Skip to main content
Log in

Four ways of (mis-)conceiving embodiment in tool use

  • Minds in Skilled Performance
  • Published:
Synthese Aims and scope Submit manuscript

Abstract

A broader conception of the user’s perceptual, cognitive, and motor capabilities considers tools as body extensions. By identifying specific tool-related motor-grounded mechanisms, the embodied approach assumes that this “extensional phenomenon” takes place not only at a behavioral level but also at a psychological level. At least four ways of conceiving embodiment in tool use have been offered in relation to the concepts of incorporation, perception, knowledge, and observation. Nevertheless, the validity of these conceptions has been rarely, if not never, assessed. In this article, we attempt to fill this gap by discussing each of these conceptions in turn, with the aim of determining whether it is justified to consider tools as detached objects of a special sort in embodied terms. We argue that tool incorporation is made possible by “distalization”, that is, an embodied mechanism specific to tool use. Nevertheless, there is neither empirical nor theoretical support for the hypothesis that specific tool-related embodied mechanisms are involved in perception, knowledge, and observation. In broad terms, there is a tendency in the literature to overinterpret tool use as an embodied phenomenon at a psychological level. Inevitably, this limitation leads us to under-intellectualize the underlying cognitive processes and, as a result, it prevents us from understanding the technical-reasoning skills that allow humans to transform dramatically the physical world.

This is a preview of subscription content, log in via an institution to check access.

Access this article

Price excludes VAT (USA)
Tax calculation will be finalised during checkout.

Instant access to the full article PDF.

Similar content being viewed by others

Notes

  1. Beck (1980) and Shumaker et al. (2011) have clearly argued that their definitions of tool use and construction are behavioral descriptions that do not imply any psychological or biological prerequisites.

  2. Here the term object does not refer only to an object with finite boundaries as a stick or a stone but can also include an object with no finite boundaries such as water or ground.

  3. Gibson (1986) was in line with this, mentioning several times the case of “missiles” to illustrate his description of tools.

  4. Patients with extinction can perceive a contralateral stimulus in isolation but can fail to perceive the same stimulus when presented together with an ipsilesional one. This phenomenon can arise cross-modally (e.g., visual-tactile extinction).

  5. Distalization does not concern all the instances of tool use as defined by Shumaker et al. (2011), particularly in cases where the tool is thrown (i.e., throwing actions). We will not discuss further this aspect. Nevertheless, note that if distalization is considered as the core criterion of tool use, then throwing actions should be excluded from the category of tool-use actions (for a similar view, see Fragaszy and Mangalam 2018)..

  6. This proposal also allows to draw a sharp distinction between “cloth-like objects” (e.g., gloves, shoes) and tools inasmuch as cloth-like objects do not need to be controlled differently to the body-only system contrary to tools.

  7. In other words, distalization can be both innate and learnt.

  8. The notion of physical understanding does not imply that an individual is able to reason about the physical world at an analogical level. As discussed in more details in Sect. 4.2., the ability to transfer what is understood technically from a situation to another situation is the key feature of technical reasoning (Osiurak et al. 2010).

  9. It would be more correct to call these tool-centered affordances “xx-with-ability” rather than “xx-ability” (e.g., cut-with-ability instead of cut-ability), because they do not correspond to the actions that an individual can perform on but rather with the tool compared to user-centered affordances “xx-ability” (e.g., grasp-ability). This nomenclature was, for instance, used by Wagman and Carello (2001).

  10. For this reason, Osiurak et al. (2017) recently proposed to limit the term affordances to the opportunities for action offered by the environment in terms of “motor actions”, and to use the term “mechanical action” when the opportunities for action concerns the interaction between two external objects. Note that one of these two external objects can be the body of the user (i.e., reflective actions, such as brushing teeth). In this case, the body is the “object” of the mechanical action.

  11. This proposal is sometimes misinterpreted, as if it suggests that the acquisition of our knowledge about the physical world (i.e., mechanical knowledge) does not emerge from our interactions with it. As demonstrated by a significant body of evidence, most of mechanical knowledge is acquired through our interactions with the physical world (e.g., Baillargeon et al. 1992; for a review, see Lockman 2000). In a way, most—if not all—scholars agree with that. Nevertheless, this does not necessarily imply that the knowledge acquired is embodied and corresponds to a simulation of the sensorimotor experience linked to its acquisition.

  12. The terms visuokinesthetic motor engram (Heilman et al. 1982), action lexicons (Rothi et al. 1991), or gesture engram (Buxbaum 2001), among others, have been employed as a synonym of manipulation knowledge.

  13. The manipulation-knowledge hypothesis can be viewed as an embodied approach to tool use because it posits that knowledge about how to use tools is based on specific tool-related motor representations. In this way, the technical-reasoning hypothesis is a non-embodied approach because it denies the existence of such tool-related motor representations.

