op je gemak en wanneer het jou het beste uitkomt verdiepen in jouw vakgebied.

BSL Academy Accare GGZ collective

Voetnoten
1
Note that the previous example of pronounceability is usually conceptualized as an instantiation of processing fluency (e.g., Song & Schwarz, 2009). However, we argue that the internal standard of vowel-consonant alternation (resulting in pronounceability in human language) is much stronger than other stimulus features being subsumed under fluency, such as figure-ground contrast or semantic fluency of statements.
 
2
Generally, the in–out effect only concerns consonants, not vowels. Experimentally manipulating the order of front and back vowels in addition to consonant order, Topolinski & Boecker (2016a) found no vowel in–out effect, while Motoki & Pathak (2022) did.
 
3
In experiments 1c and 1e, the scale ranged from 0 to 10.
 
4
Note that the resulting participant sample sizes for particularly the first 2 and 3 trials are reduced since inward and outward words were presented in random order; and for the between-subjects conditions involving 5–50 trials it was possible that a participant received stimuli from only one category and was thus discarded from the analyses. Furthermore, from 3 trials onwards the between-subject group with only 1 item per category (i.e., 2 items overall) dropped from the analysis.
 
Literatuur
go back to reference MacDonald, M. C., & Weiss, D. J. (2022). Easy does it: sequencing explains the in-out effect. Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 26(6), 447–448.PubMedCrossRef MacDonald, M. C., & Weiss, D. J. (2022). Easy does it: sequencing explains the in-out effect. Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 26(6), 447–448.PubMedCrossRef
go back to reference Aoyama, K., & Davis, B. L. (2016). Non-adjacent consonant sequence patterns in English target words during the first-word period. Journal of Child Language, 44(5), 1065–1087.PubMedCrossRef Aoyama, K., & Davis, B. L. (2016). Non-adjacent consonant sequence patterns in English target words during the first-word period. Journal of Child Language, 44(5), 1065–1087.PubMedCrossRef
go back to reference Bakhtiari, G., Körner, A., & Topolinski, S. (2016). The role of fluency in preferences for inward over outward words. Acta Psychologica, 171, 110–117.PubMedCrossRef Bakhtiari, G., Körner, A., & Topolinski, S. (2016). The role of fluency in preferences for inward over outward words. Acta Psychologica, 171, 110–117.PubMedCrossRef
go back to reference Barton, D. N., & Halberstadt, J. (2018). A social Bouba/Kiki effect: a bias for people whose names match their faces. Psychonomic Bulletin & Review, 25(3), 1013–1020.CrossRef Barton, D. N., & Halberstadt, J. (2018). A social Bouba/Kiki effect: a bias for people whose names match their faces. Psychonomic Bulletin & Review, 25(3), 1013–1020.CrossRef
go back to reference Bellemare, C., Bissonnette, L., & Kröger, S. (2014). Statistical power of within and between-subjects designs in economic experiments. Available at SSRN 2529895. Bellemare, C., Bissonnette, L., & Kröger, S. (2014). Statistical power of within and between-subjects designs in economic experiments. Available at SSRN 2529895.
go back to reference Bless, H., & Burger, A. M. (2016). A closer look at social psychologists’ silver bullet: inevitable and evitable side effects of the experimental approach. Perspectives on Psychological Science, 11(2), 296–308.PubMedCrossRef Bless, H., & Burger, A. M. (2016). A closer look at social psychologists’ silver bullet: inevitable and evitable side effects of the experimental approach. Perspectives on Psychological Science, 11(2), 296–308.PubMedCrossRef
go back to reference Cuskley, C., Simner, J., & Kirby, S. (2017). Phonological and orthographic influences in the Bouba–Kiki effect. Psychological Research Psychologische Forschung, 81(1), 119–130.PubMedCrossRef Cuskley, C., Simner, J., & Kirby, S. (2017). Phonological and orthographic influences in the Bouba–Kiki effect. Psychological Research Psychologische Forschung, 81(1), 119–130.PubMedCrossRef
