Skip to main content
Top
Gepubliceerd in: Psychological Research 8/2022

10-11-2020 | Original Article

Words have a weight: language as a source of inner grounding and flexibility in abstract concepts

Auteurs: Guy Dove, Laura Barca, Luca Tummolini, Anna M. Borghi

Gepubliceerd in: Psychological Research | Uitgave 8/2022

Log in om toegang te krijgen
share
DELEN

Deel dit onderdeel of sectie (kopieer de link)

  • Optie A:
    Klik op de rechtermuisknop op de link en selecteer de optie “linkadres kopiëren”
  • Optie B:
    Deel de link per e-mail

Abstract

The role played by language in our cognitive lives is a topic at the centre of contemporary debates in cognitive (neuro)science. In this paper we illustrate and compare two theories that offer embodied explanations of this role: the WAT (words as social tools) and the LENS (language is an embodied neuroenhancement and scaffold) theories. WAT and LENS differ from other current proposals, because they connect the impact of the neurologically realized language system on our cognition to the ways in which language shapes our interaction with the physical and social environment. Examining these theories together, their tenets and supporting evidence, sharpens our understanding of each, but also contributes to a better understanding of the contribution that language might make to the acquisition, representation and use of abstract concepts. Here we focus on how language provides a source of inner grounding, especially metacognition and inner speech, and supports the flexibility of our thought. Overall, the paper outlines a promising research program focused on the importance of language to abstract concepts within the context of a flexible, multimodal, and multilevel conception of embodied cognition.
Literatuur
go back to reference Alderson-Day, B., Weis, S., McCarthy-Jones, S., Moseley, P., Smailes, D., & Fernyhough, C. (2016). The brain’s conversation with itself: neural substrates of dialogic inner speech. Social Cognitive and Affective Neuroscience, 11(1), 110–120.PubMedCrossRef Alderson-Day, B., Weis, S., McCarthy-Jones, S., Moseley, P., Smailes, D., & Fernyhough, C. (2016). The brain’s conversation with itself: neural substrates of dialogic inner speech. Social Cognitive and Affective Neuroscience, 11(1), 110–120.PubMedCrossRef
go back to reference Andrews, M., Frank, S., & Vigliocco, G. (2014). Reconciling embodied and distributional accounts of meaning in language. Topics in Cognitive Science, 6, 359–370.PubMedCrossRef Andrews, M., Frank, S., & Vigliocco, G. (2014). Reconciling embodied and distributional accounts of meaning in language. Topics in Cognitive Science, 6, 359–370.PubMedCrossRef
go back to reference Aristotle. (1995). Aristotle: Selections (T. Irwin & G. Fine, Trans). Indianapolis: Hackett. Aristotle. (1995). Aristotle: Selections (T. Irwin & G. Fine, Trans). Indianapolis: Hackett.
go back to reference Astington, J. W., & Jenkins, J. M. (1999). A longitudinal study of the relation between language and theory-of-mind development. Developmental Psychology, 35(5), 1311–1320.PubMedCrossRef Astington, J. W., & Jenkins, J. M. (1999). A longitudinal study of the relation between language and theory-of-mind development. Developmental Psychology, 35(5), 1311–1320.PubMedCrossRef
go back to reference Badre, D., & Wagner, A. D. (2007). Left ventrolateral prefrontal cortex and the cognitive control of memory. Neuropsychologia, 45, 2883–2901.PubMedCrossRef Badre, D., & Wagner, A. D. (2007). Left ventrolateral prefrontal cortex and the cognitive control of memory. Neuropsychologia, 45, 2883–2901.PubMedCrossRef
go back to reference Barca, L., Cornelissen, P., Simpson, M., Urooj, U., Woods, W., & Ellis, A. W. (2011). The neural basis of the right visual field advantage in reading: An MEG analysis using virtual electrodes. Brain & Language, 118, 53–71.CrossRef Barca, L., Cornelissen, P., Simpson, M., Urooj, U., Woods, W., & Ellis, A. W. (2011). The neural basis of the right visual field advantage in reading: An MEG analysis using virtual electrodes. Brain & Language, 118, 53–71.CrossRef
go back to reference Barca, L., Mazzuca, C., & Borghi, A. M. (2017). Pacifier overuse and conceptual relations of abstract and emotional concepts. Frontiers in psychology, 8, 2014.PubMedPubMedCentralCrossRef Barca, L., Mazzuca, C., & Borghi, A. M. (2017). Pacifier overuse and conceptual relations of abstract and emotional concepts. Frontiers in psychology, 8, 2014.PubMedPubMedCentralCrossRef
go back to reference Barca, L., Mazzuca, C., & Borghi, A. M. (2020). Overusing the pacifier during infancy sets a footprint on abstract words processing. Journal of Child Language., 47, 1084–1099.PubMedCrossRef Barca, L., Mazzuca, C., & Borghi, A. M. (2020). Overusing the pacifier during infancy sets a footprint on abstract words processing. Journal of Child Language., 47, 1084–1099.PubMedCrossRef
go back to reference Barsalou, L. W. (1993). Structure, flexibility, and linguistic vagary in concepts: manifestations of a compositional system of perceptual symbols. Theories of memory (pp. 29–101). New Jersey: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates. Barsalou, L. W. (1993). Structure, flexibility, and linguistic vagary in concepts: manifestations of a compositional system of perceptual symbols. Theories of memory (pp. 29–101). New Jersey: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates.
go back to reference Barsalou, L. W. (2016). On staying grounded and avoiding quixotic dead ends. Psychonomic Bulletin & Review, 23(4), 1122–1142.CrossRef Barsalou, L. W. (2016). On staying grounded and avoiding quixotic dead ends. Psychonomic Bulletin & Review, 23(4), 1122–1142.CrossRef
go back to reference Barsalou, L. W., Dutriaux, L., & Scheepers, C. (2018). Moving beyond the distinction between concrete and abstract concepts. Philosophical Transactions of the Royal Society B: Biological Sciences, 373(1752), 20170144.CrossRef Barsalou, L. W., Dutriaux, L., & Scheepers, C. (2018). Moving beyond the distinction between concrete and abstract concepts. Philosophical Transactions of the Royal Society B: Biological Sciences, 373(1752), 20170144.CrossRef
go back to reference Barsalou, L. W., Santos, A., Simmons, W. K., & Wilson, C. D. (2008). Language and simulation in conceptual processing. Symbols, embodiment, and meaning, 245–283. Barsalou, L. W., Santos, A., Simmons, W. K., & Wilson, C. D. (2008). Language and simulation in conceptual processing. Symbols, embodiment, and meaning, 245–283.
go back to reference Barsalou, L. W., & Wiemer-Hastings, K. (2005). Situating abstract concepts. In D. Pecher & R. Zwaan (Eds.), Grounding cognition: The role of perception and action in memory, language, and thought (pp. 129–163). Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.CrossRef Barsalou, L. W., & Wiemer-Hastings, K. (2005). Situating abstract concepts. In D. Pecher & R. Zwaan (Eds.), Grounding cognition: The role of perception and action in memory, language, and thought (pp. 129–163). Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.CrossRef
go back to reference Bastian, M., Lerique, S., Adam, V., Franklin, M. S., Schooler, J. W., & Sackur, J. (2017). Language facilitates introspection: Verbal mind-wandering has privileged access to consciousness. Consciousness and Cognition, 49, 86–97.PubMedCrossRef Bastian, M., Lerique, S., Adam, V., Franklin, M. S., Schooler, J. W., & Sackur, J. (2017). Language facilitates introspection: Verbal mind-wandering has privileged access to consciousness. Consciousness and Cognition, 49, 86–97.PubMedCrossRef
go back to reference Bermúdez, J. L. (2018). Inner Speech, determinacy, and thinking consciously about thoughts. Inner speech: new voices (p. 199). Oxford: Oxford University Press. Bermúdez, J. L. (2018). Inner Speech, determinacy, and thinking consciously about thoughts. Inner speech: new voices (p. 199). Oxford: Oxford University Press.
go back to reference Binder, J. R. (2007). Effects of word imageability on semantic access: Neuroimaging studies. In J. Hart & M. A. Kraut (Eds.), Neural Basis of Semantic Memory (pp. 149–181). Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.CrossRef Binder, J. R. (2007). Effects of word imageability on semantic access: Neuroimaging studies. In J. Hart & M. A. Kraut (Eds.), Neural Basis of Semantic Memory (pp. 149–181). Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.CrossRef
go back to reference Binder, J. R. (2016). In defense of abstract conceptual representations. Psychonomic Bulletin & Review, 23(4), 1096–1108.CrossRef Binder, J. R. (2016). In defense of abstract conceptual representations. Psychonomic Bulletin & Review, 23(4), 1096–1108.CrossRef
go back to reference Binder, J. R., Desai, R. H., Graves, W. W., & Conant, L. L. (2009). Where is the semantic system? A critical review and meta-analysis of 120 functional neuroimaging studies. Cerebral Cortex, 19, 2767–2796.PubMedPubMedCentralCrossRef Binder, J. R., Desai, R. H., Graves, W. W., & Conant, L. L. (2009). Where is the semantic system? A critical review and meta-analysis of 120 functional neuroimaging studies. Cerebral Cortex, 19, 2767–2796.PubMedPubMedCentralCrossRef
go back to reference Binder, J. R., Westbury, C. F., McKiernan, K. A., Possing, E. T., & Medler, D. A. (2005). Distinct brain systems for processing concrete and abstract concepts. Journal of cognitive neuroscience, 17(6), 905–917.PubMedCrossRef Binder, J. R., Westbury, C. F., McKiernan, K. A., Possing, E. T., & Medler, D. A. (2005). Distinct brain systems for processing concrete and abstract concepts. Journal of cognitive neuroscience, 17(6), 905–917.PubMedCrossRef
go back to reference Bjørndahl, J. S., Fusaroli, R., Østergaard, S., & Tylén, K. (2014). Thinking together with material representations: joint epistemic actions in creative problem solving. Cognitive Semiotics, 7(1), 103–123.CrossRef Bjørndahl, J. S., Fusaroli, R., Østergaard, S., & Tylén, K. (2014). Thinking together with material representations: joint epistemic actions in creative problem solving. Cognitive Semiotics, 7(1), 103–123.CrossRef
go back to reference Blei, D. M., Ng, A. Y., & Jordan, M. I. (2003). Latent Dirichlet Allocation. Journal of Machine Learning Research, 3, 993–1022. Blei, D. M., Ng, A. Y., & Jordan, M. I. (2003). Latent Dirichlet Allocation. Journal of Machine Learning Research, 3, 993–1022.