  14. This cascade mechanism is obviously reiterative.

  15. Even if semantic knowledge appears to be neither necessary nor sufficient to actually use tools, it might be indispensable to create abstract and generalizable concepts associated with tools and objects (e.g., Lambon Ralph et al. 2017; De Bellis et al. 2018; Wurm and Caramazza 2019; see also: Bar et al. 2006; Rogers and McClelland 2004). Hence, whereas mechanical knowledge is necessary to actually use tools, semantic knowledge might be considered as a complementary addendum in order to construct object-related representations. Such knowledge might be usable in everyday life by a reasoning-based agent (i.e., humans or future artificial intelligence), in the context of a cognitive-oriented functioning. Such an integrated perspective has been recently suggested by Federico and Brandimonte (2020) by introducing the concept of “action reappraisal”. Furthermore, semantic knowledge can play a specific role in some specific tool-use situations, such as when people are asked to use tools presented in isolation (i.e., single tool use). Indeed, in such situations, semantic knowledge might be useful to inform the individual about the conventional use of the tool presented, thereby allowing the individual to generate the mechanical action associated through technical reasoning (Osiurak 2014). This can explain why patients with a selective semantic deficit can show difficulties when asked to show how to use a familiar tool presented in isolation but not when presented with its corresponding object (Corbett et al. 2015; Baumard et al. 2016; see also Sirigu et al. 1991; Hodges et al. 2000; Osiurak et al. 2008; Lesourd et al. 2016).

  16. Technical-reasoning skills have also been shown to be a good predictor of cumulative performance in micro-society experiments in which participants have to perform a task (e.g., building a tower) as members of a chain (Osiurak et al. 2016; De Oliveira et al. 2019; Osiurak et al. 2020b, in press; see also Osiurak et al. submitted)..

  17. According to the ecological/Gibsonian approach, animals can perceive directly the opportunities for action provided by the environment (i.e., affordances). As discussed above, this approach is viable only if affordances are considered as being restricted to the motor actions that are relative to the animal’s biomechanical/morphological properties (i.e., acting on). This approach can be articulated with the technical-reasoning hypothesis if we assume that technical reasoning is dedicated to form a representation of the tool-use action (i.e., acting with), which then guides the perception of the appropriate affordances (i.e., acting on; for discussion, see Osiurak et al. 2017). In this frame, we do not perceive affordances in order to determine how to use a tool with an object, but rather to realize the action intended by selecting and planning the appropriate motor actions. By contrast, it is difficult at a theoretical level to articulate the technical-reasoning hypothesis with the manipulation-knowledge hypothesis because both aim to explain how individuals determine how a tool has to be manipulated as a tool (i.e., either through technical reasoning or through the activation of manipulation knowledge). Interestingly, although both the ecological/Gibsonian approach and the manipulation-knowledge hypothesis can be labelled as embodied, they strongly diverge at a theoretical level (non-representational approach versus representational approach).

References

  • Anderson, S. J., Yamagishi, N., & Karavia, V. (2002). Attentional processes link perception and action. Proceedings of the Royal Society of London B, 269, 1225–1232.

    Google Scholar 

  • Arbib, M. A., Bonaiuto, J. B., Jacobs, S., & Frey, S. H. (2009). Tool use and the distalization of the end-effector. Psychological Research, 73, 441–462.

    Google Scholar 

  • Baber, C. (2003). Cognition and tool use: Forms of engagement in human and animal use of tools. London: Talyor & Francis.

    Google Scholar 

  • Baber, C. (2006). Cognitive aspects of tool use. Applied Ergonomics, 37, 3–15.

    Google Scholar 

  • Badets, A., Michelet, T., de Rugy, A., & Osiurak, F. (2017). Creating semantics in tool use. Cognitive Processing, 18, 129–134.

    Google Scholar 

  • Badets, A., & Osiurak, F. (2015). A goal-based mechanism for delayed motor intention: Considerations from motor skills, tool use and action memory. Psychological Research, 79, 345–360.

    Google Scholar 

  • Baillargeon, R., Needham, A., & DeVos, J. (1992). The development of young infants’ intuitions about support. Early Development and Parenting, 1, 69–78.

    Google Scholar 

  • Bar, M., Kassam, K. S., Ghuman, A. S., Boshyan, J., Schmid, A. M., Dale, A. M., et al. (2006). Top-down facilitation of visual recognition. Proceedings of the National Academy of Science USA, 103, 449–454.

    Google Scholar 

  • Barsalou, L. W. (2008). Grounded cognition. Annual Review of Psychology, 59, 617–645.

    Google Scholar 

  • Barsalou, L. W. (2009). Simulation, situated conceptualization, and prediction. Philosophical Transactions of the Royal Society B, 364, 1281–1289.

    Google Scholar 

  • Baumard, J., Lesourd, M., Jarry, C., Merck, C., Etcharry-Bouyx, F., Chauviré, V., et al. (2016). Tool use disorders in neurodegenerative diseases. Roles of semantic memory and technical reasoning. Cortex, 82, 119–132.

    Google Scholar 

  • Beck, B. B. (1980). Animal tool use behavior: The use and manufacture of tools by animals. New York: Garland Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Bhalla, M., & Proffitt, D. R. (1999). Visual-motor recalibration in geographical slant perception. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 25, 1076–1096.

    Google Scholar 

  • Bloesch, E. K., Davoli, C. C., Roth, N., Brockmole, J. R., & Abrams, R. A. (2012). Watch this! Observed tool use affects perceived distance. Psychonomic Bulletin & Review, 19, 177–183.

    Google Scholar 

  • Bonnier, P. (1905). L’Aschématie. Revue Neurologique, 13, 606–609.

    Google Scholar 

  • Borghi, A. M., & Riggio, L. (2009). Sentence comprehension and simulation of object temporary, canonical and stable affordances. Brain Research, 1253, 117–128.

    Google Scholar 

  • Boyd, R., & Richerson, P. (1985). Culture and the evolutionary process. Chicago: University of Chicago Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Boyd, R., Richerson, P. J., & Henrich, J. (2011). The cultural niche: Why social learning is essential for human adaptation. Proceedings of the National Academy of Science USA, 108, 10918–10925.

