‘If I don’t do it, I’m out of Rhythm and I can’t focus as Well’: positive and negative adult interpretations of therapies aimed at ‘fixing’ their restricted and repetitive behaviours in childhood
Restricted and repetitive behaviours (RRBs) are a central presentation in individuals diagnosed with autism spectrum disorder (ASD; henceforth referred to as autism). RRBs often cause significant challenges and barriers to individual adaptive learning, psychosocial and family functioning, and often attract stigmatising attitudes and behaviour from others (Barrett et al.,
2004). RRBs are also linked to positive personal traits such as attention to detail; sustained focus; memory; and expertise. In addition, they are a coping mechanism for many in a complex, and sometimes confusing and stressful environment (Prior & Ozonoff,
2007). For instance, sophisticated interests involving particular expertise may benefit individuals by leading to niche employment opportunities in which individuals can utilise their special interests and expertise (Howlin,
2003). Therefore, this study seeks both positive and negative interpretations of adults previously diagnosed with autism and ‘treated’ in childhood for restricted and repetitive behaviours. As a qualitative study it explores the ‘lived’ experience from the insider’s perspective. Please note that we use both identity-first language (autistic person) and person-first language (person with autism) in this paper to reflect variability in the terminology preferences of the study participants and autism community.
Restricted and repetitive behaviours and interests as described by the Diagnostic and Statistical Manual of Mental (5th ed.; DSM-V; American Psychiatric Association
2013) are subdivided into four subtypes: (1) inflexible adherence to routines; (2) preoccupation with highly restricted and fixated interests; (3) stereotyped or repetitive motor movements; and (4) unusual interest in sensory aspects of the environment. MacDonald et al., (
2007) suggested that self-stimulatory behaviours (SSBs) or ‘stimming’ are a specific group of RRBs which are readily noticed by others (e.g., spinning, rocking, phrasal repetition, or repetitive unrecognisable vocalisations). Some SSBs can be acutely harmful to the person (e.g., pinching or hitting self). Other stimming behaviours may be less noticeable and less socially problematic (e.g., rubbing, smelling, or sucking), and may provide benefits for emotional and cognitive self-regulation (Davidson,
2010; Kapp et al.,
2019; Leekam, Prior, & Uljarevic, 2011). Again, these may also have the potential to cause harm when unrecognised as with lead poisoning through ingestion of paint fragments or sucking on rusty water piping. Regardless, stimming - often interpreted as problematic - has become a therapeutic target.
Generally, there is scant knowledge of the personal experiences of being the recipient of interventions for stimming behaviours. Joyce et al., (
2017) provided the first evidence of young people diagnosed with autism and shared their perspectives on restricted and repetitive behaviours. The participants of their study considered restricted and repetitive behaviours as necessities in their lives highlighting that routines associated with these behaviours helped to reduce uncertainty associated with worry and anxiety. Similarly, autistic adults reflected on the impact of interventions received in childhood aimed at altering or removing stimming behaviours (Kapp et al.,
2019). They described stimming as a self-regulatory mechanism that provided a calming rhythm enabling them to cope with sensory overload, an internal flood of thoughts, or uncontainable emotions (Kapp et al.,
2019).
Evidence suggests that people diagnosed with autism are neurologically hyper-responsive to sensory stimuli. For example, sensory responsive behaviours (i.e., extreme negative response to, or avoidance of, sensory stimuli) may be associated with heightened responsivity in the primary sensory areas and those areas related to emotion regulation and processing (i.e. orbital-frontal cortex, hippocampus and amygdala) (Green et al.,
2013). Indeed, recent studies have found that sensory hypersensitivity is strongly related more broadly to RRBs, not only among autistic but also neurotypical children (Schulz & Stevenson,
2019; Fetta et al.,
2021). As such, stimming, resistance to change, and a predilection for routine, may each serve as adaptive coping strategies in response to an overwhelming, novel, and ever-changing environment (Lawson et al.,
2014; Pellicano & Burr,
2012). However, it is important to note that different types of hyper- or indeed hypo-sensitivities may be linked to different types of RRBs and serve different functions (Schulz & Stevenson,
2019; Fetta et al.,
2021).