go back to reference Bonner, M. F., Vesely, L., Price, C., Anderson, C., Richmond, L., Farag, C., et al. (2009). Reversal of the concreteness effect in semantic dementia. Cognitive Neuropsychology, 26, 568–579.PubMedPubMedCentralCrossRef Bonner, M. F., Vesely, L., Price, C., Anderson, C., Richmond, L., Farag, C., et al. (2009). Reversal of the concreteness effect in semantic dementia. Cognitive Neuropsychology, 26, 568–579.PubMedPubMedCentralCrossRef
go back to reference Borghi, A. M. (2019). Linguistic relativity and abstract words. Paradigmi, 37(3), 429–448. Borghi, A. M. (2019). Linguistic relativity and abstract words. Paradigmi, 37(3), 429–448.
go back to reference Borghi, A.M. (2020). A future of words: language and the challenge of abstract concepts. Journal of Cognition (under review). Borghi, A.M. (2020). A future of words: language and the challenge of abstract concepts. Journal of Cognition (under review).
go back to reference Borghi, A. M., Barca, L., Binkofski, F., Castelfranchi, C., Pezzulo, G., & Tummolini, L. (2019a). Words as social tools: Language, sociality and inner grounding in abstract concepts. Physics of Life Reviews, 29, 120–153.PubMedCrossRef Borghi, A. M., Barca, L., Binkofski, F., Castelfranchi, C., Pezzulo, G., & Tummolini, L. (2019a). Words as social tools: Language, sociality and inner grounding in abstract concepts. Physics of Life Reviews, 29, 120–153.PubMedCrossRef
go back to reference Borghi, A. M., Barca, L., Binkofski, F., Castelfranchi, C., Pezzulo, G., & Tummolini, L. (2019b). Words as social tools: Flexibility, situatedness, language and sociality in abstract concepts. Reply to comments on “Words as social tools: Language, sociality and inner grounding in abstract concepts.” Physics of Life Reviews, 29, 178–184.PubMedCrossRef Borghi, A. M., Barca, L., Binkofski, F., Castelfranchi, C., Pezzulo, G., & Tummolini, L. (2019b). Words as social tools: Flexibility, situatedness, language and sociality in abstract concepts. Reply to comments on “Words as social tools: Language, sociality and inner grounding in abstract concepts.” Physics of Life Reviews, 29, 178–184.PubMedCrossRef
go back to reference Borghi, A. M., & Binkofski, F. (2014). Words As social Tools: An embodied view on abstract concepts. SpringerBriefs in Cognition series. New York: Springer.CrossRef Borghi, A. M., & Binkofski, F. (2014). Words As social Tools: An embodied view on abstract concepts. SpringerBriefs in Cognition series. New York: Springer.CrossRef
go back to reference Borghi, A. M., Binkofski, F., Castelfranchi, C., Cimatti, F., Scorolli, C., & Tummolini, L. (2017). The challenge of abstract concepts. Psychological Bulletin, 143(3), 263.PubMedCrossRef Borghi, A. M., Binkofski, F., Castelfranchi, C., Cimatti, F., Scorolli, C., & Tummolini, L. (2017). The challenge of abstract concepts. Psychological Bulletin, 143(3), 263.PubMedCrossRef
go back to reference Borghi, A. M., Capirci, O., Gianfreda, G., & Volterra, V. (2014). The body and the fading away of abstract concepts and words: a sign language analysis. Frontiers in Psychology, 5, 811.PubMedPubMedCentralCrossRef Borghi, A. M., Capirci, O., Gianfreda, G., & Volterra, V. (2014). The body and the fading away of abstract concepts and words: a sign language analysis. Frontiers in Psychology, 5, 811.PubMedPubMedCentralCrossRef
go back to reference Borghi, A. M., & Cimatti, F. (2009). Words as tools and the problem of abstract word meanings. In: Proceedings of the annual meeting of the cognitive science society (Vol. 31, No. 31) Borghi, A. M., & Cimatti, F. (2009). Words as tools and the problem of abstract word meanings. In: Proceedings of the annual meeting of the cognitive science society (Vol. 31, No. 31)
go back to reference Borghi, A. M., Flumini, A., Cimatti, F., Marocco, D., & Scorolli, C. (2011). Manipulating objects and telling words: a study on concrete and abstract words acquisition. Frontiers in Psychology, 2, 15.PubMedPubMedCentralCrossRef Borghi, A. M., Flumini, A., Cimatti, F., Marocco, D., & Scorolli, C. (2011). Manipulating objects and telling words: a study on concrete and abstract words acquisition. Frontiers in Psychology, 2, 15.PubMedPubMedCentralCrossRef
go back to reference Borghi, F., & Tummolini, . (2020). Abstract concepts, metacognition and social metacognition. In M. D. Robinson & L. E. Thomas (Eds.), Embodied psychology: thinking, feeling, and acting. New York: Springer. ((Submitted)). Borghi, F., & Tummolini, . (2020). Abstract concepts, metacognition and social metacognition. In M. D. Robinson & L. E. Thomas (Eds.), Embodied psychology: thinking, feeling, and acting. New York: Springer. ((Submitted)).
go back to reference Boroditsky, L. (2009). How does our language shape the way we think. What’s next, pp 116–129 Boroditsky, L. (2009). How does our language shape the way we think. What’s next, pp 116–129
go back to reference Boutonnet, B., & Lupyan, G. (2015). Words jump-start vision: A label advantage in object recognition. Journal of Neuroscience, 35(24), 9329–9335.PubMedCrossRef Boutonnet, B., & Lupyan, G. (2015). Words jump-start vision: A label advantage in object recognition. Journal of Neuroscience, 35(24), 9329–9335.PubMedCrossRef
go back to reference Buckner, C. (2018). Empiricism without magic: transformational abstraction in deep convolutional neural networks. Synthese, 195, 5339–5372.CrossRef Buckner, C. (2018). Empiricism without magic: transformational abstraction in deep convolutional neural networks. Synthese, 195, 5339–5372.CrossRef
go back to reference Carruthers, P. (2018). The causes and contents of inner speech. In: Inner Speech: New Voices Carruthers, P. (2018). The causes and contents of inner speech. In: Inner Speech: New Voices
go back to reference Casasanto, D., & Boroditsky, L. (2008). Time in the mind: Using space to think about time. Cognition, 106(2), 579–593.PubMedCrossRef Casasanto, D., & Boroditsky, L. (2008). Time in the mind: Using space to think about time. Cognition, 106(2), 579–593.PubMedCrossRef
go back to reference Catricalà, E., Della Rosa, P. A., Plebani, V., Vigliocco, G., & Cappa, S. F. (2014). Abstract and concrete categories? Evidences from neurodegenerative diseases. Neuropsychologia, 64, 271–281.PubMedCrossRef Catricalà, E., Della Rosa, P. A., Plebani, V., Vigliocco, G., & Cappa, S. F. (2014). Abstract and concrete categories? Evidences from neurodegenerative diseases. Neuropsychologia, 64, 271–281.PubMedCrossRef
go back to reference Christoff, K., Irving, Z. C., Fox, K. C., Spreng, R. N., & Andrews-Hanna, J. R. (2016). Mind-wandering as spontaneous thought: a dynamic framework. Nature Reviews Neuroscience, 17(11), 718.PubMedCrossRef Christoff, K., Irving, Z. C., Fox, K. C., Spreng, R. N., & Andrews-Hanna, J. R. (2016). Mind-wandering as spontaneous thought: a dynamic framework. Nature Reviews Neuroscience, 17(11), 718.PubMedCrossRef
go back to reference Ciaramelli, E., & Treves, A. (2019). A mind free to wander: Neural and computational constraints on spontaneous thought. Frontiers in psychology, 10, 39.PubMedPubMedCentralCrossRef Ciaramelli, E., & Treves, A. (2019). A mind free to wander: Neural and computational constraints on spontaneous thought. Frontiers in psychology, 10, 39.PubMedPubMedCentralCrossRef
go back to reference Clark, A. (1998). Magic words: how language augments human computation. In P. Carruthers & J. Boucher (Eds.), Language and thought: Interdisciplinary themes (pp. 162–183). Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.CrossRef Clark, A. (1998). Magic words: how language augments human computation. In P. Carruthers & J. Boucher (Eds.), Language and thought: Interdisciplinary themes (pp. 162–183). Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.CrossRef
go back to reference Clark, A. (2006). Language, embodiment, and the cognitive niche. Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 10(8), 370–374.PubMedCrossRef Clark, A. (2006). Language, embodiment, and the cognitive niche. Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 10(8), 370–374.PubMedCrossRef
go back to reference Clark, A. (2015). Surfing uncertainty; prediction, action, and the embodied mind. New York: Oxford University Press. Clark, A. (2015). Surfing uncertainty; prediction, action, and the embodied mind. New York: Oxford University Press.