    Google Scholar 

  • Brass, M., & Heyes, C. (2005). Imitation: Is cognitive neuroscience solving the correspondence problem? Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 9, 489–495.

    Google Scholar 

  • Buxbaum, L. J. (2001). Ideomotor Apraxia: A call to action. Neurocase, 7, 445–448.

    Google Scholar 

  • Buxbaum, L. J. (2017). Learning, remembering, and predicting how to use tools: Distributed neurocognitive mechanism. Comment on Osiurak and Badets (2016). Psychological Review, 124, 346–360.

    Google Scholar 

  • Buxbaum, L. J., & Kalénine, S. (2010). Action knowledge, visuomotor activation, and embodiment in the two action systems. Annals of the New York Academy of Sciences, 1191, 201–218.

    Google Scholar 

  • Buxbaum, L. J., Kyle, K. M., & Menon, R. (2005). On beyond mirror neurons: Internal representations subserving imitation and recognition skilled object-related actions in humans. Cognitive Brain Research, 25, 226–239.

    Google Scholar 

  • Buxbaum, L. J., Schwartz, M. F., & Carew, T. G. (1997). The role of memory in object use. Cognitive Neuropsychology, 14, 219–254.

    Google Scholar 

  • Caligiore, D., Borghi, A. M., Parisi, D., & Baldassarre, G. (2010). TRoPICALS: A computational embodied neuroscience model of compatibility effects. Psychological Review, 117, 1188–1228.

    Google Scholar 

  • Cardinali, L., Frassinetti, F., Brozzolo, C., Urquizar, C., Roy, A. C., & Farnè, F. (2009). Tool-use induces morphological updating of the body schema. Current Biology, 19, 478–479.

    Google Scholar 

  • Cho, D. T., & Proctor, R. W. (2010). The object-based Simon effect: Grasping affordance or relative location of the graspable part? Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 36, 853–861.

    Google Scholar 

  • Cho, D. T., & Proctor, R. W. (2011). Correspondence effects for objects with opposing left and right protrusions. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 37, 737–749.

    Google Scholar 

  • Cho, D. T., & Proctor, R. W. (2013). Object-based correspondence effects for action-relevant and surface-property judgments with keypress responses: Evidence for a basis in spatial coding. Psychological Research, 77, 618–636.

    Google Scholar 

  • Corbett, F., Jefferies, E., Burns, A., & Lambon Ralph, M. A. (2015). Deregulated semantic cognition contributes to object-use deficits in Alzheimer’s disease: A comparison with semantic aphasia and semantic dementia. Journal of Neuropsychology, 9, 219–241.

    Google Scholar 

  • Danel, S., Osiurak, F., & von Bayern, A. M. P. (2017). From the age of 5 humans decide economically, whereas crows exhibit individual preferences. Scientific Reports, 7, 17043.

    Google Scholar 

  • Daprati, E., & Sirigu, A. (2006). How we interact with objects: Learning from brain lesions. Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 10, 265–270.

    Google Scholar 

  • Davoli, C. C., Brockmole, J. R., & Witt, J. K. (2012). Compressing perceived distance with remote tool-use: Real, imagined, and remembered. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 38, 80–89.

    Google Scholar 

  • De Bellis, F., Magliacano, A., Sagliano, L., Conson, M., Grossi, D., & Trojano, L. (2018). Left inferior parietal and posterior temporal cortices mediate the effect of action observation on semantic processing of objects: Evidence from rTMS. Psychological Research, 84, 1006–1019.

    Google Scholar 

  • De Grave, D. D. J., Brenner, E., & Smeets, J. B. J. (2011). Using a stick does not necessarily alter judged distances or reachability. PLoS ONE, 6, e16697.

    Google Scholar 

  • De Oliveira, E., Reynaud, E., & Osiurak, F. (2019). Roles of technical reasoning, theory of mind, creativity, and fluid cognition in cumulative technological culture. Human Nature, 30, 326–340.

    Google Scholar 

  • Decety, J., & Grezes, J. (1999). Neural mechanisms subserving the perception of human actions. Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 3, 172–178.

    Google Scholar 

  • Decety, J., Jeannerod, M., & Prablanc, C. (1989). The timing of mentally represented actions. Behavioural Brain Research, 34, 35–42.

    Google Scholar 

  • Decety, J., & Michel, F. (1989). Comparative analysis of actual and mental movement times in two graphic tasks. Brain and Cognition, 11, 87–97.

    Google Scholar 

  • Decroix, J., & Kalénine, S. (2018). Timing of grip and goal activation during action perception: A priming study. Experimental Brain Research, 236, 2411–2426.

    Google Scholar 

  • Decroix, J., & Kalénine, S. (2019). What first drives visual attention during the recognition of object-directed actions? The role of kinematics and goal information. Attention, Perception, & Psychophysics, 81, 2400–2409.

    Google Scholar 

  • Drillis, R. J. (1963). Folk norms and biomechanics. Human Factors, 5, 427–441.

    Google Scholar 

  • Durgin, F. H., Baird, J. A., Greenburg, M., Russell, R., Shaugnessy, K., & Waymouth, S. (2009). Who is being perceived? The experimental demands of wearing a backpack. Psychonomic Bulletin & Review, 16, 964–968.

    Google Scholar 

  • Farnè, A., Iriki, A., & Làdavas, E. (2005). Shaping multisensory action-space with tools: Evidence from patients with cross-modal extinction. Neuropsychologia, 43, 238–248.

    Google Scholar 

  • Farnè, A., & Làdavas, E. (2000). Dynamic size-change of hand peripersonal space following tool use. NeuroReport, 11, 1645–1659.