Increasingly, anxiety and arousal are recognised as having an impact on: (1) repetitive motor; and, (2) insistence on sameness behaviours; in autism (Joyce et al.,
2017; Lidstone et al.,
2014) including e.g., restricted routines and interests (Gotham et al.,
2013; Rodgers et al.,
2012). This suggests that although engagement in RRBs is associated with anxiety, and therefore can be interpreted as a symptom of anxiety, they may serve also as a coping response to attenuate anxiety and excessive arousal. Different types of RRBs may also serve different functions with Lidstone et al., (
2014) reporting that parent-rated anxiety symptoms in children were significantly associated with children’s insistence on sameness behaviours, but not with the repetitive sensory-motor behaviours. Moreover, they found that the relationship between insistence on sameness behaviours and anxiety was mediated by sensory avoiding.
Despite RRBs serving as coping mechanisms, suggesting that non-injurious forms of stimming are adaptive (Kapp et al.,
2019; Orsini & Smith,
2010), RRBs are often targeted in skill-based behavioural interventions (Patterson et al.,
2010), as well as in classroom management (Pas et al.,
2016). Often, this is the result of parents or teachers seeking professional help and interventions to reduce a child’s stimming and stereotypic behaviours (Kodak & Bergmann,
2020). This is due to unusual or ritualistic behaviours being viewed as problematic, and potentially stressful and embarrassing for parents and carers, especially if the stimming behaviours are noticed by others in public (Swaab et al.,
2017). Indeed, the interpretation of such behaviours often contributes to stigmatisation, rejection, and exclusion in society of individuals diagnosed with autism, and their families (Kinnear et al.,
2016).
Applied Behaviour Analysis (ABA) is the most ubiquitous early childhood intervention recommended by clinicians and therapists for children diagnosed with autism. It is considered to be the golden standard for the treatment of autism (Vismara & Rogers,
2010). ABA is a teaching method based on theories of learning and operant conditioning (Lovaas,
1987). It relies extensively on external reinforcement to increase a behaviour, both positive (e.g., edible rewards, verbal praise, or tokens) and negative (e.g., removal of aversive stimuli), and external punishment to diminish a behaviour, both positive (e.g., restitutional over-correction, negative practice, or physical restraint), and negative (e.g., time out procedure or losing a token). Therapists or clinicians who adopt a behavioural model or ABA principle not only intend to enhance language use, communication skills, attention, and social skills in children diagnosed with ASD, but also to modify or reduce self-injurious behaviours and behaviours that have been deemed as socially unacceptable (Kodak & Bergmann,
2020). Arguably, the ethics of conducting interventions for stimming behaviours remain controversial, questioning whether the perception of the observer, not the individual with the behaviour, is the issue.
Sandoval-Norton et al., (
2019) criticised behavioural interventions that involved long-term treatment to condition a child to stop stimming or obey commands such as ‘quiet hands’. Such treatment protocols evolved without an apparent understanding of the function of RRBs for autistic individuals with their neurological uniqueness. The question remains as to whether the use of change interventions for RRBs is irresponsible and possibly abusive. Interventions that involve various punishments and rewards without providing a meaning of the task that the individual is being asked to do, or not to do, can have negative impacts on individuals’ intrinsic motivation, self-esteem and self-efficacy to engage in the task (Sandoval-Norton et al.,
2019).
