Skip to main content
Top
Gepubliceerd in: Psychological Research 2/2023

23-03-2022 | Original Article

The visual encoding of graspable unfamiliar objects

Auteurs: Giovanni Federico, François Osiurak, Maria Antonella Brandimonte, Marco Salvatore, Carlo Cavaliere

Gepubliceerd in: Psychological Research | Uitgave 2/2023

Log in om toegang te krijgen
share
DELEN

Deel dit onderdeel of sectie (kopieer de link)

  • Optie A:
    Klik op de rechtermuisknop op de link en selecteer de optie “linkadres kopiëren”
  • Optie B:
    Deel de link per e-mail

Abstract

We explored by eye-tracking the visual encoding modalities of participants (N = 20) involved in a free-observation task in which three repetitions of ten unfamiliar graspable objects were administered. Then, we analysed the temporal allocation (t = 1500 ms) of visual-spatial attention to objects’ manipulation (i.e., the part aimed at grasping the object) and functional (i.e., the part aimed at recognizing the function and identity of the object) areas. Within the first 750 ms, participants tended to shift their gaze on the functional areas while decreasing their attention on the manipulation areas. Then, participants reversed this trend, decreasing their visual-spatial attention to the functional areas while fixing the manipulation areas relatively more. Crucially, the global amount of visual-spatial attention for objects’ functional areas significantly decreased as an effect of stimuli repetition while remaining stable for the manipulation areas, thus indicating stimulus familiarity effects. These findings support the action reappraisal theoretical approach, which considers object/tool processing as abilities emerging from semantic, technical/mechanical, and sensorimotor knowledge integration.
Voetnoten
1
The spatial disposition we used in this study represents the worst condition should one wish to emphasize semantic effects in modulating the temporal allocation of visual-spatial attention. Hence, we chose the worst experimental scenario with respect to the action reappraisal (Federico & Brandimonte, 2019).
 
