Skip to main content
Top
Gepubliceerd in: Psychological Research 8/2022

11-06-2021 | Original Article

Thematic and other semantic relations central to abstract (and concrete) concepts

Auteurs: Melissa Troyer, Ken McRae

Gepubliceerd in: Psychological Research | Uitgave 8/2022

Log in om toegang te krijgen
share
DELEN

Deel dit onderdeel of sectie (kopieer de link)

  • Optie A:
    Klik op de rechtermuisknop op de link en selecteer de optie “linkadres kopiëren”
  • Optie B:
    Deel de link per e-mail

Abstract

In this article, we discuss multiple types of meaningful (semantic) relations underlying abstract (as compared to concrete) concepts. We adopt the viewpoint that words act as cues to meaning (Elman in Ment Lexicon 6(1):1–34, 2011; Lupyan and Lewis in Lang Cogn Neurosci 34(10):1319–1337, 2019), which is dependent on the dynamic contents of a comprehender’s mental model of the situation. This view foregrounds the importance of both linguistic and real-world context as individuals make sense of words, flexibly access relevant knowledge, and understand described events and situations. We discuss theories of, and experimental work on, abstract concepts through the lens of the importance of thematic and other semantic relations. We then tie these findings to the sentence processing literature in which such meaningful relations within sentential contexts are often experimentally manipulated. In this literature, some specific classes/types of abstract words have been studied, although not comprehensively, and with limited connection to the literature on knowledge underlying abstract concepts reviewed herein. We conclude by arguing that the ways in which humans understand relatively more abstract concepts, in particular, can be informed by the careful study of words presented not in isolation, but rather in situational and linguistic contexts, and as a function of individual differences in knowledge, goals, and beliefs.
Literatuur
go back to reference Altarriba, J., Bauer, L. M., & Benvenuto, C. (1999). Concreteness, context availability, and imageability ratings and word associations for abstract, concrete, and emotion words. Behavior Research Methods, Instruments, & Computers, 31(4), 578–602. CrossRef Altarriba, J., Bauer, L. M., & Benvenuto, C. (1999). Concreteness, context availability, and imageability ratings and word associations for abstract, concrete, and emotion words. Behavior Research Methods, Instruments, & Computers, 31(4), 578–602. CrossRef
go back to reference Barr, R. A., & Caplan, L. J. (1987). Category representations and their implications for category structure. Memory & Cognition, 15, 397–418.CrossRef Barr, R. A., & Caplan, L. J. (1987). Category representations and their implications for category structure. Memory & Cognition, 15, 397–418.CrossRef
go back to reference Barsalou, L. W., Dutriaux, L., & Scheepers, C. (2018). Moving beyond the distinction between concrete and abstract concepts. Philosophical Transactions of the Royal Society B, 373, 20170144.CrossRef Barsalou, L. W., Dutriaux, L., & Scheepers, C. (2018). Moving beyond the distinction between concrete and abstract concepts. Philosophical Transactions of the Royal Society B, 373, 20170144.CrossRef
go back to reference Barsalou, L. W., & Wiemer-Hastings, K. (2005). Situating abstract concepts. In D. Pecher & R. A. Zwaan (Eds.), Grounding cognition: The role of perception and action in memory, language, and thinking (pp. 129–163). Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.CrossRef Barsalou, L. W., & Wiemer-Hastings, K. (2005). Situating abstract concepts. In D. Pecher & R. A. Zwaan (Eds.), Grounding cognition: The role of perception and action in memory, language, and thinking (pp. 129–163). Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.CrossRef
go back to reference Beilock, S. L., Lyons, I. M., Mattarella-Micke, A., & NusbaumSmall, H. C. S. L. (2008). Sports experience changes the neural processing of action language. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences, 105(36), 13269–13273.CrossRef Beilock, S. L., Lyons, I. M., Mattarella-Micke, A., & NusbaumSmall, H. C. S. L. (2008). Sports experience changes the neural processing of action language. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences, 105(36), 13269–13273.CrossRef
go back to reference Bicknell, K., Elman, J., Hare, M., McRae, K., & Kutas, M. (2010). Effects of event knowledge in processing verbal arguments. Journal of Memory and Language, 63, 489–505.PubMedPubMedCentralCrossRef Bicknell, K., Elman, J., Hare, M., McRae, K., & Kutas, M. (2010). Effects of event knowledge in processing verbal arguments. Journal of Memory and Language, 63, 489–505.PubMedPubMedCentralCrossRef