    Google Scholar 

  • Federico, G., & Brandimonte, M. A. (2019). Tool and object affordances: An ecological eye-tracking study. Brain and Cognition, 135, 103582.

    Google Scholar 

  • Federico, G., & Brandimonte, M. A. (2020). Looking to recognize: The pre-eminence of semantic over sensorimotor processing in human tool use. Scientific Reports, 10, 6157.

    Google Scholar 

  • Firestone, C., & Scholl, B. J. (2016). Cognition does not affect perception: Evaluating the evidence for “top-down” effects. Behavioral and Brain Sciences, e229, 1–77.

    Google Scholar 

  • Fragaszy, D. M., & Mangalam, M. (2018). Tooling. Advances in the Study of Behavior, 50, 177–241.

    Google Scholar 

  • Frak, V. G., Paulingnan, Y., & Jeannerod, M. (2001). Orientation of the opposition axis in mentally simulated grasping. Experimental Brain Research, 136, 120–127.

    Google Scholar 

  • Frey, S. H. (2007). What pus the how in where? Tool use and the divided visual stream hypothesis. Cortex, 43, 368–375.

    Google Scholar 

  • Gibson, J. J. (1986). The ecological approach to visual perception. New York: Taylor & Francis (Original work published 1979).

  • Goldenberg, G. (2013). Apraxia: The cognitive side of motor control. Oxford: Oxford University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Goldenberg, G., & Hagmann, S. (1998). Tool use and mechanical problem solving in apraxia. Neuropsychologia, 36, 581–589.

    Google Scholar 

  • Goldenberg, G., & Spatt, J. (2009). The neural basis of tool use. Brain, 132, 1645–1655.

    Google Scholar 

  • Hansell, M., & Ruxton, G. D. (2008). Setting tool use within the context of animal construction behaviour. Trends in Ecology & Evolution, 23, 73–78.

    Google Scholar 

  • Head, H., & Holmes, G. (1911). Sensory disturbances from cerebral lesions. Brain, 34, 102–254.

    Google Scholar 

  • Heilman, K. M., Maher, L. M., Greenwald, M. L., & Rothi, L. J. G. (1997). Conceptual apraxia from lateralized lesions. Neurology, 49, 457–464.

    Google Scholar 

  • Heilman, K. M., Rothi, L. J., & Valenstein, E. (1982). Two forms of ideomotor apraxia. Neurology, 32, 342–346.

    Google Scholar 

  • Hirose, N. (2002). An ecological approach to embodiment and cognition. Cognitive Systems Research, 3, 289–299.

    Google Scholar 

  • Hodges, J. R., Bozeat, S., Lambon Ralph, M. A., Patterson, K., & Spatt, J. (2000). The role of knowledge in object use: Evidence from semantic dementia. Brain, 123, 1913–1925.

    Google Scholar 

  • Horner, V., & Whiten, A. (2005). Causal knowledge and imitation/emulation switching in chimpanzees (Pan troglodytes) and children (Homo sapiens). Animal Cognition, 8, 164–181.

    Google Scholar 

  • Horner, V., Whiten, A., Flynn, E., & de Waal, F. B. M. (2006). Faithful replication of foraging techniques along cultural transmission chains by chimpanzees and children. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences USA, 103, 13878–13883.

    Google Scholar 

  • Humphreys, G. W. (2001). Objects, affordances, action. The Psychologist, 14, 408–412.

    Google Scholar 

  • Hunt, G. R. (1996). Manufacture and use of hook tools by New Caledonian crows. Nature, 379, 249–251.

    Google Scholar 

  • Hunt, G. R., & Gray, R. D. (2004). Direct observations of pandanus-tool manufacture and use by a New Caledonian crow (Corvus moneduloides). Animal Cognition, 7, 114–120.

    Google Scholar 

  • Iriki, A., Tanaka, M., & Iwamura, Y. (1996). Coding of modified body schema during tool use by macaque postcentral neurones. NeuroReport, 7, 2325–2330.

    Google Scholar 

  • Jarry, C., Osiurak, F., Delafuys, D., Chauviré, V., Etcharry-Bouyx, F., et al. (2013). Apraxia of tool use: More evidence for the technical reasoning hypothesis. Cortex, 49, 2322–2333.

    Google Scholar 

  • Jeannerod, M. (2001). Neural simulation of action: A unifying mechanism for motor cognition. NeuroImage, 14, 103–109.

    Google Scholar 

  • Johnson, S. H., Rotte, M., Grafton, S. T., Hinrichs, H., Gazzaniga, M. S., & Heinze, H. J. (2002). Selective activation of a parietofrontal circuit during implicitly imagined prehension. Neuroimage, 17, 1693–1704.

    Google Scholar 

  • Johnson-Frey, S. H., & Grafton, S. T. (2003). From “acting on” to “acting with”: The functional anatomy of action representation. In C. Prablanc, D. Pélisson, & Y. Rossetti (Eds.), Progress in brain research (pp. 127–139). New York: Elsevier.

    Google Scholar 

  • Johnson-Frey, S. H., Newman-Norlund, R., & Grafton, S. T. (2005). A distributed left hemisphere network active during planning of everyday tool use skills. Cerebral Cortex, 15, 681–695.

    Google Scholar 

  • Kenward, B., Weir, A. A. S., Rutz, C., & Kacelnik, A. (2005). Behavioural ecology: Tool manufacture by naive juvenile crows. Nature, 433, 121.