Furthermore, there is evidence of increased post-traumatic stress in autistic individuals who have been exposed to ABA therapy (Kupferstein,
2018). Nearly half of the autistic adult and autistic child participants in Kupferstein’s study who were exposed to ABA early childhood intervention, noted posttraumatic stress symptoms. These included intrusive symptoms (e.g., nightmares of the traumatic event and flashbacks), persistent avoidance of trauma-related stimuli, negative alterations in cognition and mood associated with the traumatic event, and marked alterations in arousal and reactivity associated with the traumatic event (Kupferstein,
2018). More recently, qualitative evidence supports these findings further raising concerns regarding the long-term negative impacts of ABA therapy on the wellbeing of autistic adults (McGill & Robinson, 2021). Despite the evidence described above, the principle of ABA or behavioural model remains widely touted as an effective intervention for individuals on the autism spectrum, and the tenets of ABA have been integrated into other change interventions (Sandoval-Norton et al.,
2019).
Despite the potential for negative psychological impact from exposure to behavioural interventions for RRBs, over time, many individuals redefine those experiences as being the trigger for greater self-determination and positive change in their lives e.g. (1) enhanced relationships, improved altruism, and compassion toward others; (2) changed self-perception with a greater acceptance of personal limitations and vulnerabilities; and (3) changed life philosophy with an appreciation of each new day and realisation that life is finite (Tedeschi & Calhoun,
1996; Joseph & Linley,
2005; McCormack et al.,
2017).
In action, posttraumatic growth does not merely define a return to pre-trauma functioning, but the use of cognitive processing to incorporate the trauma-related information into a framework of new meaning post event (Zoellner & Maercker,
2006). Drawing on the work of Joseph & Linley (
2005), the process theory of growth out of adversity posits that we have an innate human motivation to re-evaluate experiences toward an improvement in well-being and sense of fulfillment.
Nevertheless, there remains scant information regarding positive or negative outcomes from RRBs interventions in childhood and the subsequent social stigmatisation of their use in those with Autism. Therefore, using Interpretative Phenomenological Analysis (Smith,
1996), this idiographic study explores positive and negative subjective interpretations of adults diagnosed with autism reflecting on their childhood. Particularly, it seeks to understand how they perceive their RRBs or stimming behaviours, and any strategies or interventions that were introduced by others to ‘fix’ those behaviours during childhood.
Discussion
This study sheds light on the subjective interpretation of four participants diagnosed with autism who experienced interventions targeting their restricted and repetitive behaviours. Results indicated two superordinate themes 1)
Doubt, stigma and being fixed according to others, which overarched three subordinate themes: (a)
What is normal? (b)
Not good enough; and (c)
Forced to assimilate, and 2)
Embracing Authenticity, which overarched two subordinate themes: (a)
The secure base; and (b)
Growth out of diversity. From the adult perspective, the participants reflected on their childhood characterised with confusion, distress, social isolation, and self-doubt. Once signs and symptoms of autism spectrum disorder, as defined by the DSM (American Psychiatric Association,
2013), were noticed by participant’s parents or teacher, participants received a range of interventions, special education and parenting strategies targeting their RRBs. These themes illustrated the enormous impact of the stigmatic social, medical, and educational systems and associated interventions on participants’ self-esteem and personal assumptions. These participants, however, did not identify themselves and their unusual harmless behaviours as a problem that required fixing. Importantly, results from this study suggest that for these participants, RRBs require understanding for their unique contribution to the participants’ functioning in the world. Together with the participants’ family and friends who genuinely accepted them, participants have redefined their sense of flawed self to one that is uniquely neurotypical promoting psychological growth. This allows them to reject boundaries established by the bias that neurotypical is normal and non-neurotypical is pathological and therefore disordered.