go back to reference Coltheart, M., Patterson, K., & Marshall, J. C. (Eds.). (1980). Deep dyslexia. London: Routledge & Kegan Paul. Coltheart, M., Patterson, K., & Marshall, J. C. (Eds.). (1980). Deep dyslexia. London: Routledge & Kegan Paul.
go back to reference Connell, L. (2019). What have labels ever done for us? The linguistic shortcut in conceptual processing. Language, Cognition and Neuroscience, 34(10), 1308–1318.CrossRef Connell, L. (2019). What have labels ever done for us? The linguistic shortcut in conceptual processing. Language, Cognition and Neuroscience, 34(10), 1308–1318.CrossRef
go back to reference Connell, L., & Lynott, D. (2012). Strength of perceptual experience predicts word processing performance better than concreteness or imageability. Cognition, 125(3), 452–465.PubMedCrossRef Connell, L., & Lynott, D. (2012). Strength of perceptual experience predicts word processing performance better than concreteness or imageability. Cognition, 125(3), 452–465.PubMedCrossRef
go back to reference Connell, L., Lynott, D., & Banks, B. (2018). Interoception: the forgotten modality in perceptual grounding of abstract and concrete concepts. Philosophical Transactions of the Royal Society B: Biological Sciences, 373(1752), 20170143.CrossRef Connell, L., Lynott, D., & Banks, B. (2018). Interoception: the forgotten modality in perceptual grounding of abstract and concrete concepts. Philosophical Transactions of the Royal Society B: Biological Sciences, 373(1752), 20170143.CrossRef
go back to reference Crutch, S. J., Troche, J., Reilly, J., & Ridgway, G. R. (2013). Abstract conceptual feature ratings: the role of emotion, magnitude, and other cognitive domains in the organization of abstract conceptual knowledge. Frontiers in Human Neuroscience, 7, 186.PubMedPubMedCentralCrossRef Crutch, S. J., Troche, J., Reilly, J., & Ridgway, G. R. (2013). Abstract conceptual feature ratings: the role of emotion, magnitude, and other cognitive domains in the organization of abstract conceptual knowledge. Frontiers in Human Neuroscience, 7, 186.PubMedPubMedCentralCrossRef
go back to reference D’Aversa, F., Lugli, L., Borghi, A.M., Barca, L. (2020). Implicit effect of abstract/concrete components in the categorization of Chinese words (submitted) D’Aversa, F., Lugli, L., Borghi, A.M., Barca, L. (2020). Implicit effect of abstract/concrete components in the categorization of Chinese words (submitted)
go back to reference Davis, C. P., Altmann, G. T. M., & Yee, E. (2020) Situational systematicity: A role for schema in understanding the differences between abstract and concrete concepts. Cognitive Neuropsychology. Advance online publication Davis, C. P., Altmann, G. T. M., & Yee, E. (2020) Situational systematicity: A role for schema in understanding the differences between abstract and concrete concepts. Cognitive Neuropsychology. Advance online publication
go back to reference Della Rosa, P. A., Catricalà, E., Canini, M., Vigliocco, G., & Cappa, S. F. (2018). The left inferior frontal gyrus: A neural crossroads between abstract and concrete knowledge. Neuroimage, 175, 449–459.PubMedCrossRef Della Rosa, P. A., Catricalà, E., Canini, M., Vigliocco, G., & Cappa, S. F. (2018). The left inferior frontal gyrus: A neural crossroads between abstract and concrete knowledge. Neuroimage, 175, 449–459.PubMedCrossRef
go back to reference Della Rosa, P. A., Catricalà, E., Vigliocco, G., & Cappa, S. F. (2010). Beyond the abstract—concrete dichotomy: mode of acquisition, concreteness, imageability, familiarity, age of acquisition, context availability, and abstractness norms for a set of 417 Italian words. Behavior Research Methods, 42(4), 1042–1048.PubMedCrossRef Della Rosa, P. A., Catricalà, E., Vigliocco, G., & Cappa, S. F. (2010). Beyond the abstract—concrete dichotomy: mode of acquisition, concreteness, imageability, familiarity, age of acquisition, context availability, and abstractness norms for a set of 417 Italian words. Behavior Research Methods, 42(4), 1042–1048.PubMedCrossRef
go back to reference Desai, R. H. (2019). Access and content of abstract concepts. Comment on" Words as social tools: Language, sociality, and inner grounding in abstract concepts" by Anna M. Borghi et al. PhLRv, 29, 166–168. Desai, R. H. (2019). Access and content of abstract concepts. Comment on" Words as social tools: Language, sociality, and inner grounding in abstract concepts" by Anna M. Borghi et al. PhLRv, 29, 166–168.
go back to reference Desai, R. H., Reilly, M., & van Dam, W. (2018). The multifaceted abstract brain. Philosophical Transactions of the Royal Society B: Biological Sciences, 373(1752), 20170122.CrossRef Desai, R. H., Reilly, M., & van Dam, W. (2018). The multifaceted abstract brain. Philosophical Transactions of the Royal Society B: Biological Sciences, 373(1752), 20170122.CrossRef
go back to reference Dimitrova-Radojichikj, D. (2015). Concepts of colors in children with congenital blindness. Journal of Special Education and Rehabilitation, 16(1–2), 7–16. Dimitrova-Radojichikj, D. (2015). Concepts of colors in children with congenital blindness. Journal of Special Education and Rehabilitation, 16(1–2), 7–16.
go back to reference Dove, G. (2009). Beyond perceptual symbols: A call for representational pluralism. Cognition, 110(3), 412–431.PubMedCrossRef Dove, G. (2009). Beyond perceptual symbols: A call for representational pluralism. Cognition, 110(3), 412–431.PubMedCrossRef
go back to reference Dove, G. (2014). Thinking in words: language as an embodied medium of thought. Topics in cognitive science, 6(3), 371–389.PubMedCrossRef Dove, G. (2014). Thinking in words: language as an embodied medium of thought. Topics in cognitive science, 6(3), 371–389.PubMedCrossRef
go back to reference Dove, G. (2016). Three symbol ungrounding problems: Abstract concepts and the future of embodied cognition. Psychonomic Bulletin & Review, 23(4), 1109–1121.CrossRef Dove, G. (2016). Three symbol ungrounding problems: Abstract concepts and the future of embodied cognition. Psychonomic Bulletin & Review, 23(4), 1109–1121.CrossRef
go back to reference Dove, G. (2018). Language as a disruptive technology: abstract concepts, embodiment and the flexible mind. Philosophical Transactions of the Royal Society B: Biological Sciences, 373(1752), 20170135.CrossRef Dove, G. (2018). Language as a disruptive technology: abstract concepts, embodiment and the flexible mind. Philosophical Transactions of the Royal Society B: Biological Sciences, 373(1752), 20170135.CrossRef
go back to reference Dove, G. (2019). More than a scaffold: Language is a neuroenhancement. Cognitive Neuropsychology, 2019, 1–24. Dove, G. (2019). More than a scaffold: Language is a neuroenhancement. Cognitive Neuropsychology, 2019, 1–24.
go back to reference Dreyer, F. R., & Pulvermüller, F. (2018). Abstract semantics in the motor system?–An event-related fMRI study on passive reading of semantic word categories carrying abstract emotional and mental meaning. Cortex, 100, 52–70.PubMedCrossRef Dreyer, F. R., & Pulvermüller, F. (2018). Abstract semantics in the motor system?–An event-related fMRI study on passive reading of semantic word categories carrying abstract emotional and mental meaning. Cortex, 100, 52–70.PubMedCrossRef
go back to reference Edmiston, P., & Lupyan, G. (2015). What makes words special? Words as unmotivated cues. Cognition, 143, 93–100.PubMedCrossRef Edmiston, P., & Lupyan, G. (2015). What makes words special? Words as unmotivated cues. Cognition, 143, 93–100.PubMedCrossRef
go back to reference Falandays, J. B., & Spivey, M. J. (2019). Abstract meanings may be more dynamic, due to their sociality: Comment on" Words as social tools: Language, sociality and inner grounding in abstract concepts" by Anna M. Borghi et al. Physics of Life Reviews, 29, 175–177.PubMedCrossRef Falandays, J. B., & Spivey, M. J. (2019). Abstract meanings may be more dynamic, due to their sociality: Comment on" Words as social tools: Language, sociality and inner grounding in abstract concepts" by Anna M. Borghi et al. Physics of Life Reviews, 29, 175–177.PubMedCrossRef
go back to reference Fernandino, L., Humphries, C. J., Conant, L. L., Seidenberg, M. S., & Binder, J. R. (2016). Heteromodal cortical areas encode sensory-motor features of word meaning. Journal of Neuroscience, 36(38), 9763–9769.PubMedCrossRef Fernandino, L., Humphries, C. J., Conant, L. L., Seidenberg, M. S., & Binder, J. R. (2016). Heteromodal cortical areas encode sensory-motor features of word meaning. Journal of Neuroscience, 36(38), 9763–9769.PubMedCrossRef
go back to reference Fiebach, C. J., & Friederici, A. D. (2004). Processing concrete words: fMRI evidence against a specific right-hemisphere involvement. Neuropsychologia, 42(1), 62–70.PubMedCrossRef Fiebach, C. J., & Friederici, A. D. (2004). Processing concrete words: fMRI evidence against a specific right-hemisphere involvement. Neuropsychologia, 42(1), 62–70.PubMedCrossRef
go back to reference Fingerhut, J., & Prinz, J. J. (2018). Grounding evaluative concepts. Philosophical Transactions of the Royal Society B: Biological Sciences, 373(1752), 20170142.CrossRef Fingerhut, J., & Prinz, J. J. (2018). Grounding evaluative concepts. Philosophical Transactions of the Royal Society B: Biological Sciences, 373(1752), 20170142.CrossRef
go back to reference Fini, C., Era, V., Da Rold, F., Candidi, M., & Borghi, A.M. (2020). Abstract concepts in interaction: The need of others when guessing abstract concepts smooths dyadic motor interactions. OSF preprint, https://osf.io/rf6d2/. Fini, C., Era, V., Da Rold, F., Candidi, M., & Borghi, A.M. (2020). Abstract concepts in interaction: The need of others when guessing abstract concepts smooths dyadic motor interactions. OSF preprint, https://​osf.​io/​rf6d2/​.