go back to reference Ćwiek, A., Fuchs, S., Draxler, C., Asu, E. L., Dediu, D., Hiovain, K., Kawahara, S., Koutalidis, S., Krifka, M., Lippus, P., Lupyan, G., Oh, G. E., Paul, J., Petrone, C., Ridouane, R., Reiter, S., Schümchen, N., Szalontai, Á., Ünal-Logacev, Ö., … Winter, B. (2022). The Bouba/Kiki effect is robust across cultures and writing systems. Philosophical Transactions of the Royal Society B, 377(1841), 20200390.CrossRef Ćwiek, A., Fuchs, S., Draxler, C., Asu, E. L., Dediu, D., Hiovain, K., Kawahara, S., Koutalidis, S., Krifka, M., Lippus, P., Lupyan, G., Oh, G. E., Paul, J., Petrone, C., Ridouane, R., Reiter, S., Schümchen, N., Szalontai, Á., Ünal-Logacev, Ö., … Winter, B. (2022). The Bouba/Kiki effect is robust across cultures and writing systems. Philosophical Transactions of the Royal Society B, 377(1841), 20200390.CrossRef
go back to reference Dechêne, A., Stahl, C., Hansen, J., & Wänke, M. (2009). Mix me a list: context moderates the truth effect and the mere-exposure effect. Journal of Experimental Social Psychology, 45(5), 1117–1122.CrossRef Dechêne, A., Stahl, C., Hansen, J., & Wänke, M. (2009). Mix me a list: context moderates the truth effect and the mere-exposure effect. Journal of Experimental Social Psychology, 45(5), 1117–1122.CrossRef
go back to reference Erlebacher, A. (1977). Design and analysis of experiments contrasting the within-and between-subjects manipulation of the independent variable. Psychological Bulletin, 84(2), 212.CrossRef Erlebacher, A. (1977). Design and analysis of experiments contrasting the within-and between-subjects manipulation of the independent variable. Psychological Bulletin, 84(2), 212.CrossRef
go back to reference Fasold, F., Memmert, D., & Unkelbach, C. (2012). Extreme judgments depend on the expectation of following judgments: a calibration analysis. Psychology of Sport and Exercise, 13(2), 197–200.CrossRef Fasold, F., Memmert, D., & Unkelbach, C. (2012). Extreme judgments depend on the expectation of following judgments: a calibration analysis. Psychology of Sport and Exercise, 13(2), 197–200.CrossRef
go back to reference Fawcett, J. M. (2013). The production effect benefits performance in between-subject designs: a meta-analysis. Acta Psychologica, 142(1), 1–5.PubMedCrossRef Fawcett, J. M. (2013). The production effect benefits performance in between-subject designs: a meta-analysis. Acta Psychologica, 142(1), 1–5.PubMedCrossRef
go back to reference Garcia-Marques, T., Silva, R. R., Mello, J., & Hansen, J. (2019). Relative to what? Dynamic updating of fluency standards and between-participants illusions of truth. Acta Psychologica, 195, 71–79.PubMedCrossRef Garcia-Marques, T., Silva, R. R., Mello, J., & Hansen, J. (2019). Relative to what? Dynamic updating of fluency standards and between-participants illusions of truth. Acta Psychologica, 195, 71–79.PubMedCrossRef
go back to reference Garrido, M. V., Godinho, S., & Semin, G. R. (2019). The “ins” and “outs” of person perception: The influence of consonant wanderings in judgments of warmth and competence. Journal of Experimental Social Psychology, 82, 1–5.CrossRef Garrido, M. V., Godinho, S., & Semin, G. R. (2019). The “ins” and “outs” of person perception: The influence of consonant wanderings in judgments of warmth and competence. Journal of Experimental Social Psychology, 82, 1–5.CrossRef
go back to reference Gerten, J., & Topolinski, S. (2019). Shades of surprise: assessing surprise as a function of degree of deviance and expectation constraints. Cognition, 192, 103986.PubMedCrossRef Gerten, J., & Topolinski, S. (2019). Shades of surprise: assessing surprise as a function of degree of deviance and expectation constraints. Cognition, 192, 103986.PubMedCrossRef
go back to reference Gerten, J., & Topolinski, S. (2020). Exploring the temporal boundary conditions of the articulatory in–out preference effect. Psychological Research Psychologische Forschung, 84(3), 558–567.PubMedCrossRef Gerten, J., & Topolinski, S. (2020). Exploring the temporal boundary conditions of the articulatory in–out preference effect. Psychological Research Psychologische Forschung, 84(3), 558–567.PubMedCrossRef
go back to reference Gerten, J., & Topolinski, S. (2021). SNARC compatibility triggers positive affect. Cognition and Emotion, 35(2), 356–366.PubMedCrossRef Gerten, J., & Topolinski, S. (2021). SNARC compatibility triggers positive affect. Cognition and Emotion, 35(2), 356–366.PubMedCrossRef