The participants in this study spoke of passively receiving the diagnostic label and associated interventions during their childhood. As children, participants recalled not recognising their functioning and behaviours as distinct from the other neurotypical individuals. Nevertheless, their RRBs were identified as ‘problem behaviours’ broadly defined as behaviours that are not socially acceptable, negatively impacting their functioning, and physically dangerous (Kodak & Bergmann,
2020). ‘Problem behaviours’ became one of the main reasons for referral to intervention and a source of parents, caregivers or teacher’s stress and concerns. Participants recalled the experiences of being removed from a standard classroom, seeing a professional for behavioural intervention, receiving medical treatment, and being involved in specialised autism education setting. Many described feelings of isolation, confusion, restriction, suffocation, and sadness during the interventions. Participants in this study recalled that others’ judgement and perception of their RRBs led participants to wonder
what is normal? Questioning ‘normal’ guided by social norms and the medical model, participants challenged neurotypical bias suggesting a more holistic shift in thinking and an inclusive concept of neurodiversity. As such, participants suggested that a spread of acceptance of individual functioning along a continuum would preclude a need to be cured, and highlight diversity of behaviour, some of which might benefit from assistance, integration, and accommodation.
As the social stigma associated with ‘not normal’ behaviours continued, these participants recalled bullying experiences leading to negative self-evaluation and self-blame (Kinnear et al.,
2016). In this context, participants experienced feelings of guilt, sadness, and the pain of being alienated. Previous research has found that youth diagnosed with autism are more likely to experience bullying compared to their neurotypically developing peers, especially for those who also reported being more socially anxious (van Schalkwyk et al.,
2018). A participant of this study gave an insight into the interpersonal dynamic between autistic individuals who exhibit RRBs and the neurotypical individuals, suggesting that neurotypicals’ intolerance of diversity during interactions with those exhibiting RRBs may cause a rise in anxiety rather than the RRBs themselves. Nacoste (
2010) examined individuals’ emotions and struggle in response to various social markers of difference, such as race, gender, ability, and religion suggesting that individuals often experience anxiety during the interaction with others who vary from them in terms of one or more of these social markers. Likewise, in addition to others’ perception of their RRBs and criticism against their RRBs, autistic individuals also shared experiences of intolerance of uncertainty living in a rapidly changing world and overstimulated world. These factors may induce a high level of stress, anxiety, and worry, which may be associated with the engagement of insistence on sameness behaviours (Lidstone et al.,
2014; Wigham et al.,
2015). Future research may consider the interaction between these two psychological processes.
One of the participants in this study categorised interventions for RRBs into two types: interventions for harmful RRBs (e.g. self-hitting or hand banging) and interventions for harmless RRBs. It is suggested that interventions for harmful RRBs could be effective as crisis management strategies to de-escalate potentially dangerous situations. On the other hand, others’ attempts at interrupting harmless stimming behaviours and differential reinforcement of alternative behaviour, were commonly experienced by the participants. Consistent with previous research, participants reported that these approaches effectively restrained and modified their repetitive behaviours deemed ‘problematic’, such as tapping objects, flapping hands, pacing in a circle and spinning (Patterson et al.,
2010). However, the interruption interventions were implemented at the expense of participants’ internal processing. Individuals diagnosed with autism are often neurologically hypersensitive to sensory stimuli, they have trouble with filtering and processing sensory information and are often overwhelmed by stimuli in the environment that other people may not even notice (Green et al.,
2013). As a result, autistic individuals may engage in sensory modulation behaviours to regulate their sensory experience (Green et al.,
2013; Kapp et al.,
2019). Similarly, the participants of this study recognised RRBs as innate coping mechanisms to overcome an overwhelming environment providing a calming rhythm to maintain focus. One participant highlighted that RRBs are emotional regulation strategies, and the restriction of RRBs causes agitation. Recent qualitative research supports the view that autistic adults see stimming (i.e., an RRB) as a self-regulatory behaviour with many positive effects experiencing negative consequences when they are forced to repress them (Charlton et al.,
2021).