go back to reference Fischer, M. H., & Shaki, S. (2018). Number concepts: abstract and embodied. Philosophical Transactions of the Royal Society B: Biological Sciences, 373(1752), 20170125.CrossRef Fischer, M. H., & Shaki, S. (2018). Number concepts: abstract and embodied. Philosophical Transactions of the Royal Society B: Biological Sciences, 373(1752), 20170125.CrossRef
go back to reference Forde, E., & Humphreys, G. (Eds.). (2005). Category specificity in brain and mind. Hove: Psychology Press. Forde, E., & Humphreys, G. (Eds.). (2005). Category specificity in brain and mind. Hove: Psychology Press.
go back to reference Franklin, S., Howard, D., & Patterson, K. (1995). Abstract word anomia. Cognitive Neuropsychology, 12(5), 549–566.CrossRef Franklin, S., Howard, D., & Patterson, K. (1995). Abstract word anomia. Cognitive Neuropsychology, 12(5), 549–566.CrossRef
go back to reference Galantucci, B., & Sebanz, N. (2009). Joint action: current perspectives. Topics in Cognitive Science, 1(2), 255–259.PubMedCrossRef Galantucci, B., & Sebanz, N. (2009). Joint action: current perspectives. Topics in Cognitive Science, 1(2), 255–259.PubMedCrossRef
go back to reference Garagnani, M., & Pulvermüller, F. (2016). Conceptual grounding of language in action and perception: A neurocomputational model of the emergence of category specificity and semantic hubs. European Journal of Neuroscience, 43(6), 721–737.PubMedCrossRef Garagnani, M., & Pulvermüller, F. (2016). Conceptual grounding of language in action and perception: A neurocomputational model of the emergence of category specificity and semantic hubs. European Journal of Neuroscience, 43(6), 721–737.PubMedCrossRef
go back to reference Ghandhari, M., Fini, C., Darold, F., & Borghi, A. M. (2020). Different kinds of embodied language: a comparison between Italian and Persian languages. Brain and Cognition, 142, 105581.PubMedCrossRef Ghandhari, M., Fini, C., Darold, F., & Borghi, A. M. (2020). Different kinds of embodied language: a comparison between Italian and Persian languages. Brain and Cognition, 142, 105581.PubMedCrossRef
go back to reference Ghio, M., Vaghi, M. M. S., & Tettamanti, M. (2013). Fine-grained semantic categorization across the abstract and concrete domains. PLoS ONE, 8(6), e67090.PubMedPubMedCentralCrossRef Ghio, M., Vaghi, M. M. S., & Tettamanti, M. (2013). Fine-grained semantic categorization across the abstract and concrete domains. PLoS ONE, 8(6), e67090.PubMedPubMedCentralCrossRef
go back to reference Giesbrecht, B., Gamblin, C., & Swaab, T. (2004). Separable effects of semantic priming and imageability on word processing in human cortex. Cerebral Cortex, 14(5), 521–529.PubMedCrossRef Giesbrecht, B., Gamblin, C., & Swaab, T. (2004). Separable effects of semantic priming and imageability on word processing in human cortex. Cerebral Cortex, 14(5), 521–529.PubMedCrossRef
go back to reference Gilboa, A., & Marlatte, H. (2017). Neurobiology of schemas and schema-mediated memory. Trends in Cognitive Science, 21(8), 618–631.CrossRef Gilboa, A., & Marlatte, H. (2017). Neurobiology of schemas and schema-mediated memory. Trends in Cognitive Science, 21(8), 618–631.CrossRef
go back to reference Glenberg, A. M., Sato, M., Cattaneo, L., Riggio, L., Palumbo, D., & Buccino, G. (2008). Processing abstract language modulates motor system activity. The Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology, 61(6), 905–919.PubMedCrossRef Glenberg, A. M., Sato, M., Cattaneo, L., Riggio, L., Palumbo, D., & Buccino, G. (2008). Processing abstract language modulates motor system activity. The Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology, 61(6), 905–919.PubMedCrossRef
go back to reference Goldberg, R. F., Perfetti, C. A., Fiez, J. A., & Schneider, W. (2007). Selective retrieval of abstract semantic knowledge in left prefrontal cortex. The Journal of Neuroscience, 27(14), 3790–3798.PubMedPubMedCentralCrossRef Goldberg, R. F., Perfetti, C. A., Fiez, J. A., & Schneider, W. (2007). Selective retrieval of abstract semantic knowledge in left prefrontal cortex. The Journal of Neuroscience, 27(14), 3790–3798.PubMedPubMedCentralCrossRef
go back to reference Goldberg, R. F., Perfetti, C. A., & Schneider, W. (2006). Distinct and common cortical activations for multimodal semantic categories. Cognitive, Affective, & Behavioral Neuroscience, 6(3), 214–222.CrossRef Goldberg, R. F., Perfetti, C. A., & Schneider, W. (2006). Distinct and common cortical activations for multimodal semantic categories. Cognitive, Affective, & Behavioral Neuroscience, 6(3), 214–222.CrossRef
go back to reference Goodfellow, I., Bengio, Y., & Courville, A. (2016). Deep learning. Cambridge: MIT Press. Goodfellow, I., Bengio, Y., & Courville, A. (2016). Deep learning. Cambridge: MIT Press.