go back to reference Gerten, J., Zürn, M. K., & Topolinski, S. (2022). The price of predictability: estimating inconsistency premiums in social interactions. Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin, 48(2), 183–202.PubMedCrossRef Gerten, J., Zürn, M. K., & Topolinski, S. (2022). The price of predictability: estimating inconsistency premiums in social interactions. Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin, 48(2), 183–202.PubMedCrossRef
go back to reference Godinho, S., & Garrido, M. V. (2016). Oral approach-avoidance: a replication and extension for European-Portuguese phonation. European Journal of Social Psychology, 46(2), 260–264.CrossRef Godinho, S., & Garrido, M. V. (2016). Oral approach-avoidance: a replication and extension for European-Portuguese phonation. European Journal of Social Psychology, 46(2), 260–264.CrossRef
go back to reference Godinho, S., & Garrido, M. V. (2017). Branding with the in–out effect: the impact of consonantal articulation on brand evaluation. Psychology & Marketing, 34(9), 904–911.CrossRef Godinho, S., & Garrido, M. V. (2017). Branding with the in–out effect: the impact of consonantal articulation on brand evaluation. Psychology & Marketing, 34(9), 904–911.CrossRef
go back to reference Godinho, S., & Garrido, M. V. (2020). The “ins” and “outs” of product and services marketing: the influence of consonant wanderings in consumer decision-making. Psychology & Marketing, 37(10), 1352–1361.CrossRef Godinho, S., & Garrido, M. V. (2020). The “ins” and “outs” of product and services marketing: the influence of consonant wanderings in consumer decision-making. Psychology & Marketing, 37(10), 1352–1361.CrossRef
go back to reference Godinho, S., & Garrido, M. V. (2021). The in–out effect: Examining the role of perceptual fluency in the preference for words with inward-wandering consonantal articulation. Psychological Research Psychologische Forschung, 85(1), 112–120.PubMedCrossRef Godinho, S., & Garrido, M. V. (2021). The in–out effect: Examining the role of perceptual fluency in the preference for words with inward-wandering consonantal articulation. Psychological Research Psychologische Forschung, 85(1), 112–120.PubMedCrossRef
go back to reference Godinho, S., Garrido, M. V., & Horchak, O. V. (2019a). Oral approach avoidance: a replication and extension for Slavic and Turkic phonations. Experimental Psychology, 66(5), 355–360.PubMedCrossRef Godinho, S., Garrido, M. V., & Horchak, O. V. (2019a). Oral approach avoidance: a replication and extension for Slavic and Turkic phonations. Experimental Psychology, 66(5), 355–360.PubMedCrossRef
go back to reference Godinho, S., Garrido, M. V., Zürn, M., & Topolinski, S. (2019b). Oral kinematics: examining the role of edibility and valence in the in-out effect. Cognition and Emotion, 33(5), 1094–1098.PubMedCrossRef Godinho, S., Garrido, M. V., Zürn, M., & Topolinski, S. (2019b). Oral kinematics: examining the role of edibility and valence in the in-out effect. Cognition and Emotion, 33(5), 1094–1098.PubMedCrossRef
go back to reference Goeleven, E., De Raedt, R., Leyman, L., & Verschuere, B. (2008). The Karolinska directed emotional faces: a validation study. Cognition and Emotion, 22(6), 1094–1118.CrossRef Goeleven, E., De Raedt, R., Leyman, L., & Verschuere, B. (2008). The Karolinska directed emotional faces: a validation study. Cognition and Emotion, 22(6), 1094–1118.CrossRef
go back to reference Hansen, J., Dechene, A., & Wänke, M. (2008). Discrepant fluency increases subjective truth. Journal of Experimental Social Psychology, 44(3), 687–691.CrossRef Hansen, J., Dechene, A., & Wänke, M. (2008). Discrepant fluency increases subjective truth. Journal of Experimental Social Psychology, 44(3), 687–691.CrossRef
go back to reference Hansen, J., & Topolinski, S. (2011). An exploratory mindset reduces preference for prototypes and increases preference for novel exemplars. Cognition and Emotion, 25(4), 709–716.PubMedCrossRef Hansen, J., & Topolinski, S. (2011). An exploratory mindset reduces preference for prototypes and increases preference for novel exemplars. Cognition and Emotion, 25(4), 709–716.PubMedCrossRef
go back to reference Ingendahl, M., Maschmann, I., Embs, N., Maulbetsch, A., Vogel, T., & Wänke, M. (2022a). Do we know what’s in and what’s out? Submitted for publication. Ingendahl, M., Maschmann, I., Embs, N., Maulbetsch, A., Vogel, T., & Wänke, M. (2022a). Do we know what’s in and what’s out? Submitted for publication.