go back to reference Borghi, A. M., Binkofski, F., Castelfranchi, C., Cimatti, F., Scorolli, C., & Tummolini, L. (2017). The challenge of abstract concepts. Psychological Bulletin, 143(3), 263–292.PubMedCrossRef Borghi, A. M., Binkofski, F., Castelfranchi, C., Cimatti, F., Scorolli, C., & Tummolini, L. (2017). The challenge of abstract concepts. Psychological Bulletin, 143(3), 263–292.PubMedCrossRef
go back to reference Borghi, A. M., Barca, L., Binkofski, F., Castelfranchi, C., Pezzulo, G., & Tummolini, L. (2019). Words as social tools: Language, sociality and inner grounding in abstract concepts. Physics of Life Reviews, 29, 120–153.PubMedCrossRef Borghi, A. M., Barca, L., Binkofski, F., Castelfranchi, C., Pezzulo, G., & Tummolini, L. (2019). Words as social tools: Language, sociality and inner grounding in abstract concepts. Physics of Life Reviews, 29, 120–153.PubMedCrossRef
go back to reference Brysbaert, M., Warriner, A. B., & Kuperman, V. (2014). Concreteness ratings for 40 thousand generally known English word lemmas. Behavioral Research, 46, 904–911.CrossRef Brysbaert, M., Warriner, A. B., & Kuperman, V. (2014). Concreteness ratings for 40 thousand generally known English word lemmas. Behavioral Research, 46, 904–911.CrossRef
go back to reference Burgoon, E. M., Henderson, M. D., & Markman, A. R. (2013). There are many ways to see the forest for the trees: A tour guide for abstraction. Perspectives on Psychological Science, 8(5), 501–520.PubMedCrossRef Burgoon, E. M., Henderson, M. D., & Markman, A. R. (2013). There are many ways to see the forest for the trees: A tour guide for abstraction. Perspectives on Psychological Science, 8(5), 501–520.PubMedCrossRef
go back to reference Collins, A. M., & Quillian, M. R. (1969). Retrieval time from semantic memory. Journal of Verbal Learning and Verbal Behavior, 8(2), 240–247.CrossRef Collins, A. M., & Quillian, M. R. (1969). Retrieval time from semantic memory. Journal of Verbal Learning and Verbal Behavior, 8(2), 240–247.CrossRef
go back to reference Connell, L., & Lynott, D. (2012). Strength of perceptual experience predicts word processing performance better than concreteness or imageability. Cognition, 125, 425–465.CrossRef Connell, L., & Lynott, D. (2012). Strength of perceptual experience predicts word processing performance better than concreteness or imageability. Cognition, 125, 425–465.CrossRef
go back to reference Connell, L., Lynott, D., & Banks, B. (2018). Interoception: The forgotten modality in perceptual grounding of abstract and concrete concepts. Philosophical Transactions of the Royal Society B, 373, 20170143.CrossRef Connell, L., Lynott, D., & Banks, B. (2018). Interoception: The forgotten modality in perceptual grounding of abstract and concrete concepts. Philosophical Transactions of the Royal Society B, 373, 20170143.CrossRef
go back to reference Crutch, S. J., & Warrington, E. K. (2010). The differential dependence of abstract and concrete words upon associative and similarity-based information: Complementary sematnic interference and facilitation effects. Cognitive Neuropsychology, 27(1), 46–71.PubMedCrossRef Crutch, S. J., & Warrington, E. K. (2010). The differential dependence of abstract and concrete words upon associative and similarity-based information: Complementary sematnic interference and facilitation effects. Cognitive Neuropsychology, 27(1), 46–71.PubMedCrossRef
go back to reference Crutch, S. J., Williams, P., Ridgway, G. R., & Borgenicht, L. (2012). The role of polarity in antonym and synonym conceptual knowledge: Evidence from stroke aphasia and multidimensional ratings of abstract words. Neuropsychologia, 50, 2636–2644.PubMedCrossRef Crutch, S. J., Williams, P., Ridgway, G. R., & Borgenicht, L. (2012). The role of polarity in antonym and synonym conceptual knowledge: Evidence from stroke aphasia and multidimensional ratings of abstract words. Neuropsychologia, 50, 2636–2644.PubMedCrossRef
go back to reference Davis, C. P., & Yee, E. (2018). Features, labels, space, and time: Factors supporting taxonomic relationships in the anterior temporal lobe and thematic relationships in the angular gyrus. Language, Cognition, and Neuroscience, 34(10), 1347–1357.CrossRef Davis, C. P., & Yee, E. (2018). Features, labels, space, and time: Factors supporting taxonomic relationships in the anterior temporal lobe and thematic relationships in the angular gyrus. Language, Cognition, and Neuroscience, 34(10), 1347–1357.CrossRef
go back to reference Davis, C.P., Altmann, G.T.M., & Yee, E. (2020). Situational systematicity: A role for schema in understanding the differences between abstract and concrete concepts. Cognitive Neuropsychology, 37(1–2), 142–153. Davis, C.P., Altmann, G.T.M., & Yee, E. (2020). Situational systematicity: A role for schema in understanding the differences between abstract and concrete concepts. Cognitive Neuropsychology37(1–2), 142–153.