Literatuur
go back to reference Almeida, J., Fintzi, A. R., & Mahon, B. Z. (2013). Tool manipulation knowledge is retrieved by way of the ventral visual object processing pathway. Cortex, 49(9), 2334–2344.PubMedPubMedCentralCrossRef Almeida, J., Fintzi, A. R., & Mahon, B. Z. (2013). Tool manipulation knowledge is retrieved by way of the ventral visual object processing pathway. Cortex, 49(9), 2334–2344.PubMedPubMedCentralCrossRef
go back to reference Ambrosini, E., & Costantini, M. (2017). Body posture differentially impacts on visual attention towards tool, graspable, and non-graspable objects. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 43(2), 360.PubMed Ambrosini, E., & Costantini, M. (2017). Body posture differentially impacts on visual attention towards tool, graspable, and non-graspable objects. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 43(2), 360.PubMed
go back to reference Bar, M., Kassam, K. S., Ghuman, A. S., Boshyan, J., Schmid, A. M., Dale, A. M., Hämäläinen, M. S., Marinkovic, K., Schacter, D. L., Rosen, B. R., & Halgren, E. (2006). Top-down facilitation of visual recognition. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America, 103(2), 449–454.PubMedPubMedCentralCrossRef Bar, M., Kassam, K. S., Ghuman, A. S., Boshyan, J., Schmid, A. M., Dale, A. M., Hämäläinen, M. S., Marinkovic, K., Schacter, D. L., Rosen, B. R., & Halgren, E. (2006). Top-down facilitation of visual recognition. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America, 103(2), 449–454.PubMedPubMedCentralCrossRef
go back to reference Bortoletto, M., & Cunnington, R. (2010). Motor timing and motor sequencing contribute differently to the preparation for voluntary movement. NeuroImage, 49(4), 3338–3348.PubMedCrossRef Bortoletto, M., & Cunnington, R. (2010). Motor timing and motor sequencing contribute differently to the preparation for voluntary movement. NeuroImage, 49(4), 3338–3348.PubMedCrossRef
go back to reference Caspers, S., Geyer, S., Schleicher, A., Mohlberg, H., Amunts, K., & Zilles, K. (2006). The human inferior parietal cortex: Cytoarchitectonic parcellation and interindividual variability. NeuroImage, 33(2), 430–448.PubMedCrossRef Caspers, S., Geyer, S., Schleicher, A., Mohlberg, H., Amunts, K., & Zilles, K. (2006). The human inferior parietal cortex: Cytoarchitectonic parcellation and interindividual variability. NeuroImage, 33(2), 430–448.PubMedCrossRef
go back to reference Chen, Q., Garcea, F. E., Jacobs, R. A., & Mahon, B. Z. (2018). Abstract representations of object-directed action in the left inferior parietal lobule. Cerebral Cortex, 28(6), 2162–2174.PubMedCrossRef Chen, Q., Garcea, F. E., Jacobs, R. A., & Mahon, B. Z. (2018). Abstract representations of object-directed action in the left inferior parietal lobule. Cerebral Cortex, 28(6), 2162–2174.PubMedCrossRef
go back to reference Cohen, J. (2013). Statistical power analysis for the behavioral sciences. Routledge.CrossRef Cohen, J. (2013). Statistical power analysis for the behavioral sciences. Routledge.CrossRef
go back to reference Cousineau, D. (2005). Confidence intervals in within-subject designs: A simpler solution to Loftus and Masson’s method. Tutorials in Quantitative Methods for Psychology, 1(1), 42–45.CrossRef Cousineau, D. (2005). Confidence intervals in within-subject designs: A simpler solution to Loftus and Masson’s method. Tutorials in Quantitative Methods for Psychology, 1(1), 42–45.CrossRef
go back to reference De Bellis, F., Magliacano, A., Sagliano, L., Conson, M., Grossi, D., & Trojano, L. (2020). Left inferior parietal and posterior temporal cortices mediate the effect of action observation on semantic processing of objects: Evidence from rTMS. Psychological Research Psychologische Forschung, 84(4), 1006–1019.PubMedCrossRef De Bellis, F., Magliacano, A., Sagliano, L., Conson, M., Grossi, D., & Trojano, L. (2020). Left inferior parietal and posterior temporal cortices mediate the effect of action observation on semantic processing of objects: Evidence from rTMS. Psychological Research Psychologische Forschung, 84(4), 1006–1019.PubMedCrossRef
go back to reference Decroix, J., & Kalénine, S. (2019). What first drives visual attention during the recognition of object-directed actions? The role of kinematics and goal information. Attention, Perception, & Psychophysics, 81(7), 2400–2409.CrossRef Decroix, J., & Kalénine, S. (2019). What first drives visual attention during the recognition of object-directed actions? The role of kinematics and goal information. Attention, Perception, & Psychophysics, 81(7), 2400–2409.CrossRef