go back to reference Ingendahl, M., Schöne, T., Wänke, M., & Vogel, T. (2021). Fluency in the in-out effect: the role of structural mere exposure effects. Journal of Experimental Social Psychology, 92, 104079.CrossRef Ingendahl, M., Schöne, T., Wänke, M., & Vogel, T. (2021). Fluency in the in-out effect: the role of structural mere exposure effects. Journal of Experimental Social Psychology, 92, 104079.CrossRef
go back to reference Ingendahl, M., & Vogel, T. (2022a). Choosing a brand name that’s “in”–disgust sensitivity, preference for intuition, and the articulatory in-out effect. Personality and Individual Differences, 185, 111276.CrossRef Ingendahl, M., & Vogel, T. (2022a). Choosing a brand name that’s “in”–disgust sensitivity, preference for intuition, and the articulatory in-out effect. Personality and Individual Differences, 185, 111276.CrossRef
go back to reference Ingendahl, M., & Vogel, T. (2022b). The articulatory in-out effect: driven by consonant preferences? Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 122(2), e1–e10.PubMedCrossRef Ingendahl, M., & Vogel, T. (2022b). The articulatory in-out effect: driven by consonant preferences? Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 122(2), e1–e10.PubMedCrossRef
go back to reference Ingendahl, M., Vogel, T., & Topolinski, S. (2022). Can sequencing explain the in-out effect? Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 26, 449.PubMedCrossRef Ingendahl, M., Vogel, T., & Topolinski, S. (2022). Can sequencing explain the in-out effect? Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 26, 449.PubMedCrossRef
go back to reference Ingendahl, M., Vogel, T., & Topolinski, S. (2022c). The articulatory in-out effect: replicable, but inexplicable. Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 26, 8–10.PubMedCrossRef Ingendahl, M., Vogel, T., & Topolinski, S. (2022c). The articulatory in-out effect: replicable, but inexplicable. Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 26, 8–10.PubMedCrossRef
go back to reference Ingendahl, M., Vogel, T., & Wänke, M. (2022d). The articulatory in-out effect: driven by articulation fluency? Journal of Experimental Social Psychology, 99, 104273.CrossRef Ingendahl, M., Vogel, T., & Wänke, M. (2022d). The articulatory in-out effect: driven by articulation fluency? Journal of Experimental Social Psychology, 99, 104273.CrossRef
go back to reference Judd, C. M., Westfall, J., & Kenny, D. A. (2012). Treating stimuli as a random factor in social psychology: a new and comprehensive solution to a pervasive but largely ignored problem. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 103(1), 54–69.PubMedCrossRef Judd, C. M., Westfall, J., & Kenny, D. A. (2012). Treating stimuli as a random factor in social psychology: a new and comprehensive solution to a pervasive but largely ignored problem. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 103(1), 54–69.PubMedCrossRef
go back to reference Kahneman, D., & Miller, D. T. (1986). Norm theory: comparing reality to its alternatives. Psychological Review, 93(2), 136–153.CrossRef Kahneman, D., & Miller, D. T. (1986). Norm theory: comparing reality to its alternatives. Psychological Review, 93(2), 136–153.CrossRef
go back to reference Keren, G. (2014). Between-or within-subjects design: a methodological dilemma. A Handbook for Data Analysis in the Behaviorial Sciences, 1, 257–272. Keren, G. (2014). Between-or within-subjects design: a methodological dilemma. A Handbook for Data Analysis in the Behaviorial Sciences, 1, 257–272.
go back to reference Klink, R. R. (2000). Creating brand names with meaning: the use of sound symbolism. Marketing Letters, 11(1), 5–20.CrossRef Klink, R. R. (2000). Creating brand names with meaning: the use of sound symbolism. Marketing Letters, 11(1), 5–20.CrossRef
go back to reference Köhler, W. (1970). Gestalt psychology: An introduction to new concepts in modern psychology (Vol. 18). WW Norton & Company. Köhler, W. (1970). Gestalt psychology: An introduction to new concepts in modern psychology (Vol. 18). WW Norton & Company.
go back to reference Körner, A., Bakhtiari, G., & Topolinski, S. (2019). Training articulation sequences: a first systematic modulation of the articulatory in–out effect. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition, 45(10), 1725–1732.PubMed Körner, A., Bakhtiari, G., & Topolinski, S. (2019). Training articulation sequences: a first systematic modulation of the articulatory in–out effect. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition, 45(10), 1725–1732.PubMed
go back to reference Körner, A., & Rummer, R. (2021). What is preferred in the in–out effect: articulation locations or articulation movement direction? Cognition and Emotion, 36, 1–10. Körner, A., & Rummer, R. (2021). What is preferred in the in–out effect: articulation locations or articulation movement direction? Cognition and Emotion, 36, 1–10.