go back to reference De Deyne, S., Navarro, D. J., & Storms, G. (2013). Better explanations of lexical and semantic cognition using networks derived from continuous rather than single-word associations. Behavioral Research, 45, 480–498.CrossRef De Deyne, S., Navarro, D. J., & Storms, G. (2013). Better explanations of lexical and semantic cognition using networks derived from continuous rather than single-word associations. Behavioral Research, 45, 480–498.CrossRef
go back to reference Duñabeitia, J. A., Avilés, A., Afonso, O., Scheepers, C., & Carreiras, M. (2009). Qualitative differences in the representation of abstract versus concrete words: Evidence from the visual-world paradigm. Cognition, 110, 284–292.PubMedCrossRef Duñabeitia, J. A., Avilés, A., Afonso, O., Scheepers, C., & Carreiras, M. (2009). Qualitative differences in the representation of abstract versus concrete words: Evidence from the visual-world paradigm. Cognition, 110, 284–292.PubMedCrossRef
go back to reference Elman, J. L., & McRae, K. (2019). A model of event knowledge. Psychological Review, 126(2), 252–291.PubMedCrossRef Elman, J. L., & McRae, K. (2019). A model of event knowledge. Psychological Review, 126(2), 252–291.PubMedCrossRef
go back to reference Estes, Z., Golonka, S., & Jones, L. L. (2011). Thematic thinking: The apprehension and consequences of thematic relations. Psychology of Learning and Motivation, 54, 249–294.CrossRef Estes, Z., Golonka, S., & Jones, L. L. (2011). Thematic thinking: The apprehension and consequences of thematic relations. Psychology of Learning and Motivation, 54, 249–294.CrossRef
go back to reference Gentner, D., & Asmuth, J. (2019). Metaphoric extension, relational categories, and abstraction. Language, Cognition, and Neuroscience, 34(10), 1298–1307.CrossRef Gentner, D., & Asmuth, J. (2019). Metaphoric extension, relational categories, and abstraction. Language, Cognition, and Neuroscience, 34(10), 1298–1307.CrossRef
go back to reference Ghio, M., Vaghi, M. M. S., & Tettamanti, M. (2013). Fine-grained semantic categorization across the abstract and concrete domains. PLoS ONE, 8(6), e67090.PubMedPubMedCentralCrossRef Ghio, M., Vaghi, M. M. S., & Tettamanti, M. (2013). Fine-grained semantic categorization across the abstract and concrete domains. PLoS ONE, 8(6), e67090.PubMedPubMedCentralCrossRef
go back to reference Gilboa, A., & Marlatte, H. (2017). Neurobiology of schemas and schema-mediated memory. Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 21, 618–631.PubMedCrossRef Gilboa, A., & Marlatte, H. (2017). Neurobiology of schemas and schema-mediated memory. Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 21, 618–631.PubMedCrossRef
go back to reference Guilbeault, D., Nadler, E. O., Chu, M., Sardo, D. R. L., Kar, A. A., & Desikan, B. S. (2020). Color associations in abstract semantic domains. Cognition, 201, 104306.PubMedCrossRef Guilbeault, D., Nadler, E. O., Chu, M., Sardo, D. R. L., Kar, A. A., & Desikan, B. S. (2020). Color associations in abstract semantic domains. Cognition, 201, 104306.PubMedCrossRef
go back to reference Hare, M., McRae, K., & Elman, J. L. (2003). Sense and structure: Meaning as a determinant of verb subcategorization preferences. Journal of Memory and Language, 48, 281–303.CrossRef Hare, M., McRae, K., & Elman, J. L. (2003). Sense and structure: Meaning as a determinant of verb subcategorization preferences. Journal of Memory and Language, 48, 281–303.CrossRef
go back to reference Hare, M., McRae, K., & Elman, J. L. (2004). Admitting that admitting verb sense into corpus analyses makes sense. Language and Cognitive Processes, 19(2), 181–224.CrossRef Hare, M., McRae, K., & Elman, J. L. (2004). Admitting that admitting verb sense into corpus analyses makes sense. Language and Cognitive Processes, 19(2), 181–224.CrossRef
go back to reference Harpaintner, M., Trumpp, N. M., & Kiefer, M. (2018). The semantic content of abstract concepts: A property listing study of 296 abstract words. Frontiers in Psychology, 9(1748), 1–16. Harpaintner, M., Trumpp, N. M., & Kiefer, M. (2018). The semantic content of abstract concepts: A property listing study of 296 abstract words. Frontiers in Psychology, 9(1748), 1–16.