go back to reference Faul, F., Erdfelder, E., Lang, A. G., & Buchner, A. (2007). G* Power 3: A flexible statistical power analysis program for the social, behavioral, and biomedical sciences. Behavior Research Methods, 39(2), 175–191.PubMedCrossRef Faul, F., Erdfelder, E., Lang, A. G., & Buchner, A. (2007). G* Power 3: A flexible statistical power analysis program for the social, behavioral, and biomedical sciences. Behavior Research Methods, 39(2), 175–191.PubMedCrossRef
go back to reference Federico, G., & Brandimonte, M. A. (2019). Tool and object affordances: an ecological eye-tracking study. Brain and Cognition, 135, 103582.PubMedCrossRef Federico, G., & Brandimonte, M. A. (2019). Tool and object affordances: an ecological eye-tracking study. Brain and Cognition, 135, 103582.PubMedCrossRef
go back to reference Federico, G., & Brandimonte, M. A. (2020). Looking to recognise: The pre-eminence of semantic over sensorimotor processing in human tool use. Scientific Reports, 10(1), 1–16.CrossRef Federico, G., & Brandimonte, M. A. (2020). Looking to recognise: The pre-eminence of semantic over sensorimotor processing in human tool use. Scientific Reports, 10(1), 1–16.CrossRef
go back to reference Federico, G., & Brandimonte, M. A. (2022). Il ruolo del ragionamento e dell’elaborazione semantica nell’uso di utensili: La prospettiva integrata dell’action reappraisal. TOPIC-Temi Di Psicologia Dell’ordine Degli Psicologi Della Campania, 1(1), 10–53240. Federico, G., & Brandimonte, M. A. (2022). Il ruolo del ragionamento e dell’elaborazione semantica nell’uso di utensili: La prospettiva integrata dell’action reappraisal. TOPIC-Temi Di Psicologia Dell’ordine Degli Psicologi Della Campania, 1(1), 10–53240.
go back to reference Federico, G., Ferrante, D., Marcatto, F., & Brandimonte, M. A. (2021a). How the fear of COVID-19 changed the way we look at human faces. PeerJ, 9, e11380.PubMedPubMedCentralCrossRef Federico, G., Ferrante, D., Marcatto, F., & Brandimonte, M. A. (2021a). How the fear of COVID-19 changed the way we look at human faces. PeerJ, 9, e11380.PubMedPubMedCentralCrossRef
go back to reference Federico, G., Osiurak, F., Reynaud, E., & Brandimonte, M. A. (2021c). Semantic congruency effects of prime words on tool visual exploration. Brain and Cognition, 152, 105758.PubMedCrossRef Federico, G., Osiurak, F., Reynaud, E., & Brandimonte, M. A. (2021c). Semantic congruency effects of prime words on tool visual exploration. Brain and Cognition, 152, 105758.PubMedCrossRef
go back to reference Gibson, J. J. (1977). The theory of affordances. Hilldale, USA, 1(2), 67–82. Gibson, J. J. (1977). The theory of affordances. Hilldale, USA, 1(2), 67–82.
go back to reference Gomez, M. A., Skiba, R. M., & Snow, J. C. (2018). Graspable objects grab attention more than images do. Psychological Science, 29(2), 206–218.PubMedCrossRef Gomez, M. A., Skiba, R. M., & Snow, J. C. (2018). Graspable objects grab attention more than images do. Psychological Science, 29(2), 206–218.PubMedCrossRef
go back to reference Grezes, J., & Decety, J. (2002). Does visual perception of object afford action? Evidence from a Neuroimaging Study. Neuropsychologia, 40(2), 212–222.PubMedCrossRef Grezes, J., & Decety, J. (2002). Does visual perception of object afford action? Evidence from a Neuroimaging Study. Neuropsychologia, 40(2), 212–222.PubMedCrossRef
go back to reference Handy, T. C., Grafton, S. T., Shroff, N. M., Ketay, S., & Gazzaniga, M. S. (2003). Graspable objects grab attention when the potential for action is recognized. Nature Neuroscience, 6(4), 421–427.PubMedCrossRef Handy, T. C., Grafton, S. T., Shroff, N. M., Ketay, S., & Gazzaniga, M. S. (2003). Graspable objects grab attention when the potential for action is recognized. Nature Neuroscience, 6(4), 421–427.PubMedCrossRef
go back to reference Horst, J. S., & Hout, M. C. (2016). The Novel Object and Unusual Name (NOUN) Database: A collection of novel images for use in experimental research. Behavior Research Methods, 48(4), 1393–1409.PubMedCrossRef Horst, J. S., & Hout, M. C. (2016). The Novel Object and Unusual Name (NOUN) Database: A collection of novel images for use in experimental research. Behavior Research Methods, 48(4), 1393–1409.PubMedCrossRef