go back to reference Laham, S. M., Koval, P., & Alter, A. L. (2012). The name-pronunciation effect: Why people like Mr. Smith more than Mr. Colquhoun. Journal of Experimental Social Psychology, 48(3), 752–756.CrossRef Laham, S. M., Koval, P., & Alter, A. L. (2012). The name-pronunciation effect: Why people like Mr. Smith more than Mr. Colquhoun. Journal of Experimental Social Psychology, 48(3), 752–756.CrossRef
go back to reference Lakens, D. (2013). Calculating and reporting effect sizes to facilitate cumulative science: a practical primer for t tests and ANOVAs. Frontiers in Psychology, 4, 863.PubMedPubMedCentralCrossRef Lakens, D. (2013). Calculating and reporting effect sizes to facilitate cumulative science: a practical primer for t tests and ANOVAs. Frontiers in Psychology, 4, 863.PubMedPubMedCentralCrossRef
go back to reference Lindau, B., & Topolinski, S. (2018a). The articulatory in-out effect resists oral motor interference. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition, 44(2), 209–220.PubMed Lindau, B., & Topolinski, S. (2018a). The articulatory in-out effect resists oral motor interference. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition, 44(2), 209–220.PubMed
go back to reference Lindau, B., & Topolinski, S. (2018b). The influence of articulation dynamics on recognition memory. Cognition, 179, 37–55.PubMedCrossRef Lindau, B., & Topolinski, S. (2018b). The influence of articulation dynamics on recognition memory. Cognition, 179, 37–55.PubMedCrossRef
go back to reference MacDonald, M. C., & Weiss, D. J. (2022). Easy does it: sequencing explains the in-out effect. Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 26(6), 447-448.PubMedCrossRef MacDonald, M. C., & Weiss, D. J. (2022). Easy does it: sequencing explains the in-out effect. Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 26(6), 447-448.PubMedCrossRef
go back to reference Macedo, L., & Cardoso, A. (2019). A contrast-based computational model of surprise and its applications. Topics in Cognitive Science, 11(1), 88–102.PubMedCrossRef Macedo, L., & Cardoso, A. (2019). A contrast-based computational model of surprise and its applications. Topics in Cognitive Science, 11(1), 88–102.PubMedCrossRef
go back to reference Maschmann, I. T., Körner, A., Boecker, L., & Topolinski, S. (2020). Front in the mouth, front in the word: The driving mechanisms of the in-out effect. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 119(4), 792–807.PubMedCrossRef Maschmann, I. T., Körner, A., Boecker, L., & Topolinski, S. (2020). Front in the mouth, front in the word: The driving mechanisms of the in-out effect. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 119(4), 792–807.PubMedCrossRef
go back to reference Motoki, K., & Pathak, A. (2022). Articulatory global branding: Generalizability, modulators, and mechanisms of the in-out effect in non-WEIRD consumers. Journal of Business Research, 149, 231–239.CrossRef Motoki, K., & Pathak, A. (2022). Articulatory global branding: Generalizability, modulators, and mechanisms of the in-out effect in non-WEIRD consumers. Journal of Business Research, 149, 231–239.CrossRef
go back to reference Motoki, K., Saito, T., Park, J., Velasco, C., Spence, C., & Sugiura, M. (2020). Tasting names: systematic investigations of taste-speech sounds associations. Food Quality and Preference, 80, 103801.CrossRef Motoki, K., Saito, T., Park, J., Velasco, C., Spence, C., & Sugiura, M. (2020). Tasting names: systematic investigations of taste-speech sounds associations. Food Quality and Preference, 80, 103801.CrossRef
go back to reference Mussweiler, T. (2003). Comparison processes in social judgment: mechanisms and consequences. Psychological Review, 110(3), 472–489.PubMedCrossRef Mussweiler, T. (2003). Comparison processes in social judgment: mechanisms and consequences. Psychological Review, 110(3), 472–489.PubMedCrossRef
go back to reference Newman, E. J., Sanson, M., Miller, E. K., Quigley-McBride, A., Foster, J. L., Bernstein, D. M., & Garry, M. (2014). People with easier to pronounce names promote truthiness of claims. PLoS ONE, 9(2), e88671.PubMedPubMedCentralCrossRef Newman, E. J., Sanson, M., Miller, E. K., Quigley-McBride, A., Foster, J. L., Bernstein, D. M., & Garry, M. (2014). People with easier to pronounce names promote truthiness of claims. PLoS ONE, 9(2), e88671.PubMedPubMedCentralCrossRef
go back to reference Onishi, K. H., Chambers, K. E., & Fisher, C. (2002). Learning phonotactic constraints from brief auditory experience. Cognition, 83(1), B13–B23.PubMedCrossRef Onishi, K. H., Chambers, K. E., & Fisher, C. (2002). Learning phonotactic constraints from brief auditory experience. Cognition, 83(1), B13–B23.PubMedCrossRef
