Skip to main content
Top
Gepubliceerd in: Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders 7/2017

09-05-2017 | Original Paper

Can Children with Autism Spectrum Disorders Learn New Vocabulary From Linguistic Context?

Auteurs: Rebecca Lucas, Louisa Thomas, Courtenay Frazier Norbury

Gepubliceerd in: Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders | Uitgave 7/2017

Log in om toegang te krijgen
share
DELEN

Deel dit onderdeel of sectie (kopieer de link)

  • Optie A:
    Klik op de rechtermuisknop op de link en selecteer de optie “linkadres kopiëren”
  • Optie B:
    Deel de link per e-mail

Abstract

This study investigated whether children with autism spectrum disorders (ASD) can learn vocabulary from linguistic context. Thirty-five children with ASD (18 with age-appropriate structural language; 17 with language impairment [ALI]) and 29 typically developing peers were taught 20 Science words. Half were presented in linguistic context from which meaning could be inferred, whilst half were accompanied by an explicit definition. Children with ASD were able to learn from context. Condition did not influence phonological learning, but receptive semantic knowledge was greatest in the context condition, and expressive semantic knowledge greatest in the definitional condition. The ALI group learnt less than their peers. This suggests that at least some vocabulary should be taught explicitly, and children with ALI may need additional tuition.
Bijlagen
Alleen toegankelijk voor geautoriseerde gebruikers
Literatuur
go back to reference Adams, C., Clarke, E., & Haynes, R. (2009). Inference and sentence comprehension in children with specific or pragmatic language impairments. International Journal of Language & Communication Disorders, 44(3), 301–318. doi:10.1080/13682820802051788.CrossRef Adams, C., Clarke, E., & Haynes, R. (2009). Inference and sentence comprehension in children with specific or pragmatic language impairments. International Journal of Language & Communication Disorders, 44(3), 301–318. doi:10.​1080/​1368282080205178​8.CrossRef
go back to reference American Psychological Association (2013). Diagnostic and statistical manual of mental disorders (DSM-V). Arlington, VA: American Psychiatric Association.CrossRef American Psychological Association (2013). Diagnostic and statistical manual of mental disorders (DSM-V). Arlington, VA: American Psychiatric Association.CrossRef
go back to reference Armbruster, B., Lehr, F., & Osborn, J. (2010). Put reading first: The research building blocks for teaching children to read. In R. Adler (Ed.), NICHD (3rd ed.). Rockville: NICHD. Armbruster, B., Lehr, F., & Osborn, J. (2010). Put reading first: The research building blocks for teaching children to read. In R. Adler (Ed.), NICHD (3rd ed.). Rockville: NICHD.
go back to reference Brown, H., Weighall, A., Henderson, L., & Gaskell, G. (2012). Enhanced recognition and recall of new words in 7- and 12-year-olds following a period of offline consolidation. Journal of Experimental Child Psychology, 112(1), 56–72. doi:10.1016/j.jecp.2011.11.010.CrossRefPubMed Brown, H., Weighall, A., Henderson, L., & Gaskell, G. (2012). Enhanced recognition and recall of new words in 7- and 12-year-olds following a period of offline consolidation. Journal of Experimental Child Psychology, 112(1), 56–72. doi:10.​1016/​j.​jecp.​2011.​11.​010.CrossRefPubMed
go back to reference Cain, K., Oakhill, J., & Lemmon, K. (2004). Individual differences in the inference of word meanings from context: The influence of reading comprehension, vocabulary knowledge, and memory capacity. Journal of Educational Psychology, 96(4), 671–681. doi:10.1037/0022-0663.96.4.671.CrossRef Cain, K., Oakhill, J., & Lemmon, K. (2004). Individual differences in the inference of word meanings from context: The influence of reading comprehension, vocabulary knowledge, and memory capacity. Journal of Educational Psychology, 96(4), 671–681. doi:10.​1037/​0022-0663.​96.​4.​671.CrossRef
go back to reference Department for Education. (2015). Special educational needs in England: January 2015 Statistics: Special educational needs. Department for Education. (2015). Special educational needs in England: January 2015 Statistics: Special educational needs.
go back to reference Dodwell, K., & Bavin, E. (2008). Children with specific language impairment: an investigation of their narratives and memory. International Journal of Language & Communication Disorders, 43(2), 201–218. doi:10.1080/13682820701366147.CrossRef Dodwell, K., & Bavin, E. (2008). Children with specific language impairment: an investigation of their narratives and memory. International Journal of Language & Communication Disorders, 43(2), 201–218. doi:10.​1080/​1368282070136614​7.CrossRef