go back to reference Goodglass, H., Hyde, M. R., & Blumstein, S. (1969). Frequency, picturability and availability of nouns in aphasia. Cortex, 5, 104–119.PubMedCrossRef Goodglass, H., Hyde, M. R., & Blumstein, S. (1969). Frequency, picturability and availability of nouns in aphasia. Cortex, 5, 104–119.PubMedCrossRef
go back to reference Granito, C., Scorolli, C., & Borghi, A. M. (2015). Naming a lego world. The role of language in the acquisition of abstract concepts. PLoS ONE, 10(1), e0114615.PubMedPubMedCentralCrossRef Granito, C., Scorolli, C., & Borghi, A. M. (2015). Naming a lego world. The role of language in the acquisition of abstract concepts. PLoS ONE, 10(1), e0114615.PubMedPubMedCentralCrossRef
go back to reference Harpaintner, M., Trumpp, N. M., & Kiefer, M. (2018). The semantic content of abstract concepts: A property listing study of 296 abstract words. Frontiers in Psychology, 9, 1748.PubMedPubMedCentralCrossRef Harpaintner, M., Trumpp, N. M., & Kiefer, M. (2018). The semantic content of abstract concepts: A property listing study of 296 abstract words. Frontiers in Psychology, 9, 1748.PubMedPubMedCentralCrossRef
go back to reference Hickok, G., & Poeppel, D. (2004). Dorsal and ventral streams: A framework for understanding aspects of the functional anatomy of language. Cognition, 92, 67–99.PubMedCrossRef Hickok, G., & Poeppel, D. (2004). Dorsal and ventral streams: A framework for understanding aspects of the functional anatomy of language. Cognition, 92, 67–99.PubMedCrossRef
go back to reference Hickok, G., & Poeppel, D. (2007). The cortical organization of speech processing. Nature Reviews Neuroscience, 8, 393–402.PubMedCrossRef Hickok, G., & Poeppel, D. (2007). The cortical organization of speech processing. Nature Reviews Neuroscience, 8, 393–402.PubMedCrossRef
go back to reference Hoffman, P. (2016). The meaning of ‘life’ and other abstract words: Insights from neuropsychology. Journal of Neuropsychology, 10, 317–343.PubMedCrossRef Hoffman, P. (2016). The meaning of ‘life’ and other abstract words: Insights from neuropsychology. Journal of Neuropsychology, 10, 317–343.PubMedCrossRef
go back to reference Hoffman, P., Binney, R. J., & Lambon Ralph, M. A. (2015). Differing contributions of inferior prefrontal and anterior temporal cortex to concrete and abstract conceptual knowledge. Cortex, 63, 250–266.PubMedPubMedCentralCrossRef Hoffman, P., Binney, R. J., & Lambon Ralph, M. A. (2015). Differing contributions of inferior prefrontal and anterior temporal cortex to concrete and abstract conceptual knowledge. Cortex, 63, 250–266.PubMedPubMedCentralCrossRef
go back to reference Hoffman, P., & Lambon Ralph, M. A. (2011). Reverse concreteness are not a typical feature of semantic dementia: Evidence for the hub-and-spoke model of conceptual representation. Cerebral Cortex, 21, 2103–2112.PubMedCrossRef Hoffman, P., & Lambon Ralph, M. A. (2011). Reverse concreteness are not a typical feature of semantic dementia: Evidence for the hub-and-spoke model of conceptual representation. Cerebral Cortex, 21, 2103–2112.PubMedCrossRef
go back to reference Hoffman, P., Lambon Ralph, M. A., & Rogers, T. T. (2013). Semantic diversity: A measure of semantic ambiguity based on variability in the contextual usage of words. Behavior Research Methods, 45, 718–730.PubMedCrossRef Hoffman, P., Lambon Ralph, M. A., & Rogers, T. T. (2013). Semantic diversity: A measure of semantic ambiguity based on variability in the contextual usage of words. Behavior Research Methods, 45, 718–730.PubMedCrossRef
go back to reference Hoffman, P., & Woollams, A. M. (2015). Opposing effects of semantic diversity in lexical and semantic relatedness decisions. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 41, 385–402.PubMed Hoffman, P., & Woollams, A. M. (2015). Opposing effects of semantic diversity in lexical and semantic relatedness decisions. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 41, 385–402.PubMed
go back to reference Hostetter, A. B., & Alibali, M. W. (2008). Visible embodiment: Gestures as simulated action. Psychonomic Bulletin & Review, 15(3), 495–514.CrossRef Hostetter, A. B., & Alibali, M. W. (2008). Visible embodiment: Gestures as simulated action. Psychonomic Bulletin & Review, 15(3), 495–514.CrossRef
go back to reference Humphries, C., Binder, J. R., Medler, D. A., & Liebenthal, E. (2006). Syntactic and semantic modulation of neural activity during auditory sentence comprehension. Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience, 18, 665–679.PubMedPubMedCentralCrossRef Humphries, C., Binder, J. R., Medler, D. A., & Liebenthal, E. (2006). Syntactic and semantic modulation of neural activity during auditory sentence comprehension. Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience, 18, 665–679.PubMedPubMedCentralCrossRef
go back to reference Jefferies, E., & Lambon Ralph, M. A. (2006). Semantic impairment in stroke aphasia versus semantic dementia: A case-series comparison. Brain, 129, 2132–2147.PubMedCrossRef Jefferies, E., & Lambon Ralph, M. A. (2006). Semantic impairment in stroke aphasia versus semantic dementia: A case-series comparison. Brain, 129, 2132–2147.PubMedCrossRef
go back to reference Katz, R. B., & Goodglass, H. (1990). Deep dysphasia: Analysis of a rare form of repetition disorder. Brain and Language, 39(1), 153–185.PubMedCrossRef Katz, R. B., & Goodglass, H. (1990). Deep dysphasia: Analysis of a rare form of repetition disorder. Brain and Language, 39(1), 153–185.PubMedCrossRef
go back to reference Kemmerer, D. (2019). Concepts in the brain: the view from cross-linguistic diversity. New York: Oxford University Press.CrossRef Kemmerer, D. (2019). Concepts in the brain: the view from cross-linguistic diversity. New York: Oxford University Press.CrossRef
go back to reference Kim, J. S., Elli, G. V., & Bedny, M. (2019). Knowledge of animal appearance among sighted and blind adults. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences, 116(23), 11213–11222.CrossRef Kim, J. S., Elli, G. V., & Bedny, M. (2019). Knowledge of animal appearance among sighted and blind adults. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences, 116(23), 11213–11222.CrossRef
go back to reference Knoblich, G., Butterfill, S., & Sebanz, N. (2011). Psychological research on joint action: theory and data. Psychology of learning and motivation (Vol. 54, pp. 59–101). New York: Academic Press. Knoblich, G., Butterfill, S., & Sebanz, N. (2011). Psychological research on joint action: theory and data. Psychology of learning and motivation (Vol. 54, pp. 59–101). New York: Academic Press.
go back to reference Kompa, N. A., & Mueller, J. L. (2020). How abstract (non-embodied) linguistic representations augment cognitive control. Frontiers in Psychology, 11, 1–13.CrossRef Kompa, N. A., & Mueller, J. L. (2020). How abstract (non-embodied) linguistic representations augment cognitive control. Frontiers in Psychology, 11, 1–13.CrossRef
go back to reference Lakoff, G., & Johnson, M. (2003). Metaphors we live by 1980. Chicago: University of Chicago Press.CrossRef Lakoff, G., & Johnson, M. (2003). Metaphors we live by 1980. Chicago: University of Chicago Press.CrossRef
go back to reference Landauer, T. K., Foltz, P. W., & Laham, D. (1998). Introduction to latent semantic analysis. Discourse Processes, 25, 259–284.CrossRef Landauer, T. K., Foltz, P. W., & Laham, D. (1998). Introduction to latent semantic analysis. Discourse Processes, 25, 259–284.CrossRef
go back to reference Langland-Hassan, P., & Vicente, A. (Eds.). (2018). Inner speech: New voices. USA: Oxford University Press. Langland-Hassan, P., & Vicente, A. (Eds.). (2018). Inner speech: New voices. USA: Oxford University Press.
go back to reference Lecours, A. R., & Joanette, Y. (1980). Linguistic and other psychological aspects of paroxysmal aphasia. Brain and Language, 10(1), 1–23.PubMedCrossRef Lecours, A. R., & Joanette, Y. (1980). Linguistic and other psychological aspects of paroxysmal aphasia. Brain and Language, 10(1), 1–23.PubMedCrossRef
go back to reference Lenci, A., Baroni, M., Cazzolli, G., & Marotta, G. (2013). BLIND: a set of semantic feature norms from the congenitally blind. Behavior Research Methods, 45, 1218–1233.PubMedCrossRef Lenci, A., Baroni, M., Cazzolli, G., & Marotta, G. (2013). BLIND: a set of semantic feature norms from the congenitally blind. Behavior Research Methods, 45, 1218–1233.PubMedCrossRef
go back to reference Lewis, M., Zetterson, M., & Lupyan, G. (2019). Distributional semantics as a source of visual knowledge. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences, 116(39), 19237–19238.CrossRef Lewis, M., Zetterson, M., & Lupyan, G. (2019). Distributional semantics as a source of visual knowledge. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences, 116(39), 19237–19238.CrossRef
go back to reference Lœvenbruck, H., Grandchamp, R., Rapin, L., Nalborczyk, L., & Dohen, M. (2018). A cognitive neuroscience view of inner language. In P. Langland-Hassan & A. Vicente (Eds.), Inner speech: New voices. USA: Oxford University Press. Lœvenbruck, H., Grandchamp, R., Rapin, L., Nalborczyk, L., & Dohen, M. (2018). A cognitive neuroscience view of inner language. In P. Langland-Hassan & A. Vicente (Eds.), Inner speech: New voices. USA: Oxford University Press.
go back to reference Loiselle, M., Rouleau, I., Nguyen, D. K., Dubeau, F., & Joubert, S. (2012). Comprehension of concrete and abstract words in patients with selective anterior temporal lobe resection and in patients with selective amygdalo-hippocampectomy. Neuropsychologia, 50, 630–639.PubMedCrossRef Loiselle, M., Rouleau, I., Nguyen, D. K., Dubeau, F., & Joubert, S. (2012). Comprehension of concrete and abstract words in patients with selective anterior temporal lobe resection and in patients with selective amygdalo-hippocampectomy. Neuropsychologia, 50, 630–639.PubMedCrossRef
go back to reference Lourwerse, M. (2018). Knowing the meaning of a word by the linguistic and perceptual company it keeps. Topics in Cognitive Science, 10(3), 573–589.CrossRef Lourwerse, M. (2018). Knowing the meaning of a word by the linguistic and perceptual company it keeps. Topics in Cognitive Science, 10(3), 573–589.CrossRef
go back to reference Louwerse, M. (2011). Symbol interdependency in symbolic and embodied cognition. Topics in Cognitive Science, 3, 273–302.PubMedCrossRef Louwerse, M. (2011). Symbol interdependency in symbolic and embodied cognition. Topics in Cognitive Science, 3, 273–302.PubMedCrossRef
go back to reference Louwerse, M. M., & Jeuniaux, P. (2010). The linguistic and embodied nature of conceptual processing. Cognition, 114(1), 96–104.PubMedCrossRef Louwerse, M. M., & Jeuniaux, P. (2010). The linguistic and embodied nature of conceptual processing. Cognition, 114(1), 96–104.PubMedCrossRef
go back to reference Lund, K., & Burgess, C. (1996). Producing high-dimensional semantic spaces from lexical co-occurrence. Behavior and Research Methods, Instruments, & Computers, 28, 203–208.CrossRef Lund, K., & Burgess, C. (1996). Producing high-dimensional semantic spaces from lexical co-occurrence. Behavior and Research Methods, Instruments, & Computers, 28, 203–208.CrossRef
go back to reference Lund, T. C., Sidhu, D. M., & Pexman, P. M. (2019). Sensitivity to emotion information in children’s lexical processing. Cognition, 190, 61–71.PubMedCrossRef Lund, T. C., Sidhu, D. M., & Pexman, P. M. (2019). Sensitivity to emotion information in children’s lexical processing. Cognition, 190, 61–71.PubMedCrossRef
go back to reference Lupyan, G., & Mirman, D. (2013). Linking language and categorization: Evidence from aphasia. Cortex, 49(5), 1187–1194.PubMedCrossRef Lupyan, G., & Mirman, D. (2013). Linking language and categorization: Evidence from aphasia. Cortex, 49(5), 1187–1194.PubMedCrossRef
go back to reference Lupyan, G., & Winter, B. (2018). Language is more abstract than you think, or, why aren’t languages more iconic? Philosophical Transactions of the Royal Society B: Biological Sciences, 373(1752), 20170137.CrossRef Lupyan, G., & Winter, B. (2018). Language is more abstract than you think, or, why aren’t languages more iconic? Philosophical Transactions of the Royal Society B: Biological Sciences, 373(1752), 20170137.CrossRef
go back to reference Lynott, D., & Connell, L. (2010). Embodied conceptual combination. Frontiers in Psychology, 1, 1–14.CrossRef Lynott, D., & Connell, L. (2010). Embodied conceptual combination. Frontiers in Psychology, 1, 1–14.CrossRef
go back to reference Lynott, D., Connell, L., Brysbaert, M., Brand, J., & Carney, J. (2019). The Lancaster Sensorimotor Norms: multidimensional measures of perceptual and action strength for 40,000 English words. Behavior Research Methods, 1–21. Lynott, D., Connell, L., Brysbaert, M., Brand, J., & Carney, J. (2019). The Lancaster Sensorimotor Norms: multidimensional measures of perceptual and action strength for 40,000 English words. Behavior Research Methods, 1–21.