go back to reference Pathak, A., Calvert, G. A., & Motoki, K. (2021). Sound symbolism overrides articulation dynamics in the taste continuum. Food Quality and Preference, 91, 104186.CrossRef Pathak, A., Calvert, G. A., & Motoki, K. (2021). Sound symbolism overrides articulation dynamics in the taste continuum. Food Quality and Preference, 91, 104186.CrossRef
go back to reference Payne, B. K., Cheng, C. M., Govorun, O., & Stewart, B. D. (2005). An inkblot for attitudes: affect misattribution as implicit measurement. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 89(3), 277–293.PubMedCrossRef Payne, B. K., Cheng, C. M., Govorun, O., & Stewart, B. D. (2005). An inkblot for attitudes: affect misattribution as implicit measurement. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 89(3), 277–293.PubMedCrossRef
go back to reference Reber, R., Schwarz, N., & Winkielman, P. (2004). Processing fluency and aesthetic pleasure: is beauty in the perceiver’s processing experience? Personality and Social Psychology Review, 8(4), 364–382.PubMedCrossRef Reber, R., Schwarz, N., & Winkielman, P. (2004). Processing fluency and aesthetic pleasure: is beauty in the perceiver’s processing experience? Personality and Social Psychology Review, 8(4), 364–382.PubMedCrossRef
go back to reference Rossi, P., Pantoja, F., Borges, A., & Werle, C. O. (2017). What a delicious name! Articulatory movement effects on food perception and consumption. Journal of the Association for Consumer Research, 2(4), 392–401.CrossRef Rossi, P., Pantoja, F., Borges, A., & Werle, C. O. (2017). What a delicious name! Articulatory movement effects on food perception and consumption. Journal of the Association for Consumer Research, 2(4), 392–401.CrossRef
go back to reference Rumelhart, D. E., & Ortony, A. (2017). The representation of knowledge in memory 1. In R. C. Anderson, R. J. Spiro, & W. E. Montague (Eds.), Schooling and the acquisition of knowledge (pp. 99–135). Routledge.CrossRef Rumelhart, D. E., & Ortony, A. (2017). The representation of knowledge in memory 1. In R. C. Anderson, R. J. Spiro, & W. E. Montague (Eds.), Schooling and the acquisition of knowledge (pp. 99–135). Routledge.CrossRef
go back to reference Rummer, R., Schweppe, J., Schlegelmilch, R., & Grice, M. (2014). Mood is linked to vowel type: the role of articulatory movements. Emotion, 14(2), 246.PubMedCrossRef Rummer, R., Schweppe, J., Schlegelmilch, R., & Grice, M. (2014). Mood is linked to vowel type: the role of articulatory movements. Emotion, 14(2), 246.PubMedCrossRef
go back to reference Sapir, E. (1929). A study in phonetic symbolism. Journal of Experimental Psychology, 12(3), 225.CrossRef Sapir, E. (1929). A study in phonetic symbolism. Journal of Experimental Psychology, 12(3), 225.CrossRef
go back to reference Silva, R. R., Chrobot, N., Newman, E., Schwarz, N., & Topolinski, S. (2017). Make it short and easy: Username complexity determines trustworthiness above and beyond objective reputation. Frontiers in Psychology, 8, 2200.PubMedPubMedCentralCrossRef Silva, R. R., Chrobot, N., Newman, E., Schwarz, N., & Topolinski, S. (2017). Make it short and easy: Username complexity determines trustworthiness above and beyond objective reputation. Frontiers in Psychology, 8, 2200.PubMedPubMedCentralCrossRef
go back to reference Silva, R. R., & Topolinski, S. (2018). My username is IN! The influence of inward vs. outward wandering usernames on judgments of online seller trustworthiness. Psychology & Marketing, 35(4), 307–319.CrossRef Silva, R. R., & Topolinski, S. (2018). My username is IN! The influence of inward vs. outward wandering usernames on judgments of online seller trustworthiness. Psychology & Marketing, 35(4), 307–319.CrossRef
go back to reference Slepian, M. L., & Galinsky, A. D. (2016). The voiced pronunciation of initial phonemes predicts the gender of names. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 110(4), 509.PubMedCrossRef Slepian, M. L., & Galinsky, A. D. (2016). The voiced pronunciation of initial phonemes predicts the gender of names. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 110(4), 509.PubMedCrossRef
go back to reference Song, H., & Schwarz, N. (2009). If it’s difficult to pronounce, it must be risky: fluency, familiarity, and risk perception. Psychological Science, 20(2), 135–138.PubMedCrossRef Song, H., & Schwarz, N. (2009). If it’s difficult to pronounce, it must be risky: fluency, familiarity, and risk perception. Psychological Science, 20(2), 135–138.PubMedCrossRef
go back to reference Titze, I. R. (1994). Principles of voice production. Prentice-Hall. Titze, I. R. (1994). Principles of voice production. Prentice-Hall.