go back to reference Hoenig, K., Müller, C., Herrnberger, B., Sim, E.-J., Spitzer, M., Ehret, G., & Kiefer, M. (2011). Neuroplasticity of semantic representations for musical instruments in professional musicians. NeuroImage, 56, 1714–1725.PubMedCrossRef Hoenig, K., Müller, C., Herrnberger, B., Sim, E.-J., Spitzer, M., Ehret, G., & Kiefer, M. (2011). Neuroplasticity of semantic representations for musical instruments in professional musicians. NeuroImage, 56, 1714–1725.PubMedCrossRef
go back to reference Hoffman, P., Lambon Ralph, M. A., & Rogers, T. T. (2013). Semantic diversity: A measure of semantic ambiguity based on variability in the contextual usage of words. Behavioral Research, 45, 718–730.CrossRef Hoffman, P., Lambon Ralph, M. A., & Rogers, T. T. (2013). Semantic diversity: A measure of semantic ambiguity based on variability in the contextual usage of words. Behavioral Research, 45, 718–730.CrossRef
go back to reference Hoffman, P. (2016). The meaning of ‘life’ and other abstract words: Insights from neuropsychology. Journal of Neuropsychology, 10, 317–343.PubMedCrossRef Hoffman, P. (2016). The meaning of ‘life’ and other abstract words: Insights from neuropsychology. Journal of Neuropsychology, 10, 317–343.PubMedCrossRef
go back to reference Huang, H.-W., Federmeier, K. D. (2015). Imaginative language: What event-related potentials have revealed about the nature and source of concreteness effects. Language and Linguistics 16(4): 503–515. Huang, H.-W., Federmeier, K. D. (2015). Imaginative language: What event-related potentials have revealed about the nature and source of concreteness effects. Language and Linguistics 16(4): 503–515.
go back to reference Jackendoff, R. (2002). Foundations of Language: Brain, Meaning, Grammar, and Evolution. Oxford: Oxford Press.CrossRef Jackendoff, R. (2002). Foundations of Language: Brain, Meaning, Grammar, and Evolution. Oxford: Oxford Press.CrossRef
go back to reference Jackendoff, R. (2019). Mental representations for language. In P. Hagoort (Ed.), Human Language: From Genes and Brains to Behavior (pp. 7–20). MIT Press. Jackendoff, R. (2019). Mental representations for language. In P. Hagoort (Ed.), Human Language: From Genes and Brains to Behavior (pp. 7–20). MIT Press.