go back to reference Machery, E. (2015). By default. In S. Laurence & E. Margolis (Eds.), The conceptual mind: new directions in the study of concepts (pp. 567–588). Cambridge: MIT Press. Machery, E. (2015). By default. In S. Laurence & E. Margolis (Eds.), The conceptual mind: new directions in the study of concepts (pp. 567–588). Cambridge: MIT Press.
go back to reference Mani, N., & Plunkett, K. (2010). In the infant’s mind’s ear: Evidence for implicit naming in 18-month-olds. Psychological Science, 21(7), 908–913.PubMedCrossRef Mani, N., & Plunkett, K. (2010). In the infant’s mind’s ear: Evidence for implicit naming in 18-month-olds. Psychological Science, 21(7), 908–913.PubMedCrossRef
go back to reference Martin, N., & Saffran, E. M. (1992). A computational account of deep dysphasia: Evidence from a single case study. Brain and Language, 43, 240–274.PubMedCrossRef Martin, N., & Saffran, E. M. (1992). A computational account of deep dysphasia: Evidence from a single case study. Brain and Language, 43, 240–274.PubMedCrossRef
go back to reference Mazzuca, C., Lugli, L., Benassi, M., Nicoletti, R., & Borghi, A. M. (2018). Abstract, emotional and concrete concepts and the activation of mouth-hand effectors. PeerJ, 6, e5987.PubMedPubMedCentralCrossRef Mazzuca, C., Lugli, L., Benassi, M., Nicoletti, R., & Borghi, A. M. (2018). Abstract, emotional and concrete concepts and the activation of mouth-hand effectors. PeerJ, 6, e5987.PubMedPubMedCentralCrossRef
go back to reference Mazzuca, C., Majid, A., Lugli, L., Nicoletti, R., & Borghi, A. (2020). Gender is a multifaceted concept: evidence that specific life experiences differentially shape the concept of gender. Language and Cognition Mazzuca, C., Majid, A., Lugli, L., Nicoletti, R., & Borghi, A. (2020). Gender is a multifaceted concept: evidence that specific life experiences differentially shape the concept of gender. Language and Cognition
go back to reference Mellem, M. S., Jasmin, K. M., Peng, C., & Martin, A. (2016). Sentence processing in anterior superior temporal cortex shows a social-emotional bias. Neuropsychologia, 89, 217–224.PubMedPubMedCentralCrossRef Mellem, M. S., Jasmin, K. M., Peng, C., & Martin, A. (2016). Sentence processing in anterior superior temporal cortex shows a social-emotional bias. Neuropsychologia, 89, 217–224.PubMedPubMedCentralCrossRef
go back to reference Meteyard, L., Cuadrado, S. R., Bahrami, B., & Vigliocco, G. (2012). Coming of age: A review of embodiment and the neuroscience of semantics. Cortex, 48(7), 788–804.PubMedCrossRef Meteyard, L., Cuadrado, S. R., Bahrami, B., & Vigliocco, G. (2012). Coming of age: A review of embodiment and the neuroscience of semantics. Cortex, 48(7), 788–804.PubMedCrossRef
go back to reference Meyer, K., & Damasio, A. (2009). Convergence and divergence in a neural architecture for recognition and memory. Trends in Neurosciences, 32(7), P376–P382.CrossRef Meyer, K., & Damasio, A. (2009). Convergence and divergence in a neural architecture for recognition and memory. Trends in Neurosciences, 32(7), P376–P382.CrossRef
go back to reference Mkrtychian, N., Blagovechtchenski, E., Kurmakaeva, D., Gnedykh, D., Kostromina, S., & Shtyrov, Y. (2019). Concrete vs abstract semantics: From mental representations to functional brain mapping. Frontiers in Human Neuroscience, 13, 267.PubMedPubMedCentralCrossRef Mkrtychian, N., Blagovechtchenski, E., Kurmakaeva, D., Gnedykh, D., Kostromina, S., & Shtyrov, Y. (2019). Concrete vs abstract semantics: From mental representations to functional brain mapping. Frontiers in Human Neuroscience, 13, 267.PubMedPubMedCentralCrossRef
go back to reference Montefinese, M. (2019). Semantic representation of abstract and concrete words: A minireview of the neural evidence. Journal of Neurophysiology, 121, 1585–1587.PubMedCrossRef Montefinese, M. (2019). Semantic representation of abstract and concrete words: A minireview of the neural evidence. Journal of Neurophysiology, 121, 1585–1587.PubMedCrossRef
go back to reference Morin, A. (2018). The self-reflective functions of inner speech: Thirteen years later. In P. Langland-Hassan & A. Vicente (Eds.), Inner speech: New voices. USA: Oxford University Press. Morin, A. (2018). The self-reflective functions of inner speech: Thirteen years later. In P. Langland-Hassan & A. Vicente (Eds.), Inner speech: New voices. USA: Oxford University Press.
go back to reference Morin, A., & Hamper, B. (2012). Self-reflection and the inner voice: activation of the left inferior frontal gyrus during perceptual and conceptual self-referential thinking. The Open Neuroimaging Journal, 6, 78.PubMedPubMedCentralCrossRef Morin, A., & Hamper, B. (2012). Self-reflection and the inner voice: activation of the left inferior frontal gyrus during perceptual and conceptual self-referential thinking. The Open Neuroimaging Journal, 6, 78.PubMedPubMedCentralCrossRef
go back to reference Noppeney, U., & Wallesch, C.-W. (2000). Language and cognition: Kurt Goldstein’s theory of semantics. Brain and Langauge, 44(3), 367–386. Noppeney, U., & Wallesch, C.-W. (2000). Language and cognition: Kurt Goldstein’s theory of semantics. Brain and Langauge, 44(3), 367–386.