    Google Scholar 

  • Kostov, K., & Janyan, A. (in press). Critical bottom-up attentional factors in the handle orientation effect: Asymmetric luminance transients and object-center eccentricity relative to fixation. Psychological Research.

  • Lambon Ralph, M. A., Jefferies, E., Patterson, K., & Rogers, T. T. (2017). The neural and computational bases of semantic cognition. Nature Reviews Neuroscience, 18, 42–55.

    Google Scholar 

  • Lesourd, M., Baumard, J., Jarry, C., Etcharry-Bouyx, F., Belliard, S., Moreaud, O., et al. (2016). Mechanical problem-solving in Alzheimer’s disease and semantic dementia. Neuropsychology, 30, 612–623.

    Google Scholar 

  • Lesourd, M., Naëgelé, B., Jaillard, A., Detante, O., & Osiurak, F. (2020). Using tools efficiently despite defective hand posture: A single-case study. Cortex, 129, 406–422.

    Google Scholar 

  • Linkenauger, S. A., Witt, J. K., Stefanucci, J. K., Bakdash, J. Z., & Proffitt, D. R. (2009). The effects of handedness and reachability on perceived distance. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 35, 1649–1660.

    Google Scholar 

  • Lockman, J. J. (2000). A perception-action perspective on tool use development. Child Development, 71, 137–144.

    Google Scholar 

  • Malaivijitnond, S., Lekprayoon, C., Tandavanittj, N., Panha, S., Cheewatham, C., & Hamada, Y. (2007). Stone-tool usage by Thai long-tailed macaques (Macaca fascicularis). American Journal of Primatology, 69, 227–233.

    Google Scholar 

  • Mangalam, M. (2016). What makes a tool. In T. K. Shackelford & V. A. Weekes-Shackelford (Eds.), Encyclopedia of evolutionary psychological science (pp. 1–5). Cham: Springer.

    Google Scholar 

  • Mangalam, M., & Fragaszy, D. M. (2016). Transforming the body-only system into the body-plus-tool system. Animal Behaviour, 117, 115–122.

    Google Scholar 

  • Maravita, A., Husain, M., Clarke, K., & Driver, J. (2001). Reaching with a tool extends visual–tactile interactions into far space: evidence from cross-modal extinction. Neuropsychologia, 39, 580–585.

    Google Scholar 

  • Maravita, A., & Iriki, A. (2004). Tools for the body (schema). Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 8, 79–86.

    Google Scholar 

  • Massen, C., & Prinz, W. (2007). Activation of actions rules in action observation. Journal of Experimental Psychology. Learning, Memory, and Cognition, 33, 1118–1130.

    Google Scholar 

  • Massen, C., & Prinz, W. (2009). Movements, actions and tool-use actions: An ideomotor approach to imitation. Philosophical Transactions of the Royal Society of London B, 364, 2349–2358.

    Google Scholar 

  • Merleau-Ponty, M. (1962). Phenomenology of perception. New York: Routledge (Original work published 1945)

  • Miller, L. E., Montroni, L., Koun, E., Salemme, R., Hayward, V., & Farnè, A. (2018). Sensing with tools extends somatosensory processing beyond the body. Nature, 561, 239–242.

    Google Scholar 

  • Mizelle, J. C., & Wheaton, L. A. (2010). The neuroscience of storing and molding tool action concepts: How “plastic” is grounded cognition? Frontiers in Psychology, 1, 196.

    Google Scholar 

  • Molto, L., Morgado, N., Guinet, E., Fazioli, L., Heurley, L. P., & Palluel-Germain, R. (2020). Motor simulation in tool-use effect on distance estimation. A replication of Witt and Proffitt (2008). Psychonomic Bulletin & Review, 27, 301–306.

    Google Scholar 

  • Molto, L., Nalborczyk, L., Palluel-Germain, R., & Morgado, N. (in press). Action effects on visual perception of distances: A multilevel Bayesian meta-analysis. Psychological Science.

  • Morgado, N., Gentaz, E., Guinet, E., Osiurak, F., & Palluel-Germain, R. (2013). Within reach but no so reachable: Obstacles matter in visual perception of reaching distances. Psychonomic Bulletin & Review, 20, 462–467.

    Google Scholar 

  • Nagell, K., Olguin, R. S., & Tomasello, M. (1993). Processes of social learning in the tool use of chimpanzees (Pan troglodytes) and human children (Homo sapiens). Journal of Comparative Psychology, 107, 174–186.

    Google Scholar 

  • Naish, K. R., Reader, A. T., Houston-Price, C., Bremner, A. J., & Holmes, N. P. (2013). To eat or not to eat? Kinematics and muscle activity of reach-to-grasp movements are influenced by the action goal, but observers do not detect these differences. Experimental Brain Research, 225, 261–275.

    Google Scholar 

  • Negri, G. A., Rumiati, R. I., Zadini, A., Ukmar, M., Mahon, B. Z., & Caramazza, A. (2007). What is the role of motor simulation in action and object recognition? Evidence from apraxia. Cognitive Neuropsychology, 24, 795–816.

    Google Scholar 

  • Nicholson, T., Roser, M., & Bach, P. (2017). Understanding the goals of everyday instrumental actions is primarily linked to object, not motor-kinematic, information: Evidence from fMRI. PLoS ONE, 12, 1–21.

    Google Scholar 

  • Norman, D. A. (1999). Affordance, conventions, and design. Interactions, 6, 38–43.

    Google Scholar 

  • Osiurak, F. (2014). What neuropsychology tells us about human tool use? The four constraints theory (4CT): Mechanics, space, time, and effort. Neuropsychology Review, 24, 88–115.