go back to reference Kamide, Y., Altmann, G. T. M., & Haywood, S. L. (2003). The time-course of prediction in incremental sentence processing: Evidence from anticipatory eye movements. Journal of Memory and Language, 49, 133–159.CrossRef Kamide, Y., Altmann, G. T. M., & Haywood, S. L. (2003). The time-course of prediction in incremental sentence processing: Evidence from anticipatory eye movements. Journal of Memory and Language, 49, 133–159.CrossRef
go back to reference Kousta, S.-T., Vigliocco, G., Vinson, D. P., Andrews, M., & Del Campo, E. (2011). Journal of Experimental Psychology: General, 140(1), 14–34.CrossRef Kousta, S.-T., Vigliocco, G., Vinson, D. P., Andrews, M., & Del Campo, E. (2011). Journal of Experimental Psychology: General, 140(1), 14–34.CrossRef
go back to reference Lakoff, G., & Johnson, M. (1980). Conceptual metaphor in everyday language. The Journal of Philosophy, 77(8), 453–486.CrossRef Lakoff, G., & Johnson, M. (1980). Conceptual metaphor in everyday language. The Journal of Philosophy, 77(8), 453–486.CrossRef
go back to reference Lenci, A., Lebani, G. E., & Passaro, L. C. (2018). The emotions of abstract words: A distributional semantic analysis. Topics in Cognitive Science, 10(3), 550–572.PubMedCrossRef Lenci, A., Lebani, G. E., & Passaro, L. C. (2018). The emotions of abstract words: A distributional semantic analysis. Topics in Cognitive Science, 10(3), 550–572.PubMedCrossRef
go back to reference Lupyan, G., & Lewis, M. (2019). From words-as-mappings to words-as-cues: The role of language in semantic knowledge. Language, Cognition, and Neuroscience, 34(10), 1319–1337.CrossRef Lupyan, G., & Lewis, M. (2019). From words-as-mappings to words-as-cues: The role of language in semantic knowledge. Language, Cognition, and Neuroscience, 34(10), 1319–1337.CrossRef
go back to reference Marques, J. F., & Nunes, L. D. (2012). The contributions of language and experience to the representation of abstract and concrete words: Different weights but similar organizations. Memory and Cognition, 40, 1266–1275.PubMedCrossRef Marques, J. F., & Nunes, L. D. (2012). The contributions of language and experience to the representation of abstract and concrete words: Different weights but similar organizations. Memory and Cognition, 40, 1266–1275.PubMedCrossRef
go back to reference McRae, K., Ferretti, T. R., & Amyote, L. (1997). Thematic roles as verb-specific concepts. Language and Cognitive Processes, 12(2–3), 137–176.CrossRef McRae, K., Ferretti, T. R., & Amyote, L. (1997). Thematic roles as verb-specific concepts. Language and Cognitive Processes, 12(2–3), 137–176.CrossRef
go back to reference McRae, K., Khalkhali, S., & Hare, M. (2012). Semantic and associative relations in adolescents and young adults: Examining a tenuous dichotomy. In V. F. Reyna, S. B. Chapman, M. R. Dougherty, & J. Confrey (Eds.), The adolescent brain: Learning, reasoning, and decision making (pp. 39–66). Washington, DC: APA.CrossRef McRae, K., Khalkhali, S., & Hare, M. (2012). Semantic and associative relations in adolescents and young adults: Examining a tenuous dichotomy. In V. F. Reyna, S. B. Chapman, M. R. Dougherty, & J. Confrey (Eds.), The adolescent brain: Learning, reasoning, and decision making (pp. 39–66). Washington, DC: APA.CrossRef
go back to reference McRae, K., & Matsuki, K. (2009). People use their knowledge of common events to understand language, and do so as quickly as possible. Language and Linguistics Compass, 3(6), 1417–1429.PubMedPubMedCentralCrossRef McRae, K., & Matsuki, K. (2009). People use their knowledge of common events to understand language, and do so as quickly as possible. Language and Linguistics Compass, 3(6), 1417–1429.PubMedPubMedCentralCrossRef
go back to reference McRae, K., Nedjadrasul, D., Pau, R., & LoKing, B. P. L. (2018). Abstract concepts and pictures of real-world situations activate one another. Topics in Cognitive Science, 10, 518–532.PubMedCrossRef McRae, K., Nedjadrasul, D., Pau, R., & LoKing, B. P. L. (2018). Abstract concepts and pictures of real-world situations activate one another. Topics in Cognitive Science, 10, 518–532.PubMedCrossRef
go back to reference Metusalem, R., Kutas, M., Urbach, T. P., Hare, M., McRae, K., & Elman, J. L. (2012). Generalized event knowledge activation during online sentence comprehension. Journal of Memory and Language, 66(4), 545–567.PubMedPubMedCentralCrossRef Metusalem, R., Kutas, M., Urbach, T. P., Hare, M., McRae, K., & Elman, J. L. (2012). Generalized event knowledge activation during online sentence comprehension. Journal of Memory and Language, 66(4), 545–567.PubMedPubMedCentralCrossRef
go back to reference Mirman, D., & Graziano, K. M. (2012). Individual differences in the strength of taxonomic versus thematic relations. Journal of Experimental Psychology: General, 141(4), 601–609.CrossRef Mirman, D., & Graziano, K. M. (2012). Individual differences in the strength of taxonomic versus thematic relations. Journal of Experimental Psychology: General, 141(4), 601–609.CrossRef
go back to reference Nedjadrasul, D. (2017). Abstract and concrete concepts according to word association. Master’s thesis, University of Western Ontario. Nedjadrasul, D. (2017). Abstract and concrete concepts according to word association. Master’s thesis, University of Western Ontario.