go back to reference Nummenmaa, L., Glerean, E., Viinikainen, M., Jääskeläinen, I. P., Hari, R., & Sams, M. (2012). Emotions promote social interaction by synchronizing brain activity across individuals. PNAS, 109(24), 9599–9604.PubMedPubMedCentralCrossRef Nummenmaa, L., Glerean, E., Viinikainen, M., Jääskeläinen, I. P., Hari, R., & Sams, M. (2012). Emotions promote social interaction by synchronizing brain activity across individuals. PNAS, 109(24), 9599–9604.PubMedPubMedCentralCrossRef
go back to reference Oppenheim, G. M., & Dell, G. S. (2010). Motor movement matters: The flexible abstractness of inner speech. Memory & cognition, 38(8), 1147–1160.CrossRef Oppenheim, G. M., & Dell, G. S. (2010). Motor movement matters: The flexible abstractness of inner speech. Memory & cognition, 38(8), 1147–1160.CrossRef
go back to reference Paivio, A. (1990). Mental representations: A dual coding approach. New York: Oxford University Press.CrossRef Paivio, A. (1990). Mental representations: A dual coding approach. New York: Oxford University Press.CrossRef
go back to reference Papagno, C., Fogliata, A., Catricalà, E., & Miniussi, C. (2009). The lexical processing of abstract and concrete nouns. Brain research, 1263, 78–86.PubMedCrossRef Papagno, C., Fogliata, A., Catricalà, E., & Miniussi, C. (2009). The lexical processing of abstract and concrete nouns. Brain research, 1263, 78–86.PubMedCrossRef
go back to reference Pecher, D., Zeelenberg, R., & Barsalou, L. W. (2003). Verifying properties from different modalities for concepts produces switching costs. Psychological Science, 14, 119–124.PubMedCrossRef Pecher, D., Zeelenberg, R., & Barsalou, L. W. (2003). Verifying properties from different modalities for concepts produces switching costs. Psychological Science, 14, 119–124.PubMedCrossRef
go back to reference Pezzulo, G., Barca, L., & D’Ausilio, A. (2014). The sensorimotor and social side of the architecture of speech. Behavioral and Brain Sciences Commentary Invitation, 36(6), 569–570.CrossRef Pezzulo, G., Barca, L., & D’Ausilio, A. (2014). The sensorimotor and social side of the architecture of speech. Behavioral and Brain Sciences Commentary Invitation, 36(6), 569–570.CrossRef
go back to reference Pezzulo, G., Iodice, P., Donnarumma, F., Dindo, H., & Knoblich, G. (2017). Avoiding accidents at the champagne reception: A study of joint lifting and balancing. Psychological Science, 28(3), 338–345.PubMedCrossRef Pezzulo, G., Iodice, P., Donnarumma, F., Dindo, H., & Knoblich, G. (2017). Avoiding accidents at the champagne reception: A study of joint lifting and balancing. Psychological Science, 28(3), 338–345.PubMedCrossRef
go back to reference Pickering, M. J., & Garrod, S. (2013). An integrated theory of language production and comprehension. Behavioral and Brain Sciences, 36(4), 329–347.PubMedCrossRef Pickering, M. J., & Garrod, S. (2013). An integrated theory of language production and comprehension. Behavioral and Brain Sciences, 36(4), 329–347.PubMedCrossRef
go back to reference Ponari, M., Norbury, C. F., & Vigliocco, G. (2018). Acquisition of abstract concepts is influenced by emotional valence. Developmental Science, 21(2), e12549.CrossRef Ponari, M., Norbury, C. F., & Vigliocco, G. (2018). Acquisition of abstract concepts is influenced by emotional valence. Developmental Science, 21(2), e12549.CrossRef
go back to reference Pulvermüller, F. (2013). How neurons make meaning: Brain mechanisms for embodied and abstract-symbolic semantics. Trends in Cognitive Science, 17(9), 458–470.CrossRef Pulvermüller, F. (2013). How neurons make meaning: Brain mechanisms for embodied and abstract-symbolic semantics. Trends in Cognitive Science, 17(9), 458–470.CrossRef
go back to reference Pulvermüller, F. (2018). The case of CAUSE: neurobiological mechanisms for grounding an abstract concept. Philosophical Transactions of the Royal Society B: Biological Sciences, 373(1752), 20170129.CrossRef Pulvermüller, F. (2018). The case of CAUSE: neurobiological mechanisms for grounding an abstract concept. Philosophical Transactions of the Royal Society B: Biological Sciences, 373(1752), 20170129.CrossRef
go back to reference Redcay, E., & Schilbach, L. (2019). Using second-person neuroscience to elucidate the mechanisms of social interaction. Nature Reviews Neuroscience, 20(8), 495–505.PubMedPubMedCentralCrossRef Redcay, E., & Schilbach, L. (2019). Using second-person neuroscience to elucidate the mechanisms of social interaction. Nature Reviews Neuroscience, 20(8), 495–505.PubMedPubMedCentralCrossRef
go back to reference Reilly, J., & Peelle, J. E. (2008). Effects of semantic impairment on language processing in semantic dementia. Seminars in Speech and Language, 29(1), 32–43.PubMedCrossRef Reilly, J., & Peelle, J. E. (2008). Effects of semantic impairment on language processing in semantic dementia. Seminars in Speech and Language, 29(1), 32–43.PubMedCrossRef
go back to reference Riordan, B., & Jones, M. N. (2010). Redundancy in perceptual and linguistic experience: Comparing feature-based and distributional models of semantic information. Topics in Cognitive Science, 3, 303–345.PubMedCrossRef Riordan, B., & Jones, M. N. (2010). Redundancy in perceptual and linguistic experience: Comparing feature-based and distributional models of semantic information. Topics in Cognitive Science, 3, 303–345.PubMedCrossRef
go back to reference Roversi, C., Borghi, A. M., & Tummolini, L. (2013). A marriage is an artefact and not a walk that we take together: an experimental study on the categorization of artefacts. Review of Philosophy and Psychology, 4(3), 527–542.CrossRef Roversi, C., Borghi, A. M., & Tummolini, L. (2013). A marriage is an artefact and not a walk that we take together: an experimental study on the categorization of artefacts. Review of Philosophy and Psychology, 4(3), 527–542.CrossRef
go back to reference Rumiati, R. I., & Foroni, F. (2016). We are what we eat: How food is represented in our mind/brain. Psychonomic Bulletin & Review, 23(4), 1043–1054.CrossRef Rumiati, R. I., & Foroni, F. (2016). We are what we eat: How food is represented in our mind/brain. Psychonomic Bulletin & Review, 23(4), 1043–1054.CrossRef
go back to reference Sabsevitz, D., Medler, D., Seidenberg, M., & Binder, J. (2005). Modulation of the semantic system by word imageability. Neuroimage, 27(1), 188–200.PubMedCrossRef Sabsevitz, D., Medler, D., Seidenberg, M., & Binder, J. (2005). Modulation of the semantic system by word imageability. Neuroimage, 27(1), 188–200.PubMedCrossRef
go back to reference Sakreida, K., Scorolli, C., Menz, M. M., Heim, S., Borghi, A. M., & Binkofski, F. (2013). Are abstract action words embodied? An fMRI investigation at the interface between language and motor cognition. Frontiers in Human Neuroscience, 7, 125.PubMedPubMedCentralCrossRef Sakreida, K., Scorolli, C., Menz, M. M., Heim, S., Borghi, A. M., & Binkofski, F. (2013). Are abstract action words embodied? An fMRI investigation at the interface between language and motor cognition. Frontiers in Human Neuroscience, 7, 125.PubMedPubMedCentralCrossRef
go back to reference Schaller, S. (2012). A man without Words (2nd ed.). Berkeley: University of California Press. Schaller, S. (2012). A man without Words (2nd ed.). Berkeley: University of California Press.
go back to reference Schilbach, L. (2015). Eye to eye, face to face and brain to brain: Novel approaches to study the behavioral dynamics and neural mechanisms of social interactions. Current Opinions in Behavioral Science, 3, 130–135.CrossRef Schilbach, L. (2015). Eye to eye, face to face and brain to brain: Novel approaches to study the behavioral dynamics and neural mechanisms of social interactions. Current Opinions in Behavioral Science, 3, 130–135.CrossRef
go back to reference Schwanenflugel, P. J., Akin, C., & Luh, W. M. (1992). Context availability and the recall of abstract and concrete words. Memory & Cognition, 20(1), 96–104.CrossRef Schwanenflugel, P. J., Akin, C., & Luh, W. M. (1992). Context availability and the recall of abstract and concrete words. Memory & Cognition, 20(1), 96–104.CrossRef
go back to reference Scorolli, C., Binkofski, F., Buccino, G., Nicoletti, R., Riggio, L., & Borghi, A. M. (2011). Abstract and concrete sentences, embodiment, and languages. Frontiers in Psychology, 2, 227.PubMedPubMedCentralCrossRef Scorolli, C., Binkofski, F., Buccino, G., Nicoletti, R., Riggio, L., & Borghi, A. M. (2011). Abstract and concrete sentences, embodiment, and languages. Frontiers in Psychology, 2, 227.PubMedPubMedCentralCrossRef
go back to reference Scorolli, C., Jacquet, P. O., Binkofski, F., Nicoletti, R., Tessari, A., & Borghi, A. M. (2012). Abstract and concrete phrases processing differentially modulates cortico-spinal excitability. Brain Research, 1488, 60–71.PubMedCrossRef Scorolli, C., Jacquet, P. O., Binkofski, F., Nicoletti, R., Tessari, A., & Borghi, A. M. (2012). Abstract and concrete phrases processing differentially modulates cortico-spinal excitability. Brain Research, 1488, 60–71.PubMedCrossRef
go back to reference Shallice, T., & Warrington, E. K. (1975). Word recognition in a phonemic dyslexic patient. Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology, 27, 187–199.PubMedCrossRef Shallice, T., & Warrington, E. K. (1975). Word recognition in a phonemic dyslexic patient. Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology, 27, 187–199.PubMedCrossRef
go back to reference Shea, N. (2018). Metacognition and abstract concepts. Philosophical Transactions of the Royal Society B: Biological Sciences, 373(1752), 20170133.CrossRef Shea, N. (2018). Metacognition and abstract concepts. Philosophical Transactions of the Royal Society B: Biological Sciences, 373(1752), 20170133.CrossRef
go back to reference Shepard, R. N., & Cooper, L. A. (1992). Representation of colors in the blind, color-blind, and normally sighted. Psychological Science, 3(2), 97–104.CrossRef Shepard, R. N., & Cooper, L. A. (1992). Representation of colors in the blind, color-blind, and normally sighted. Psychological Science, 3(2), 97–104.CrossRef
go back to reference Shields, C. (2016). Aristotle. Stanford Encyclopedia of Philosophy. Shields, C. (2016). Aristotle. Stanford Encyclopedia of Philosophy.