go back to reference Humphreys, G. W., Kumar, S., Yoon, E. Y., Wulff, M., Roberts, K. L., & Riddoch, M. J. (2013). Attending to the possibilities of action. Philosophical Transactions of the Royal Society b: Biological Sciences, 368(1628), 20130059.CrossRef Humphreys, G. W., Kumar, S., Yoon, E. Y., Wulff, M., Roberts, K. L., & Riddoch, M. J. (2013). Attending to the possibilities of action. Philosophical Transactions of the Royal Society b: Biological Sciences, 368(1628), 20130059.CrossRef
go back to reference Humphreys, G. F., Lambon Ralph, M. A. L., & Simons, J. S. (2021). A unifying account of angular gyrus contributions to episodic and semantic cognition. Trends in Neurosciences., 44, 452–463.PubMedCrossRef Humphreys, G. F., Lambon Ralph, M. A. L., & Simons, J. S. (2021). A unifying account of angular gyrus contributions to episodic and semantic cognition. Trends in Neurosciences., 44, 452–463.PubMedCrossRef
go back to reference Ishibashi, R., Pobric, G., Saito, S., & Lambon Ralph, M. A. (2016). The neural network for tool-related cognition: An activation likelihood estimation meta-analysis of 70 neuroimaging contrasts. Cognitive Neuropsychology, 33(3–4), 241–256.PubMedPubMedCentralCrossRef Ishibashi, R., Pobric, G., Saito, S., & Lambon Ralph, M. A. (2016). The neural network for tool-related cognition: An activation likelihood estimation meta-analysis of 70 neuroimaging contrasts. Cognitive Neuropsychology, 33(3–4), 241–256.PubMedPubMedCentralCrossRef
go back to reference Koechlin, E., & Summerfield, C. (2007). An information theoretical approach to prefrontal executive function. Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 11(6), 229–235.PubMedCrossRef Koechlin, E., & Summerfield, C. (2007). An information theoretical approach to prefrontal executive function. Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 11(6), 229–235.PubMedCrossRef
go back to reference Lambon Ralph, M. A., Jefferies, E., Patterson, K., & Rogers, T. T. (2017). The neural and computational bases of semantic cognition. Nature Reviews Neuroscience, 18(1), 42–55.CrossRef Lambon Ralph, M. A., Jefferies, E., Patterson, K., & Rogers, T. T. (2017). The neural and computational bases of semantic cognition. Nature Reviews Neuroscience, 18(1), 42–55.CrossRef
go back to reference Lesourd, M., Servant, M., Baumard, J., Reynaud, E., Ecochard, C., Medjaoui, F. T., Bartolo, A., & Osiurak, F. (2021). Semantic and action tool knowledge in the brain: identifying common and distinct networks. Neuropsychologia, 159, 107918.PubMedCrossRef Lesourd, M., Servant, M., Baumard, J., Reynaud, E., Ecochard, C., Medjaoui, F. T., Bartolo, A., & Osiurak, F. (2021). Semantic and action tool knowledge in the brain: identifying common and distinct networks. Neuropsychologia, 159, 107918.PubMedCrossRef
go back to reference Loftus, G. R., & Masson, M. E. (1994). Using confidence intervals in within-subject designs. Psychonomic Bulletin & Review, 1(4), 476–490.CrossRef Loftus, G. R., & Masson, M. E. (1994). Using confidence intervals in within-subject designs. Psychonomic Bulletin & Review, 1(4), 476–490.CrossRef
go back to reference Massen, C., & Prinz, W. (2007). Programming tool-use actions. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 33(3), 692.PubMed Massen, C., & Prinz, W. (2007). Programming tool-use actions. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 33(3), 692.PubMed
go back to reference Masson, M. E., Bub, D. N., & Breuer, A. T. (2011). Priming of reach and grasp actions by handled objects. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 37(5), 1470.PubMed Masson, M. E., Bub, D. N., & Breuer, A. T. (2011). Priming of reach and grasp actions by handled objects. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 37(5), 1470.PubMed
go back to reference Milner, A. D., & Goodale, M. A. (2008). Two visual systems re-viewed. Neuropsychologia, 46(3), 774–785.PubMedCrossRef Milner, A. D., & Goodale, M. A. (2008). Two visual systems re-viewed. Neuropsychologia, 46(3), 774–785.PubMedCrossRef
go back to reference Mirman, D. (2014). Growth curve analysis: A hands-on tutorial on using multilevel regression to analyze time course data. In: Proceedings of the Annual Meeting of the Cognitive Science Society (Vol. 36, No. 36). Mirman, D. (2014). Growth curve analysis: A hands-on tutorial on using multilevel regression to analyze time course data. In: Proceedings of the Annual Meeting of the Cognitive Science Society (Vol. 36, No. 36).