go back to reference Nelson, D. L., McEvoy, C. L., & Dennis, S. (2000). What is free association and what does it measure? Memory & Cognition, 28(6), 887–899. Accessed 8 June 2021CrossRef Nelson, D. L., McEvoy, C. L., & Dennis, S. (2000). What is free association and what does it measure? Memory & Cognition, 28(6), 887–899. Accessed 8 June 2021CrossRef
go back to reference Nelson, K. (1983). The derivation of concepts and categories from event representations. In E. K. Scholnick (Ed.), New trends in conceptual representation: Challenges to Piaget’s theory? (pp. 129–149). Hillsdale: Erlbaum. Nelson, K. (1983). The derivation of concepts and categories from event representations. In E. K. Scholnick (Ed.), New trends in conceptual representation: Challenges to Piaget’s theory? (pp. 129–149). Hillsdale: Erlbaum.
go back to reference Norman, D. A., & Rumelhart, D. E. (1981). The LNR approach to human information processing. Cognition, 10, 235–240.PubMedCrossRef Norman, D. A., & Rumelhart, D. E. (1981). The LNR approach to human information processing. Cognition, 10, 235–240.PubMedCrossRef
go back to reference Paivio, A. (1991). Dual coding theory: Retrospect and current status. Canadian Journal of Psychology, 45, 255–287.CrossRef Paivio, A. (1991). Dual coding theory: Retrospect and current status. Canadian Journal of Psychology, 45, 255–287.CrossRef
go back to reference Paivio, A., Yuille, J. C., & Madigan, S. A. (1968). Concreteness, imagery, and meaningfulness values for 925 nouns. Journal of Experimental Psychology Monograph Supplement, 76(1), 1–25.CrossRef Paivio, A., Yuille, J. C., & Madigan, S. A. (1968). Concreteness, imagery, and meaningfulness values for 925 nouns. Journal of Experimental Psychology Monograph Supplement, 76(1), 1–25.CrossRef
go back to reference Panero, M. E., Weisberg, D. S., Black, J., Goldstein, T. R., Barnes, J. L., Brownell, H., & Winner, E. (2016). Does reading a single passage of literary fiction really improve theory of mind? An attempt at replication. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 111(5), e46–e54.PubMedCrossRef Panero, M. E., Weisberg, D. S., Black, J., Goldstein, T. R., Barnes, J. L., Brownell, H., & Winner, E. (2016). Does reading a single passage of literary fiction really improve theory of mind? An attempt at replication. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 111(5), e46–e54.PubMedCrossRef
go back to reference Primativo, S., Reilly, J., & Crutch, S. (2017). Abstract conceptual feature ratings predict gaze within written word arrays: Evidence from a visual wor(l)d paradigm. Cognitive Science, 41(3), 659–685.PubMedCrossRef Primativo, S., Reilly, J., & Crutch, S. (2017). Abstract conceptual feature ratings predict gaze within written word arrays: Evidence from a visual wor(l)d paradigm. Cognitive Science, 41(3), 659–685.PubMedCrossRef
go back to reference Roversi, C., Borghi, A. M., & Tummolini, L. (2013). A marriage is an artefact and not a walk that we take together: An experimental study on the categorization of artefacts. Review of Philosophy and Psychology, 4, 527–542.CrossRef Roversi, C., Borghi, A. M., & Tummolini, L. (2013). A marriage is an artefact and not a walk that we take together: An experimental study on the categorization of artefacts. Review of Philosophy and Psychology, 4, 527–542.CrossRef
go back to reference Schank, R. C., & Abelson, R. P. (1977). Scripts, plans, goals, and understanding: An inquiry into human knowledge structures. New Jersey: Erlbaum. Schank, R. C., & Abelson, R. P. (1977). Scripts, plans, goals, and understanding: An inquiry into human knowledge structures. New Jersey: Erlbaum.
go back to reference Schwanenflugel, P.J. (2013). Why are abstract concepts hard to understand? In P.J. Schwanenflugel (Ed.), The psychology of word meanings (pp. 235–262). Psychology Press, Hillsdale, NJ. Schwanenflugel, P.J. (2013). Why are abstract concepts hard to understand? In P.J. Schwanenflugel (Ed.), The psychology of word meanings (pp. 235–262). Psychology Press, Hillsdale, NJ.
go back to reference Schwanenflugel, P. J., & Shoben, E. J. (1983). Differential effects in the comprehension of abstract and concrete verbal materials. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition, 9(1), 82–102. Schwanenflugel, P. J., & Shoben, E. J. (1983). Differential effects in the comprehension of abstract and concrete verbal materials. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition, 9(1), 82–102.