go back to reference Topolinski, S. (2017a). Articulation patterns in names: a hidden route to consumer preference. Journal of the Association for Consumer Research, 2(4), 382–391.CrossRef Topolinski, S. (2017a). Articulation patterns in names: a hidden route to consumer preference. Journal of the Association for Consumer Research, 2(4), 382–391.CrossRef
go back to reference Topolinski, S. (2017b). On the search for the perfect brand name. In M. R. Solomon & T. M. Lowrey (Eds.), The Routledge companion to consumer behavior (pp. 276–288). New York: Routledge/Taylor & Francis.CrossRef Topolinski, S. (2017b). On the search for the perfect brand name. In M. R. Solomon & T. M. Lowrey (Eds.), The Routledge companion to consumer behavior (pp. 276–288). New York: Routledge/Taylor & Francis.CrossRef
go back to reference Topolinski, S., & Bakhtiari, G. (2016). Sequential approach-avoidance movements: the case of inward and outward articulation. Social Psychology, 47(2), 98–117.CrossRef Topolinski, S., & Bakhtiari, G. (2016). Sequential approach-avoidance movements: the case of inward and outward articulation. Social Psychology, 47(2), 98–117.CrossRef
go back to reference Topolinski, S., Bakhtiari, G., & Erle, T. M. (2016). Can I cut the Gordian tnok? The impact of pronounceability, actual solvability, and length on intuitive problem assessments of anagrams. Cognition, 146, 439–452.PubMedCrossRef Topolinski, S., Bakhtiari, G., & Erle, T. M. (2016). Can I cut the Gordian tnok? The impact of pronounceability, actual solvability, and length on intuitive problem assessments of anagrams. Cognition, 146, 439–452.PubMedCrossRef
go back to reference Topolinski, S., & Boecker, L. (2016a). Minimal conditions of motor inductions of approach-avoidance states: The case of oral movements. Journal of Experimental Psychology: General, 145(12), 1589–1603.PubMedCrossRef Topolinski, S., & Boecker, L. (2016a). Minimal conditions of motor inductions of approach-avoidance states: The case of oral movements. Journal of Experimental Psychology: General, 145(12), 1589–1603.PubMedCrossRef
go back to reference Topolinski, S., & Boecker, L. (2016b). Mouth-watering words: Articulatory inductions of eating-like mouth movements increase perceived food palatability. Appetite, 99, 112–121.PubMedCrossRef Topolinski, S., & Boecker, L. (2016b). Mouth-watering words: Articulatory inductions of eating-like mouth movements increase perceived food palatability. Appetite, 99, 112–121.PubMedCrossRef
go back to reference Topolinski, S., Boecker, L., Erle, T. M., Bakhtiari, G., & Pecher, D. (2017). Matching between oral inward–outward movements of object names and oral movements associated with denoted objects. Cognition and Emotion, 31(1), 3–18.PubMedCrossRef Topolinski, S., Boecker, L., Erle, T. M., Bakhtiari, G., & Pecher, D. (2017). Matching between oral inward–outward movements of object names and oral movements associated with denoted objects. Cognition and Emotion, 31(1), 3–18.PubMedCrossRef
go back to reference Topolinski, S., Ingendahl, M., & Vogel, T. (2022). Can sequencing of articulation ease explain the in-out effect? A preregistered competitive test. Submitted for publication Topolinski, S., Ingendahl, M., & Vogel, T. (2022). Can sequencing of articulation ease explain the in-out effect? A preregistered competitive test. Submitted for publication
go back to reference Topolinski, S., Maschmann, I. T., Pecher, D., & Winkielman, P. (2014). Oral approach–avoidance: affective consequences of muscular articulation dynamics. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 106(6), 885–896.PubMedCrossRef Topolinski, S., Maschmann, I. T., Pecher, D., & Winkielman, P. (2014). Oral approach–avoidance: affective consequences of muscular articulation dynamics. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 106(6), 885–896.PubMedCrossRef