go back to reference Myachykov, A., Ellis, R., Cangelosi, A., & Fischer, M. H. (2013). Visual and linguistic cues to graspable objects. Experimental Brain Research, 229(4), 545–559.PubMedCrossRef Myachykov, A., Ellis, R., Cangelosi, A., & Fischer, M. H. (2013). Visual and linguistic cues to graspable objects. Experimental Brain Research, 229(4), 545–559.PubMedCrossRef
go back to reference Naish, K. R., Reader, A. T., Houston-Price, C., Bremner, A. J., & Holmes, N. P. (2013). To eat or not to eat? Kinematics and muscle activity of reach-to-grasp movements are influenced by the action goal, but observers do not detect these differences. Experimental Brain Research, 225(2), 261–275.PubMedCrossRef Naish, K. R., Reader, A. T., Houston-Price, C., Bremner, A. J., & Holmes, N. P. (2013). To eat or not to eat? Kinematics and muscle activity of reach-to-grasp movements are influenced by the action goal, but observers do not detect these differences. Experimental Brain Research, 225(2), 261–275.PubMedCrossRef
go back to reference Natraj, N., Pella, Y. M., Borghi, A. M., & Wheaton, L. A. (2015). The visual encoding of tool–object affordances. Neuroscience, 310, 512–527.PubMedCrossRef Natraj, N., Pella, Y. M., Borghi, A. M., & Wheaton, L. A. (2015). The visual encoding of tool–object affordances. Neuroscience, 310, 512–527.PubMedCrossRef
go back to reference Nicholson, T., Roser, M., & Bach, P. (2017). Understanding the goals of everyday instrumental actions is primarily linked to object, not motor-kinematic, information: Evidence from fMRI. PLoS ONE, 12(1), e0169700.PubMedPubMedCentralCrossRef Nicholson, T., Roser, M., & Bach, P. (2017). Understanding the goals of everyday instrumental actions is primarily linked to object, not motor-kinematic, information: Evidence from fMRI. PLoS ONE, 12(1), e0169700.PubMedPubMedCentralCrossRef
go back to reference Osiurak, F., & Badets, A. (2014). Pliers, not fingers: Tool-action effect in a motor intention paradigm. Cognition, 130(1), 66–73.PubMedCrossRef Osiurak, F., & Badets, A. (2014). Pliers, not fingers: Tool-action effect in a motor intention paradigm. Cognition, 130(1), 66–73.PubMedCrossRef
go back to reference Oldfield, R. C. (1971). The assessment and analysis of handedness: The Edinburgh inventory. Neuropsychologia, 9, 97–113.PubMedCrossRef Oldfield, R. C. (1971). The assessment and analysis of handedness: The Edinburgh inventory. Neuropsychologia, 9, 97–113.PubMedCrossRef
go back to reference Osiurak, F., Rossetti, Y., & Badets, A. (2017). What is an affordance? 40 years later. Neuroscience & Biobehavioral Reviews, 77, 403–417.CrossRef Osiurak, F., Rossetti, Y., & Badets, A. (2017). What is an affordance? 40 years later. Neuroscience & Biobehavioral Reviews, 77, 403–417.CrossRef
go back to reference Papoutsaki, A., Sangkloy, P., Laskey, J., Daskalova, N., Huang, J., & Hays, J. (2016). WebGazer: Scalable webcam eye tracking using user interactions. In: Proceedings of the twenty-fifth international joint conference on artificial intelligence-IJCAI 2016. Papoutsaki, A., Sangkloy, P., Laskey, J., Daskalova, N., Huang, J., & Hays, J. (2016). WebGazer: Scalable webcam eye tracking using user interactions. In: Proceedings of the twenty-fifth international joint conference on artificial intelligence-IJCAI 2016.
go back to reference Reynaud, E., Lesourd, M., Navarro, J., & Osiurak, F. (2016). On the neurocognitive origins of human tool use: A critical review of neuroimaging data. Neuroscience & Biobehavioral Reviews, 64, 421–437.CrossRef Reynaud, E., Lesourd, M., Navarro, J., & Osiurak, F. (2016). On the neurocognitive origins of human tool use: A critical review of neuroimaging data. Neuroscience & Biobehavioral Reviews, 64, 421–437.CrossRef
go back to reference Riddoch, M. J., Humphreys, G. W., Edwards, S., Baker, T., & Willson, K. (2003). Seeing the action: Neuropsychological evidence for action-based effects on object selection. Nature Neuroscience, 6(1), 82–89.PubMedCrossRef Riddoch, M. J., Humphreys, G. W., Edwards, S., Baker, T., & Willson, K. (2003). Seeing the action: Neuropsychological evidence for action-based effects on object selection. Nature Neuroscience, 6(1), 82–89.PubMedCrossRef
go back to reference Rizzolatti, G., & Matelli, M. (2003). Two different streams form the dorsal visual system: Anatomy and functions. Experimental Brain Research, 153(2), 146–157.PubMedCrossRef Rizzolatti, G., & Matelli, M. (2003). Two different streams form the dorsal visual system: Anatomy and functions. Experimental Brain Research, 153(2), 146–157.PubMedCrossRef