go back to reference Schwanenflugel, P. J., & Stowe, R. W. (1989). Context availability and the processing of abstract and concrete words in sentences. Reading Research Quarterly, 24(1), 114–126.CrossRef Schwanenflugel, P. J., & Stowe, R. W. (1989). Context availability and the processing of abstract and concrete words in sentences. Reading Research Quarterly, 24(1), 114–126.CrossRef
go back to reference Setti, A. & Caramelli, N. (2005). Different domains in abstract concepts. In B. Bara, L. Barsalou, & M. Bucciarelli (Eds.), Proceedings of the 27th annual meeting of the cognitive science society. Setti, A. & Caramelli, N. (2005). Different domains in abstract concepts. In B. Bara, L. Barsalou, & M. Bucciarelli (Eds.), Proceedings of the 27th annual meeting of the cognitive science society.
go back to reference Troche, J., Crutch, S., & Reilly, J. (2014). Clustering, hierarchical organization, and the topography of abstract and concrete nouns. Frontiers in Psychology, 5(360), 1–10. Troche, J., Crutch, S., & Reilly, J. (2014). Clustering, hierarchical organization, and the topography of abstract and concrete nouns. Frontiers in Psychology, 5(360), 1–10.
go back to reference Troyer, M., & Kutas, M. (2020). To catch a Snitch: Brain potentials reveal variability in the functional organization of (fictional) world knowledge during reading. Journal of Memory and Language, 113, 104111.PubMedPubMedCentralCrossRef Troyer, M., & Kutas, M. (2020). To catch a Snitch: Brain potentials reveal variability in the functional organization of (fictional) world knowledge during reading. Journal of Memory and Language, 113, 104111.PubMedPubMedCentralCrossRef
go back to reference Urbach, T. P., DeLong, K. A., & Kutas, M. (2015). Quantifiers are incrementally interpreted in context, more than less. Journal of Memory and Language, 83, 79–96.PubMedPubMedCentralCrossRef Urbach, T. P., DeLong, K. A., & Kutas, M. (2015). Quantifiers are incrementally interpreted in context, more than less. Journal of Memory and Language, 83, 79–96.PubMedPubMedCentralCrossRef
go back to reference Van Berkum, J. J. A., Holleman, B., Nieuwland, M., Otten, M., & Murre, J. (2009). Right or wrong? The brain’s fast response to morally objectionable statements. Psychological Science, 20(9), 1092–1099.PubMedCrossRef Van Berkum, J. J. A., Holleman, B., Nieuwland, M., Otten, M., & Murre, J. (2009). Right or wrong? The brain’s fast response to morally objectionable statements. Psychological Science, 20(9), 1092–1099.PubMedCrossRef
go back to reference Van Petten, C., & Kutas, M. (1990). Interactions between sentence context and word frequency in event-related brain potentials. Memory and Cognition, 18(4), 380–393.PubMedCrossRef Van Petten, C., & Kutas, M. (1990). Interactions between sentence context and word frequency in event-related brain potentials. Memory and Cognition, 18(4), 380–393.PubMedCrossRef
go back to reference Venhuizen, N. J., Crocker, M. W., & Brouwer, H. (2019). Expectation-based comprehension: Modeling the interaction of world knowledge and linguistic experience. Discourse Processes, 56(3), 229–255.CrossRef Venhuizen, N. J., Crocker, M. W., & Brouwer, H. (2019). Expectation-based comprehension: Modeling the interaction of world knowledge and linguistic experience. Discourse Processes, 56(3), 229–255.CrossRef
go back to reference Villani, C., Lugli, L., Liuzza, M. T., & Borghi, A. M. (2019). Varieties of abstract concepts and their multiple dimensions. Language and Cognition, 11, 403–430.CrossRef Villani, C., Lugli, L., Liuzza, M. T., & Borghi, A. M. (2019). Varieties of abstract concepts and their multiple dimensions. Language and Cognition, 11, 403–430.CrossRef
go back to reference Wattenmaker, W. D., & Shoben, E. J. (1987). Context and the recallability of concrete and abstract sentences. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition, 13(1), 140–150. Wattenmaker, W. D., & Shoben, E. J. (1987). Context and the recallability of concrete and abstract sentences. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition, 13(1), 140–150.