go back to reference Simmons, W. K., & Barsalou, L. W. (2003). The similarity-in-topography principle: Reconciling theories of conceptual deficits. Cognitive Neuropsychology, 20(3–6), 451–486.PubMedCrossRef Simmons, W. K., & Barsalou, L. W. (2003). The similarity-in-topography principle: Reconciling theories of conceptual deficits. Cognitive Neuropsychology, 20(3–6), 451–486.PubMedCrossRef
go back to reference Sirigu, A., Duhamel, J. R., & Poncet, M. (1991). The role of sensorimotor experience in object recognition: A case of multimodal agnosia. Brain, 114, 2555–2573.PubMedCrossRef Sirigu, A., Duhamel, J. R., & Poncet, M. (1991). The role of sensorimotor experience in object recognition: A case of multimodal agnosia. Brain, 114, 2555–2573.PubMedCrossRef
go back to reference Stevanovic, M., Henttonen, P., Koskinen, E., Peräkylä, A., von Wendt, T. N., & Sams, M. (2019). Physiological responses to affiliation during conversation: Comparing neurotypical males and males with Asperger syndrome. PLoS ONE, 14(9), e222084.CrossRef Stevanovic, M., Henttonen, P., Koskinen, E., Peräkylä, A., von Wendt, T. N., & Sams, M. (2019). Physiological responses to affiliation during conversation: Comparing neurotypical males and males with Asperger syndrome. PLoS ONE, 14(9), e222084.CrossRef
go back to reference Swiney, L. (2018). Activity, agency, and inner speech pathology. In P. Langland-Hassan & A. Vicente (Eds.), Inner speech: new voices (p. 299). Oxford: Oxford University Press. Swiney, L. (2018). Activity, agency, and inner speech pathology. In P. Langland-Hassan & A. Vicente (Eds.), Inner speech: new voices (p. 299). Oxford: Oxford University Press.
go back to reference Thompson-Schill, S. L. (2003). Neuroimaging studies of semantic memory: inferring “how” from “where.” Neuropsychologia, 41(3), 280–292.PubMedCrossRef Thompson-Schill, S. L. (2003). Neuroimaging studies of semantic memory: inferring “how” from “where.” Neuropsychologia, 41(3), 280–292.PubMedCrossRef
go back to reference Tillas, A. (2015). Language as grist to the mill of cognition. Cognitive Processing, 16(3), 219–243.PubMedCrossRef Tillas, A. (2015). Language as grist to the mill of cognition. Cognitive Processing, 16(3), 219–243.PubMedCrossRef
go back to reference Tillotson, S. M., Siakaluk, P. D., & Pexman, P. M. (2008). Body—object interaction ratings for 1,618 monosyllabic nouns. Behavior Research Methods, 40(4), 1075–1078.PubMedCrossRef Tillotson, S. M., Siakaluk, P. D., & Pexman, P. M. (2008). Body—object interaction ratings for 1,618 monosyllabic nouns. Behavior Research Methods, 40(4), 1075–1078.PubMedCrossRef
go back to reference Troche, J., Crutch, S. J., & Reilly, J. (2017). Defining a conceptual topography of word concreteness: clustering properties of emotion, sensation, and magnitude among 750 english words. Frontiers in Psychology, 8, 1787.PubMedPubMedCentralCrossRef Troche, J., Crutch, S. J., & Reilly, J. (2017). Defining a conceptual topography of word concreteness: clustering properties of emotion, sensation, and magnitude among 750 english words. Frontiers in Psychology, 8, 1787.PubMedPubMedCentralCrossRef
go back to reference Tummolini, L., & Castelfranchi, C. (2006). The cognitive and behavioral mediation of institutions: Towards an account of institutional actions. Cognitive Systems Research, 7, 307–323.CrossRef Tummolini, L., & Castelfranchi, C. (2006). The cognitive and behavioral mediation of institutions: Towards an account of institutional actions. Cognitive Systems Research, 7, 307–323.CrossRef
go back to reference Tylen, K., Philipsen, J. S., Østergaard, S., Raczaszek-Leonardi, J., Stjernfelt, F., & Fusaroli, R. (2018). Interaction, cognitive diversity and abstraction. In CogSci Tylen, K., Philipsen, J. S., Østergaard, S., Raczaszek-Leonardi, J., Stjernfelt, F., & Fusaroli, R. (2018). Interaction, cognitive diversity and abstraction. In CogSci
go back to reference van den Heuvel, M. P., & Sporns, O. (2013). Network hubs in the human brain. Trends in Cognitive Science, 17, 683–696.CrossRef van den Heuvel, M. P., & Sporns, O. (2013). Network hubs in the human brain. Trends in Cognitive Science, 17, 683–696.CrossRef
go back to reference Vandenberghe, R., Nobre, A. C., & Price, C. J. (2002). The response of left temporal cortex to sentences. Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience, 14, 550–560.PubMedCrossRef Vandenberghe, R., Nobre, A. C., & Price, C. J. (2002). The response of left temporal cortex to sentences. Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience, 14, 550–560.PubMedCrossRef
go back to reference Vigliocco, G., Vinson, D. P., Lewis, W., & Garrett, M. F. (2004). Representing meanings of object and action words: The featural and unitary semantic space hypothesis. Cognitive Psychology, 48, 422–488.PubMedCrossRef Vigliocco, G., Vinson, D. P., Lewis, W., & Garrett, M. F. (2004). Representing meanings of object and action words: The featural and unitary semantic space hypothesis. Cognitive Psychology, 48, 422–488.PubMedCrossRef
go back to reference Villani, C., Lugli, L., Liuzza, M. T., & Borghi, A. M. (2019). Varieties of abstract concepts and their multiple dimensions. Language and Cognition, 11(3), 403–430.CrossRef Villani, C., Lugli, L., Liuzza, M. T., & Borghi, A. M. (2019). Varieties of abstract concepts and their multiple dimensions. Language and Cognition, 11(3), 403–430.CrossRef
go back to reference Vygotsky, L. S. (1986). Thought and language. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press. Vygotsky, L. S. (1986). Thought and language. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.
go back to reference Wang, J., Conder, J. A., Blitzer, D. N., & Shinkareva, S. V. (2010). Neural representation of abstract and concrete concepts: a meta-analysis of imaging studies. Human Brain Mapping, 31(10), 1459–1468.PubMedPubMedCentralCrossRef Wang, J., Conder, J. A., Blitzer, D. N., & Shinkareva, S. V. (2010). Neural representation of abstract and concrete concepts: a meta-analysis of imaging studies. Human Brain Mapping, 31(10), 1459–1468.PubMedPubMedCentralCrossRef
go back to reference Wang, X., Men, W., Gao, J., Caramazza, A., & Bi, Y. (2020). Two forms of knowledge representations in the human brain. Neuron, 107(2), 383–393.e5.PubMedCrossRef Wang, X., Men, W., Gao, J., Caramazza, A., & Bi, Y. (2020). Two forms of knowledge representations in the human brain. Neuron, 107(2), 383–393.e5.PubMedCrossRef
go back to reference Warrington, E. K., & Shallice, T. (1984). Category specific semantic impairments. Brain, 107, 829–854.PubMedCrossRef Warrington, E. K., & Shallice, T. (1984). Category specific semantic impairments. Brain, 107, 829–854.PubMedCrossRef
go back to reference Winter, B., Marghetis, T., & Matlock, T. (2015). Of magnitudes and metaphors: explaining cognitive interactions between space, time, and number. Cortex; a journal devoted to the study of the nervous system and behavior, 64, 209–224.PubMedCrossRef Winter, B., Marghetis, T., & Matlock, T. (2015). Of magnitudes and metaphors: explaining cognitive interactions between space, time, and number. Cortex; a journal devoted to the study of the nervous system and behavior, 64, 209–224.PubMedCrossRef
go back to reference Yee, E. (2019). Abstraction and concepts: when, how, where, what and why? Language, Cognition and Neuroscience, 34(10), 1257–1265.CrossRef Yee, E. (2019). Abstraction and concepts: when, how, where, what and why? Language, Cognition and Neuroscience, 34(10), 1257–1265.CrossRef
go back to reference Yi, H. A., Moore, P., & Grossman, M. (2007). Reversal of the concreteness effect for verbs in semantic dementia. Neuropsychology, 21(1), 9–19.PubMedCrossRef Yi, H. A., Moore, P., & Grossman, M. (2007). Reversal of the concreteness effect for verbs in semantic dementia. Neuropsychology, 21(1), 9–19.PubMedCrossRef
go back to reference Zannino, G.D., Fini, C., Benassi, M., Carlesimo, G.A., Borghi, A.M. (2020). Articulatory suppression and processing of abstract concepts: the role of inner speech. osf.io/npmeh. (under review) Zannino, G.D., Fini, C., Benassi, M., Carlesimo, G.A., Borghi, A.M. (2020). Articulatory suppression and processing of abstract concepts: the role of inner speech. osf.io/npmeh. (under review)
go back to reference Zdrazilova, L., Sidhu, D. M., & Pexman, P. M. (2018). Communicating abstract meaning: concepts revealed in words and gestures. Philosophical Transactions of the Royal Society B: Biological Sciences, 373(1752), 20170138.CrossRef Zdrazilova, L., Sidhu, D. M., & Pexman, P. M. (2018). Communicating abstract meaning: concepts revealed in words and gestures. Philosophical Transactions of the Royal Society B: Biological Sciences, 373(1752), 20170138.CrossRef
Metagegevens
Titel
Words have a weight: language as a source of inner grounding and flexibility in abstract concepts
Auteurs
Guy Dove
Laura Barca
Luca Tummolini
Anna M. Borghi
Publicatiedatum
10-11-2020
Uitgeverij
Springer Berlin Heidelberg
Gepubliceerd in
Psychological Research / Uitgave 8/2022
Print ISSN: 0340-0727
Elektronisch ISSN: 1430-2772
DOI
https://doi.org/10.1007/s00426-020-01438-6

Andere artikelen Uitgave 8/2022

Psychological Research 8/2022 Naar de uitgave