The subordinate theme
Forced to assimilate reflected the boundaries and difficulties experienced by the participants in various social settings and during interpersonal interactions. They described the social norms and informal rules as restrictions against their RRBs. These participants came to realise that they are neurologically different from individuals considered to have had a neurotypical development. It has been proposed that people with autism skilfully developed strategies to disguise themselves as neurotypical in a stressful world (Dean et al.,
2016; Tierney et al.,
2016). Known as ‘passing as non-autistic’ (PAN: Libsack et al., 2021) this may bring advantages and deflect criticism. However, negative implications are recognised including pressure to conform to neurotypical social expectations, poor mental health e.g. internalised depression and anxiety, avoidance of supporting services, and significantly delayed, inaccurate, or lack of an ASD diagnosis that may bring benefits (Libsack et al. 2021; Miller et al.,
2021; Cage & Troxell-Whitman,
2020). Although the difficulties in screening for ASD and a controversy over universal screening remain a challenge (McCarty & Frye,
2020), early diagnostic evaluation for ASD has become increasingly common which may potentially attenuate the issue of a delayed diagnosis (Pierce et al.,
2019; Sicherman et al.,
2021).
Despite the many challenges faced by the participates, this study provided evidence that growth out of adversity was possible for these participants. Through family support, genuine friendship and tailored interventions to address participants’ needs, each participant sought to overcome the criticism and aversive experiences. Consistent with work of Joseph & Linley (
2005), participants demonstrated an actualising tendency towards psychological growth through a favourable and supportive psychosocial environment. They expressed gradual self-acceptance over time, resulting in a shift away from self-doubt and self-blame towards a new perspective of their RRBs. The sense of shame associated with the RRBs was reframed to free them from a restricted state of self-doubt. They recognised the potential of sensory enrichment and their uniqueness. Moreover, positive change in interpersonal relationships was identified. Participants spoke of opening themselves up to receive help and develop supportive friendships.
There are clinical implications from this study. These findings demonstrated that the four autistic participants who experienced distress associated with stigma, blame, rejection, and shame, interpreted trusting relationships as a springboard for self-valuing. As such, therapists are encouraged to assist autistic individuals to build self-awareness and acceptance and gain a better understanding of themselves in the world that engages with healthy self-esteem and self-image (Huang et al.,
2017). Therapists can mentor trusting and supportive relationships within the therapeutic space and provide a safe place for their clients to explore an intervention suited to their psychological needs rather than being ‘fixed’, thus enabling them to confidently engage with self, others, and their life philosophy. As reported by participants, engaging in a collaborative therapeutic relationship facilitated growth, self-acceptance and awareness in this study. Spencer et al., (
2019) suggested that a collaborative therapeutic relationship involved clients playing an active role in decision making, recognising the client’s expertise and building trust. Gifford & Knott (
2016) suggested that diagnostic labels affect care staffs’ perception and attribution of challenging behaviours in individuals with development disorders or learning disability, and the diagnostic label also affects care staffs’ emotional responses. Therefore, therapists should be mindful of their unconscious bias and attribution towards neurological and developmental differences.
Limitations
The current study has several limitations. As an interpretative study, researchers may have introduced bias associated with personal subjective experience during the process of data collection, at interview, or theme selection and analysis. Researchers have consciously sought to minimise such biases through robust discussions and rereading the original transcripts to ensure that interpretations and themes remained grounded in participants’ accounts. Considering autism is known as a ‘spectrum’ condition due to a wide variation in type and severity of presentation and individual experience, the participants in this study were predominantly identified as ‘high functioning’ although this is a controversial label, not adequately describing the social difficulties associated with autism. As the qualitative findings of this study are limited to the participants and cannot be generalised, they are limited also in shining a lens on the diversity of functioning in individuals with autism. Similarly, there were limitations due to the homogenous nature of the group of individuals with a diagnosis of ASD included in the current study. Further studies could seek intersectional experiences providing information on diversities of autistic identities and positioning e.g., gender, sexuality, ethnicity, race, social, and educational/intellectual status.
Publisher’s Note
Springer Nature remains neutral with regard to jurisdictional claims in published maps and institutional affiliations.