go back to reference Roberts, K. L., & Humphreys, G. W. (2010). Action relationships concatenate representations of separate objects in the ventral visual system. NeuroImage, 52(4), 1541–1548.PubMedCrossRef Roberts, K. L., & Humphreys, G. W. (2010). Action relationships concatenate representations of separate objects in the ventral visual system. NeuroImage, 52(4), 1541–1548.PubMedCrossRef
go back to reference Rothkegel, L. O., Trukenbrod, H. A., Schütt, H. H., Wichmann, F. A., & Engbert, R. (2017). Temporal evolution of the central fixation bias in scene viewing. Journal of Vision, 17(13), 3–3.PubMedCrossRef Rothkegel, L. O., Trukenbrod, H. A., Schütt, H. H., Wichmann, F. A., & Engbert, R. (2017). Temporal evolution of the central fixation bias in scene viewing. Journal of Vision, 17(13), 3–3.PubMedCrossRef
go back to reference Semmelmann, K., & Weigelt, S. (2018). Online webcam-based eye tracking in cognitive science: A first look. Behavior Research Methods, 50(2), 451–465.PubMedCrossRef Semmelmann, K., & Weigelt, S. (2018). Online webcam-based eye tracking in cognitive science: A first look. Behavior Research Methods, 50(2), 451–465.PubMedCrossRef
go back to reference Tamaki, Y., Nobusako, S., Takamura, Y., Miyawaki, Y., Terada, M., & Morioka, S. (2020). Effects of tool novelty and action demands on gaze searching during tool observation. Frontiers in Psychology, 11, 3060.CrossRef Tamaki, Y., Nobusako, S., Takamura, Y., Miyawaki, Y., Terada, M., & Morioka, S. (2020). Effects of tool novelty and action demands on gaze searching during tool observation. Frontiers in Psychology, 11, 3060.CrossRef
go back to reference Thill, S., Caligiore, D., Borghi, A. M., Ziemke, T., & Baldassarre, G. (2013). Theories and computational models of affordance and mirror systems: An integrative review. Neuroscience & Biobehavioral Reviews, 37(3), 491–521.CrossRef Thill, S., Caligiore, D., Borghi, A. M., Ziemke, T., & Baldassarre, G. (2013). Theories and computational models of affordance and mirror systems: An integrative review. Neuroscience & Biobehavioral Reviews, 37(3), 491–521.CrossRef
go back to reference Tucker, M., & Ellis, R. (1998). On the relations between seen objects and components of potential actions. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 24(3), 830.PubMed Tucker, M., & Ellis, R. (1998). On the relations between seen objects and components of potential actions. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 24(3), 830.PubMed
go back to reference Van Der Linden, L., Mathôt, S., & Vitu, F. (2015). The role of object affordances and center of gravity in eye movements toward isolated daily-life objects. Journal of Vision, 15(5), 8–8.PubMedCrossRef Van Der Linden, L., Mathôt, S., & Vitu, F. (2015). The role of object affordances and center of gravity in eye movements toward isolated daily-life objects. Journal of Vision, 15(5), 8–8.PubMedCrossRef
go back to reference Van Elk, M., Van Schie, H. T., & Bekkering, H. (2008). Conceptual knowledge for understanding other’s actions is organized primarily around action goals. Experimental Brain Research, 189(1), 99–107.PubMedPubMedCentralCrossRef Van Elk, M., Van Schie, H. T., & Bekkering, H. (2008). Conceptual knowledge for understanding other’s actions is organized primarily around action goals. Experimental Brain Research, 189(1), 99–107.PubMedPubMedCentralCrossRef
go back to reference Wurm, M. F., & Caramazza, A. (2019). Distinct roles of temporal and frontoparietal cortex in representing actions across vision and language. Nature Communications, 10(1), 1–10.CrossRef Wurm, M. F., & Caramazza, A. (2019). Distinct roles of temporal and frontoparietal cortex in representing actions across vision and language. Nature Communications, 10(1), 1–10.CrossRef
Metagegevens
Titel
The visual encoding of graspable unfamiliar objects
Auteurs
Giovanni Federico
François Osiurak
Maria Antonella Brandimonte
Marco Salvatore
Carlo Cavaliere
Publicatiedatum
23-03-2022
Uitgeverij
Springer Berlin Heidelberg
Gepubliceerd in
Psychological Research / Uitgave 2/2023
Print ISSN: 0340-0727
Elektronisch ISSN: 1430-2772
DOI
https://doi.org/10.1007/s00426-022-01673-z

Andere artikelen Uitgave 2/2023

Psychological Research 2/2023 Naar de uitgave