go back to reference Topolinski, S., & Strack, F. (2009a). The analysis of intuition: Processing fluency and affect in judgements of semantic coherence. Cognition and Emotion, 23(8), 1465–1503.CrossRef Topolinski, S., & Strack, F. (2009a). The analysis of intuition: Processing fluency and affect in judgements of semantic coherence. Cognition and Emotion, 23(8), 1465–1503.CrossRef
go back to reference Topolinski, S., & Strack, F. (2009b). The architecture of intuition: Fluency and affect determine intuitive judgments of semantic and visual coherence and judgments of grammaticality in artificial grammar learning. Journal of Experimental Psychology, 138(1), 39–63.PubMedCrossRef Topolinski, S., & Strack, F. (2009b). The architecture of intuition: Fluency and affect determine intuitive judgments of semantic and visual coherence and judgments of grammaticality in artificial grammar learning. Journal of Experimental Psychology, 138(1), 39–63.PubMedCrossRef
go back to reference Topolinski, S., & Strack, F. (2010). False fame prevented: avoiding fluency effects without judgmental correction. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 98(5), 721–733.PubMedCrossRef Topolinski, S., & Strack, F. (2010). False fame prevented: avoiding fluency effects without judgmental correction. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 98(5), 721–733.PubMedCrossRef
go back to reference Topolinski, S., Zürn, M., & Schneider, I. K. (2015). What’s in and what’s out in branding? A novel articulation effect for brand names. Frontiers in Psychology, 6, 585.PubMedPubMedCentralCrossRef Topolinski, S., Zürn, M., & Schneider, I. K. (2015). What’s in and what’s out in branding? A novel articulation effect for brand names. Frontiers in Psychology, 6, 585.PubMedPubMedCentralCrossRef
go back to reference Unkelbach, C., & Memmert, D. (2014). Serial-position effects in evaluative judgments. Current Directions in Psychological Science, 23(3), 195–200.CrossRef Unkelbach, C., & Memmert, D. (2014). Serial-position effects in evaluative judgments. Current Directions in Psychological Science, 23(3), 195–200.CrossRef
go back to reference Unkelbach, C., Ostheimer, V., Fasold, F., & Memmert, D. (2012). A calibration explanation of serial position effects in evaluative judgments. Organizational Behavior and Human Decision Processes, 119(1), 103–113.CrossRef Unkelbach, C., Ostheimer, V., Fasold, F., & Memmert, D. (2012). A calibration explanation of serial position effects in evaluative judgments. Organizational Behavior and Human Decision Processes, 119(1), 103–113.CrossRef
go back to reference Wänke, M., & Hansen, J. (2015). Relative processing fluency. Current Directions in Psychological Science, 24(3), 195–199.CrossRef Wänke, M., & Hansen, J. (2015). Relative processing fluency. Current Directions in Psychological Science, 24(3), 195–199.CrossRef
go back to reference Zürn, M. K., Gerten, J., & Topolinski, S. (2021). Maybe favors: How to get more good deeds done. Journal of Experimental Psychology, 27(3), 503–507.PubMed Zürn, M. K., Gerten, J., & Topolinski, S. (2021). Maybe favors: How to get more good deeds done. Journal of Experimental Psychology, 27(3), 503–507.PubMed
go back to reference Zürn, M., & Topolinski, S. (2017). When trust comes easy: articulatory fluency increases transfers in the trust game. Journal of Economic Psychology, 61, 74–86.CrossRef Zürn, M., & Topolinski, S. (2017). When trust comes easy: articulatory fluency increases transfers in the trust game. Journal of Economic Psychology, 61, 74–86.CrossRef
Metagegevens
Titel
On the emergence of the in–out effect across trials: two items do the trick
Auteurs
Sascha Topolinski
Lea Boecker
Charlotte S. Löffler
Beatriz Gusmão
Moritz Ingendahl
Publicatiedatum
22-07-2022
Uitgeverij
Springer Berlin Heidelberg
Gepubliceerd in
Psychological Research / Uitgave 4/2023
Print ISSN: 0340-0727
Elektronisch ISSN: 1430-2772
DOI
https://doi.org/10.1007/s00426-022-01715-6

Andere artikelen Uitgave 4/2023

Psychological Research 4/2023 Naar de uitgave