    Google Scholar 

  • Osiurak, F. (2017). Cognitive paleoanthropology and technology: Toward a parsimonious theory (PATH). Review of General Psychology, 21, 292–307.

    Google Scholar 

  • Osiurak, F. (2020). The tool instinct. New York: Wiley.

    Google Scholar 

  • Osiurak, F., Aubin, G., Allain, P., Jarry, C., Richard, I., & Le Gall, D. (2008). Object utilization versus object usage: A single-case study. Neurocase, 14, 169–183.

    Google Scholar 

  • Osiurak, F., & Badets, A. (2014). Pliers, not fingers: Tool-action effect in a motor intention paradigm. Cognition, 130, 66–73.

    Google Scholar 

  • Osiurak, F., & Badets, A. (2016). Tool use and affordance: Manipulation-based versus reasoning-based approaches. Psychological Review, 123, 534–568.

    Google Scholar 

  • Osiurak, F., & Badets, A. (2017). Use of tools and misuse of embodied cognition. Psychological Review, 124, 361–368.

    Google Scholar 

  • Osiurak, F., Badets, A., Rossetti, Y., Lesourd, M., Navarro, J., & Reynaud, E. (2020a). Disembodying (tool use) action understanding. Neuroscience and Biobehavioral Reviews, 114, 229–231.

    Google Scholar 

  • Osiurak, F., Cretel, C., Duhau-Marmon, N., Fournier, I., Marignier, L., De Oliveira, E., Navarro, J., & Reynaud, E. (in press). The pedagogue, the engineer, and the friend: From whom do we learn? Human Nature.

  • Osiurak, F., & Danel, S. (2018). Dexterity and tool use: Beyond the embodied theory. Animal Behaviour, 139, e1–e4.

    Google Scholar 

  • Osiurak, F., De Oliveira, E., Navarro, J., Lesourd, M., Claidière, N., & Reynaud, E. (2016). Physical intelligence does matter to cumulative technological culture. Journal of Experimental Psychology: General, 145, 941–948.

    Google Scholar 

  • Osiurak, F., De Oliveira, E., Navarro, J., & Reynaud, E. (2020b). The castaway island: Distinct roles of theory of mind and technical reasoning in cumulative technological culture. Journal of Experimental Psychology: General, 149, 58–66.

    Google Scholar 

  • Osiurak, F., & Heinke, D. (2018). Looking for intoolligence: A unified framework for the cognitive study of human tool use and technology. American Psychologist, 73, 169–185.

    Google Scholar 

  • Osiurak, F., Jarry, C., Allain, P., Aubin, G., Etcharry-Bouyx, F., Richard, I., et al. (2009). Unusual use of objects after unilateral brain damage: The technical reasoning model. Cortex, 45, 769–783.

    Google Scholar 

  • Osiurak, F., Jarry, C., & Le Gall, D. (2010). Grasping the affordances, understanding the reasoning: Toward a dialectical theory of human tool use. Psychological Review, 117, 517–540.

    Google Scholar 

  • Osiurak, F., Lasserre, S., Arbanti, J., Brogniart, J., Bluet, A., Navarro, J., & Reynaud, E. (submitted). Technical reasoning is necessary not to reinvent the wheel.

  • Osiurak, F., Lesourd, M., Delporte, L., & Rossetti, Y. (2018). Tool use and generalized motor programs: We all are natural born poly-dexters. Scientific Reports, 8, 10429.

    Google Scholar 

  • Osiurak, F., Lesourd, M., Navarro, J., & Reynaud, E. (2020c). Technition: When tools come out of the closet. Perspectives on Psychological Science, 15, 880–897.

    Google Scholar 

  • Osiurak, F., Morgado, N., & Palluel-Germain, R. (2012). Tool use and perceived distance: When unreachable becomes sponatenously reachable. Experimental Brain Research, 218, 331–339.

    Google Scholar 

  • Osiurak, F., Morgado, N., Vallet, G. T., Drot, M., & Palluel-Germain, R. (2014). Getting a tool gives wings: Underestimation of effort for tool use. Psychological Research, 78, 1–9.

    Google Scholar 

  • Osiurak, F., & Reynaud, E. (2020). The elephant in the china shop: When technical reasoning meets cumulative technological culture. Behavioral and Brain Sciences, 43, e156.

    Google Scholar 

  • Osiurak, F., & Rossetti, Y. (2017). Definition: Limb apraxia. Cortex, 93, 228.

    Google Scholar 

  • Osiurak, F., Rossetti, Y., & Badets, A. (2017). What is an affordance? 40 years later. Neuroscience and Biobehavioral Reviews, 77, 403–417.

    Google Scholar 

  • Parsons, L. M. (1987). Imagined spatial transformation of one’s body. Journal of Experimental Psychology: General, 116, 172–191.

    Google Scholar 

  • Peck, A. J., Jeffers, R. G., Carello, C., & Turvey, M. T. (1996). Haptically perceiving the length of one rod by means of another. Ecological Psychology, 8, 237–258.

    Google Scholar 

  • Pellicano, A., & Binkofski, F. (in press). The prominent role of perceptual salience in object discrimination: Over discrimination of graspable side does not activate grasping affordances. Psychological Research.

  • Pellicano, A., Iani, C., Borghi, A. M., Rubichi, S., & Nicoletti, R. (2010). Simon-like and functional affordance effects with tools: The effects of object perceptual discrimination and object action state. Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology, 63, 2190–2201.