go back to reference Feinstein, L., & Duckworth, K. (2006). Development in the early years: Its importance for school performance and adult outcomes. Wider Benefits of Learning Research Report (Vol. 20). London: Center for Research on the Wider Benefits of Learning, Institute of Education Feinstein, L., & Duckworth, K. (2006). Development in the early years: Its importance for school performance and adult outcomes. Wider Benefits of Learning Research Report (Vol. 20). London: Center for Research on the Wider Benefits of Learning, Institute of Education
go back to reference Gardner, M. (1990). Receptive one-word picture vocabulary test (revised): Manual. Novato, CA: Academic Therapy Publications. Gardner, M. (1990). Receptive one-word picture vocabulary test (revised): Manual. Novato, CA: Academic Therapy Publications.
go back to reference Goodlin-Jones, B., Tang, K., Liu, J., & Thomas, F. (2008). Sleep patterns in preschool-age children with autism, developmental delay, and typical development. Journal of the American Academy of Child & Adolescent Psychiatry, 47(8), 930–938. doi:10.1097/CHI.ObO13e3181799f7c.CrossRef Goodlin-Jones, B., Tang, K., Liu, J., & Thomas, F. (2008). Sleep patterns in preschool-age children with autism, developmental delay, and typical development. Journal of the American Academy of Child & Adolescent Psychiatry, 47(8), 930–938. doi:10.​1097/​CHI.​ObO13e3181799f7c​.CrossRef
go back to reference Lindgren, K., Folstein, S., Tomblin, J., & Tager-Flusberg, H. (2009). Language and reading abilities of children with autism spectrum disorders and specific language impairment and their first-degree relatives. Autism Research, 2(1), 22–38. doi:10.1002/aur.63.CrossRefPubMedPubMedCentral Lindgren, K., Folstein, S., Tomblin, J., & Tager-Flusberg, H. (2009). Language and reading abilities of children with autism spectrum disorders and specific language impairment and their first-degree relatives. Autism Research, 2(1), 22–38. doi:10.​1002/​aur.​63.CrossRefPubMedPubMedCentral
go back to reference Loucas, T., Charman, T., Pickles, A., Simonoff, E., Chandler, S., Meldrum, D., & Baird, G. (2008). Autistic symptomatology and language ability in autism spectrum disorder and specific language impairment. Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry, 49(11), 1184–1192. doi:10.1111/j.1469-7610.2008.01951.x.CrossRefPubMed Loucas, T., Charman, T., Pickles, A., Simonoff, E., Chandler, S., Meldrum, D., & Baird, G. (2008). Autistic symptomatology and language ability in autism spectrum disorder and specific language impairment. Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry, 49(11), 1184–1192. doi:10.​1111/​j.​1469-7610.​2008.​01951.​x.CrossRefPubMed
go back to reference McGregor, K., Rost, G., Arenas, R., Farris-Trimble, A., & Stiles, D. (2013). Children with ASD can use gaze in support of word recognition and learning. Journal of Child Psychology & Psychiatry, 54(7), 745–753. doi:10.1111/jcpp.12073.CrossRef McGregor, K., Rost, G., Arenas, R., Farris-Trimble, A., & Stiles, D. (2013). Children with ASD can use gaze in support of word recognition and learning. Journal of Child Psychology & Psychiatry, 54(7), 745–753. doi:10.​1111/​jcpp.​12073.CrossRef
go back to reference Morgan, P., Farkas, G., Hillemeier, M., Hammer, C., & Maczuga, S. (2015). 24-month-old children with larger oral vocabularies display greater academic and behavioral functioning at kindergarten entry. Child Development, 86(5), 1351–1370. doi:10.1111/cdev.12398.CrossRefPubMedPubMedCentral Morgan, P., Farkas, G., Hillemeier, M., Hammer, C., & Maczuga, S. (2015). 24-month-old children with larger oral vocabularies display greater academic and behavioral functioning at kindergarten entry. Child Development, 86(5), 1351–1370. doi:10.​1111/​cdev.​12398.CrossRefPubMedPubMedCentral
go back to reference Nagy, W., & Townsend, D. (2012). Words as tools: Learning academic vocabulary as language acquisition. Reading Research Quarterly, 47(1), 91–108. doi:10.1002/RRQ.011.CrossRef Nagy, W., & Townsend, D. (2012). Words as tools: Learning academic vocabulary as language acquisition. Reading Research Quarterly, 47(1), 91–108. doi:10.​1002/​RRQ.​011.CrossRef
go back to reference Nash, H., & Snowling, M. (2006). Teaching new words to children with poor existing vocabulary knowledge: a controlled evaluation of the definition and context methods. International Journal of Language & Communication Disorders, 41(3), 335–354. doi:10.1080/13682820600602295.CrossRef Nash, H., & Snowling, M. (2006). Teaching new words to children with poor existing vocabulary knowledge: a controlled evaluation of the definition and context methods. International Journal of Language & Communication Disorders, 41(3), 335–354. doi:10.​1080/​1368282060060229​5.CrossRef