go back to reference Wiemer-Hastings, K., & Xu, X. (2005). Content differences for abstract and concrete concepts. Cognitive Science, 29, 719–736.CrossRef Wiemer-Hastings, K., & Xu, X. (2005). Content differences for abstract and concrete concepts. Cognitive Science, 29, 719–736.CrossRef
go back to reference Wilson-Mendenhall, C. D., Simmons, W. K., Martin, A., & Barsalou, L. W. (2013). Contextual processing of abstract concepts reveals neural representations of nonlinguistic semantic content. Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience, 25(6), 920–935.PubMedPubMedCentralCrossRef Wilson-Mendenhall, C. D., Simmons, W. K., Martin, A., & Barsalou, L. W. (2013). Contextual processing of abstract concepts reveals neural representations of nonlinguistic semantic content. Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience, 25(6), 920–935.PubMedPubMedCentralCrossRef
go back to reference Wittenberg, E., Paczynski, M., Wiese, H., Jackendoff, R., & Kuperberg, G. (2014). The difference between “giving a rose” and “giving a kiss”: Sustained neural activity to the light verb construction. Journal of Memory and Language, 73, 31–42.PubMedPubMedCentralCrossRef Wittenberg, E., Paczynski, M., Wiese, H., Jackendoff, R., & Kuperberg, G. (2014). The difference between “giving a rose” and “giving a kiss”: Sustained neural activity to the light verb construction. Journal of Memory and Language, 73, 31–42.PubMedPubMedCentralCrossRef
go back to reference Wittenberg, E., & Snedeker, J. (2014). It takes two to kiss, but does it take three to give a kiss? Categorization based on thematic roles. Language, Cognition, and Neuroscience, 29(5), 635–641.CrossRef Wittenberg, E., & Snedeker, J. (2014). It takes two to kiss, but does it take three to give a kiss? Categorization based on thematic roles. Language, Cognition, and Neuroscience, 29(5), 635–641.CrossRef
go back to reference Wu, L., & Barsalou, L. W. (2009). Perceptual similarity in conceptual combination: Evidence from property generation. Acta Psychologica, 132, 173–189.PubMedCrossRef Wu, L., & Barsalou, L. W. (2009). Perceptual similarity in conceptual combination: Evidence from property generation. Acta Psychologica, 132, 173–189.PubMedCrossRef
go back to reference Yee, E., Jones, M.N., & McRae, K. (2017). Semantic Memory. In J.T. Wixted & S. Thompson-Schill (Eds.), The Stevens’ Handbook of Experimental Psychology and Cognitive Neuroscience (4th Edition, Volume 3: Language and Thought). New York: Wiley. Yee, E., Jones, M.N., & McRae, K. (2017). Semantic Memory. In J.T. Wixted & S. Thompson-Schill (Eds.), The Stevens’ Handbook of Experimental Psychology and Cognitive Neuroscience (4th Edition, Volume 3: Language and Thought). New York: Wiley.
go back to reference Yee, E., & Thompson-Schill, S. L. (2016). Putting concepts into context. Psychonomic Bulletin and Review, 23, 1015–1027.PubMedCrossRef Yee, E., & Thompson-Schill, S. L. (2016). Putting concepts into context. Psychonomic Bulletin and Review, 23, 1015–1027.PubMedCrossRef
go back to reference Zdrazilova, L., Sidhu, D. M., & Pexman, P. M. (2018). Communicating abstract meaning: Concepts revealed in words and gestures. Philosophical Transactions of the Royal Society B, 373, 20170138.CrossRef Zdrazilova, L., Sidhu, D. M., & Pexman, P. M. (2018). Communicating abstract meaning: Concepts revealed in words and gestures. Philosophical Transactions of the Royal Society B, 373, 20170138.CrossRef
go back to reference Zwaan, R. A., & Radvansky, G. A. (1998). Situation models in language comprehension and memory. Psychological Bulletin, 123(2), 162–185.PubMedCrossRef Zwaan, R. A., & Radvansky, G. A. (1998). Situation models in language comprehension and memory. Psychological Bulletin, 123(2), 162–185.PubMedCrossRef
Metagegevens
Titel
Thematic and other semantic relations central to abstract (and concrete) concepts
Auteurs
Melissa Troyer
Ken McRae
Publicatiedatum
11-06-2021
Uitgeverij
Springer Berlin Heidelberg
Gepubliceerd in
Psychological Research / Uitgave 8/2022
Print ISSN: 0340-0727
Elektronisch ISSN: 1430-2772
DOI
https://doi.org/10.1007/s00426-021-01484-8

Andere artikelen Uitgave 8/2022

Psychological Research 8/2022 Naar de uitgave