    Google Scholar 

  • Pellicano, A., Lugli, L., Binkofski, F., Rubichi, S., Iani, C., & Nicoletti, R. (2019). The unimanual handle-to-hand correspondence effect: Evidence for a location coding account. Psychological Research, 83, 1383–1399.

    Google Scholar 

  • Penn, D. C., Holyoak, K. J., & Povinelli, D. J. (2008). Darwin’s mistake: Explaining the discontinuity between human and nonhuman minds. Behavioral and Brain Sciences, 31, 109–130.

    Google Scholar 

  • Philbeck, J. W., & Witt, J. K. (2015). Action-specific influences on perception and postperceptual processes: Present controversies and future directions. Psychological Bulletin, 141, 1120–1144.

    Google Scholar 

  • Pinker, S. (2010). The cognitive niche: Coevolution of intelligence, sociality, and language. Proceedings of the National Academy of Science, 107, 8993–8999.

    Google Scholar 

  • Poizner, H., Clark, M., Merians, A. S., Macauley, B., Rothi, L. J. G., & Heilman, K. M. (1995). Joint coordination deficits in limb apraxia. Brain, 118, 227–242.

    Google Scholar 

  • Proffitt, D. R. (2006). Embodied perception and the economy of action. Perspectives in Psychological Science, 1, 110–122.

    Google Scholar 

  • Proffitt, D. R., Stefanucci, J., Banton, T., & Epstein, W. (2003). The role of effort in distance perception. Psychological Science, 14, 409–428.

    Google Scholar 

  • Reynaud, E., Lesourd, M., Navarro, J., & Osiurak, F. (2016). On the neurocognitive origins of human tool use: A critical review of neuroimaging data. Neuroscience and Biobehavioral Reviews, 64, 421–437.

    Google Scholar 

  • Reynaud, E., Navarro, J., Lesourd, M., & Osiurak, F. (2019). To watch is to work: A critical review of neuroimaging data on Tool-use Observation Network (ToON). Neuropsychology Review, 29, 484–497.

    Google Scholar 

  • Rizzolatti, G., & Matelli, M. (2003). Two different streams form the dorsal visual system: Anatomy and functions. Experimental Brain Research, 153, 146–157.

    Google Scholar 

  • Robertson, I. (2020). A little too technical: The threat of intellectualising technical reasoning. Behavioral and Brain Sciences, 43, e156.

    Google Scholar 

  • Rogers, T. T., & McClelland, J. L. (2004). Semantic cognition: A parallel distributed processing approach. Cambridge: MIT press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Rothi, L. J. G., Ochipa, C., & Heilman, K. M. (1991). A cognitive neuropsychological model of limb praxis. Cognitive Neuropsychology, 8, 443–458.

    Google Scholar 

  • Rumiati, R. I., & Humphreys, G. W. (1998). Recognition by action: Dissociating visual and semantic routes to action in normal observers. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 24, 631–647.

    Google Scholar 

  • Rutz, C., & Hunt, G. R. (2020). New Caledonian crows afford invaluable comparative insights into human cumulative technological culture. Behavioral and Brain Sciences, 43, e156.

    Google Scholar 

  • Schnall, S., Zadra, J. R., & Proffitt, D. R. (2010). Direct evidence for the economy of action: Glucose and the perception of geographical slant. Perception, 39, 464–482.

    Google Scholar 

  • Shipton, C., & Nielsen, M. (2015). Before cumulative culture: The evolutionary origins of overimitation and shared intentionality. Human Nature, 26, 33145.

    Google Scholar 

  • Shumaker, R. W., Walkup, K. R., & Beck, B. B. (2011). Animal tool behavior. Baltimore: John Hopkins University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Sirigu, A., Duhamel, J. R., Cohen, L., Pillon, B., Dubois, B., & Agid, Y. (1996). The mental representation of hand movements after parietal cortex damage. Science, 273, 1564–1568.

    Google Scholar 

  • Sirigu, A., Duhamel, J. R., & Poncet, M. (1991). The role of sensorimotor experience in object recognition. Brain, 114, 2555–2573.

    Google Scholar 

  • Stoffregen, T. A., Yang, C. M., Giveans, M. R., Flanagan, M., & Bardy, B. G. (2009). Movement in the perception of an affordance for wheelchair locomotion. Ecological Psychology, 21, 1–36.

    Google Scholar 

  • Stout, D., & Hecht, E. E. (2017). Evolutionary neuroscience of cumulative culture. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences, 114, 7861–7868.

    Google Scholar 

  • Symes, E., Ellis, R., & Tucker, M. (2007). Visual object affordances: Object orientation. Acta Psychologica, 124, 238–255.

    Google Scholar 

  • Taylor, A. H., & Jelbert, S. (2020). The crow in the room: New Caledonian crows offer insight into the necessary and sufficient conditions for cumulative cultural evolution. Behavioral and Brain Sciences, 43, e156.

    Google Scholar 

  • Tennie, C., Call, J., & Tomasello, M. (2010). Evidence for emulation in chimpanzees in social settings using the floating peanut task. PLoS ONE, 5, e10544.

    Google Scholar 

  • Thill, S., Caligiore, D., Borghi, A. M., Ziemke, T., & Baldassarre, G. (2013). Theories and computational models of affordance and mirror system: An integrative review. Neuroscience and Biobehavioral Reviews, 37, 491–521.

    Google Scholar 

  • Thompson, E. L., Bird, G., & Catmur, C. (2019). Conceptualizing and testing action understanding. Neuroscience and Biobehavioral Reviews, 105, 106–114.