go back to reference Norbury, C., & Bishop, D. (2002). Inferential processing and story recall in children with communication problems: A comparison of specific language impairment, pragmatic language impairment and high-functioning autism. International Journal of Language & Communication Disorders, 37(3), 227–251. doi:10.1080/13682820210136269.CrossRef Norbury, C., & Bishop, D. (2002). Inferential processing and story recall in children with communication problems: A comparison of specific language impairment, pragmatic language impairment and high-functioning autism. International Journal of Language & Communication Disorders, 37(3), 227–251. doi:10.​1080/​1368282021013626​9.CrossRef
go back to reference Norbury, C., & Nation, K. (2011). Understanding variability in reading comprehension in adolescents with Autism Spectrum Disorders: Interactions with language status and decoding skill. Scientific Studies of Reading, 15(3), 191–210. doi:10.1080/10888431003623553.CrossRef Norbury, C., & Nation, K. (2011). Understanding variability in reading comprehension in adolescents with Autism Spectrum Disorders: Interactions with language status and decoding skill. Scientific Studies of Reading, 15(3), 191–210. doi:10.​1080/​1088843100362355​3.CrossRef
go back to reference Riches, N., Loucas, T., Charman, T., Simonoff, E., & Baird, G. (2010). Sentence repetition in children with specific language impairment and autism: A study of linguistic factors affecting recall. International Journal of Language & Communication Disorders, 45, 47–60. doi:10.1111/j.1460-6984.2012.00158.CrossRef Riches, N., Loucas, T., Charman, T., Simonoff, E., & Baird, G. (2010). Sentence repetition in children with specific language impairment and autism: A study of linguistic factors affecting recall. International Journal of Language & Communication Disorders, 45, 47–60. doi:10.​1111/​j.​1460-6984.​2012.​00158.CrossRef
go back to reference Ricketts, J., Bishop, D. V., & Nation, K. (2009). Orthographic facilitation in oral vocabulary acquisition. The Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology, 62(10), 1948–1966. doi:10.1080/17470210802696104. Ricketts, J., Bishop, D. V., & Nation, K. (2009). Orthographic facilitation in oral vocabulary acquisition. The Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology, 62(10), 1948–1966. doi:10.​1080/​1747021080269610​4.
go back to reference Ricketts, J., Dockrell, J., Patel, N., Charman, T., & Lindsay, G. (2015). Do children with specific language impairment and autism spectrum disorders benefit from the presence of orthography when learning new spoken words? Journal of Experimental Child Psychology, 134(0), 43–61. doi:10.1016/j.jecp.2015.01.015.CrossRefPubMed Ricketts, J., Dockrell, J., Patel, N., Charman, T., & Lindsay, G. (2015). Do children with specific language impairment and autism spectrum disorders benefit from the presence of orthography when learning new spoken words? Journal of Experimental Child Psychology, 134(0), 43–61. doi:10.​1016/​j.​jecp.​2015.​01.​015.CrossRefPubMed
go back to reference Rutter, M., Bailey, A., & Lord, C. (2003). Social communication questionnaire (SCQ). Los Angeles, CA: Western Psychological Services. Rutter, M., Bailey, A., & Lord, C. (2003). Social communication questionnaire (SCQ). Los Angeles, CA: Western Psychological Services.
go back to reference Semel, E., Wiig, E., & Secord, W. (2003). Clinical evaluation of language fundamentals (CELF-4 UK). London: Harcourt Assessment. Semel, E., Wiig, E., & Secord, W. (2003). Clinical evaluation of language fundamentals (CELF-4 UK). London: Harcourt Assessment.
go back to reference Stahl, S., & Fairbanks, M. (1986). The effects of vocabulary instruction: A model-based meta-analysis. Review of Educational Research, 56(1), 72–110. doi:10.2307/1170287.CrossRef Stahl, S., & Fairbanks, M. (1986). The effects of vocabulary instruction: A model-based meta-analysis. Review of Educational Research, 56(1), 72–110. doi:10.​2307/​1170287.CrossRef
go back to reference Wechsler, D. (1999). Wechsler abbreviated scales of intelligence (WASI). San Antonio, TX: Harcourt Assesment. Wechsler, D. (1999). Wechsler abbreviated scales of intelligence (WASI). San Antonio, TX: Harcourt Assesment.
Metagegevens
Titel
Can Children with Autism Spectrum Disorders Learn New Vocabulary From Linguistic Context?
Auteurs
Rebecca Lucas
Louisa Thomas
Courtenay Frazier Norbury
Publicatiedatum
09-05-2017
Uitgeverij
Springer US
Gepubliceerd in
Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders / Uitgave 7/2017
Print ISSN: 0162-3257
Elektronisch ISSN: 1573-3432
DOI
https://doi.org/10.1007/s10803-017-3151-z

Andere artikelen Uitgave 7/2017

Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders 7/2017 Naar de uitgave