    Google Scholar 

  • Tomasello, M., Carpenter, M., Call, J., Behne, T., & Moll, H. (2005). Understanding and sharing intentions: The origins of cultural cognition. Behavioral and Brain Sciences, 28, 675–735.

    Google Scholar 

  • Tomasello, M., Davis-Dasilva, M., Camak, L., & Bard, K. (1987). Observational learning of tool-use by young chimpanzees. Human Evolution, 2, 175–183.

    Google Scholar 

  • Tomasello, M., Kruger, A. C., & Ratner, H. H. (1993). Cultural learning. Behavioral and Brain Sciences, 16, 495–552.

    Google Scholar 

  • Tucker, M., & Ellis, R. (1998). On the relations between seen objects and components of potential actions. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 24, 830–846.

    Google Scholar 

  • Vaesen, K. (2012). The cognitive bases of human tool use. Behavioral and Brain Sciences, 35, 203–218.

    Google Scholar 

  • van Elk, M. (2014). The left inferior parietal lobe represents stored hand-postures for object use and action prediction. Frontiers in Psychology, 5, 333.

    Google Scholar 

  • van Elk, M., van Schie, H. T., & Bekkering, H. (2008). Conceptual knowledge for understanding other’s actions is organized primarily around action goals. Experimental Brain Research, 189, 99–107.

    Google Scholar 

  • van Elk, M., van Schie, H., & Bekkering, H. (2014). Action semantics: A unifying conceptual framework for the selective use of multimodal and modality-specific object knowledge. Physics of Life Reviews, 11, 220–250.

    Google Scholar 

  • van Owerwalle, F., & Baetens, K. (2009). Understanding others’ actions and goals by mirror and mentalizing systems: A meta-analysis. NeuroImage, 48, 564–584.

    Google Scholar 

  • Vingerhoets, G., Vandamme, K., & Vercammen, A. (2009). Conceptual and physical object qualities contribute differently to motor affordances. Brain and Cognition, 69, 481–489.

    Google Scholar 

  • Virgo, J., Pillon, J., Navarro, J., Reynaud, E., & Osiurak, F. (2017). Are you sure you’re faster when using a cognitive tool? American Journal of Psychology, 130, 493–503.

    Google Scholar 

  • Wagman, J. B., & Carello, C. (2001). Affordances and inertial constraints on tool use. Ecological Psychology, 13, 173–195.

    Google Scholar 

  • Wagman, J. B., & Carello, C. (2003). Haptically creating affordances: The user-tool interface. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Applied, 9, 175–186.

    Google Scholar 

  • Watson, C. E., & Buxbaum, L. J. (2014). Uncovering the architecture of action semantics. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 40, 1832–1848.

    Google Scholar 

  • Whiten, A., Goodall, J., McGrew, W. C., Nishida, T., Reynolds, V., Sugiyama, Y., et al. (1999). Cultures in chimpanzees. Nature, 399, 682–685.

    Google Scholar 

  • Whiten, A., Horner, V., & de Waal, F. B. M. (2005). Conformity to cultural norms of tool use in chimpanzees. Nature, 437, 737–740.

    Google Scholar 

  • Whiten, A., Horner, V., & Marshall-Pescini, S. R. J. (2003). Cultural panthropology. Evolutionary Anthropolology, 12, 92–105.

    Google Scholar 

  • Witt, J. K. (2011a). Action’s effect on perception. Current Directions in Psychological Science, 20, 201–206.

    Google Scholar 

  • Witt, J. K. (2011b). Tool use influences perceived shape and perceived parallelism, which serves as indirect measures of perceived distance. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 37, 1148–1156.

    Google Scholar 

  • Witt, J. K., & Proffitt, D. R. (2008). Action-specific influences on distance perception: A role for motor simulation. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 34, 1479–1492.

    Google Scholar 

  • Witt, J. K., Proffitt, D. R., & Epstein, W. (2005). Tool use affects perceived distance, but only when you intend to use it. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 31, 880–888.

    Google Scholar 

  • Wohlschläger, A., Gattis, M., & Bekkering, H. (2003). Action generation and action perception in imitation: An instance of the ideomotor principle. Philosophical Transactions of the Royal Society B, 358, 501–515.

    Google Scholar 

  • Wolpert, L. (2003). Causal belief and the origins of technology. Philosophical Transactions of the Royal Society of London, 361A, 1709–1719.

    Google Scholar 

  • Wurm, M. F., & Caramazza, A. (2019). Distinct roles of temporal and frontoparietal cortex in representing actions across vision and language. Nature Communications, 10, 289.

    Google Scholar 

Download references

Acknowledgements

This work was performed within the framework of the LABEX CORTEX (ANR-11-LABX-0042) of Université de Lyon, within the program “Investissements d’Avenir” (ANR-11- IDEX-0007) operated by the French National Research Agency (ANR).

Author information

Authors and Affiliations

Authors

Corresponding author

Correspondence to François Osiurak.

Additional information

Publisher's Note

Springer Nature remains neutral with regard to jurisdictional claims in published maps and institutional affiliations.

Rights and permissions

Reprints and permissions

About this article

Check for updates. Verify currency and authenticity via CrossMark

Cite this article

Osiurak, F., Federico, G. Four ways of (mis-)conceiving embodiment in tool use. Synthese 199, 3853–3879 (2021). https://doi.org/10.1007/s11229-020-02960-1

Download citation

  • Received:

  • Accepted:

  • Published:

  • Issue Date:

  • DOI: https://doi.org/10.1007/s11229-020-02960-1

Keywords

Navigation