Skip to main content
Top
Gepubliceerd in: Psychological Research 5/2021

04-07-2020 | Original Article

Temporal error monitoring with directional error magnitude judgements: a robust phenomenon with no effect of being watched

Auteurs: Tutku Öztel, Terry Eskenazi, Fuat Balcı

Gepubliceerd in: Psychological Research | Uitgave 5/2021

Log in om toegang te krijgen
share
DELEN

Deel dit onderdeel of sectie (kopieer de link)

  • Optie A:
    Klik op de rechtermuisknop op de link en selecteer de optie “linkadres kopiëren”
  • Optie B:
    Deel de link per e-mail

Abstract

A key aspect of metacognition is the ability to monitor performance. A recent line of work has shown that error-monitoring ability captures both the magnitude and direction of timing errors, thereby pointing at the metric composition of error monitoring [e.g., Akdoğan and Balcı (J Exp Psychol https://​dx.​doi.​org/​10.​1037/​xge0000265, 2017)]. These studies, however, primarily used a composite variable that combined isolated measures of ordinal confidence ratings (as a proxy for error magnitude judgement) and “shorter/longer than the target” judgements. In two experiments we tested temporal error monitoring (TEM) performance with a more direct measure of directional error magnitude rating on a continuum. The second aim of this study is to test if TEM performance is modulated by the feeling of being watched that was previously shown to influence metacognitive-like monitoring processes. We predicted that being watched would improve TEM performance, particularly in participants with high timing precision (a proxy for high task mastery), and disrupt TEM performance in participants with low timing precision (a proxy for low task mastery). In both experiments, we found strong evidence for TEM ability. However, we did not find any reliable effect of the social stimulus on TEM performance. In short, our results demonstrate that metric error monitoring is a robust metacognitive phenomenon, which is not sensitive to social influence.
Bijlagen
Alleen toegankelijk voor geautoriseerde gebruikers
Literatuur
go back to reference Aiello, J. R., & Douthitt, E. A. (2001). Social facilitation from Triplett to electronic performance monitoring. Group Dynamics: Theory, Research, and Practice, 5(3), 163.CrossRef Aiello, J. R., & Douthitt, E. A. (2001). Social facilitation from Triplett to electronic performance monitoring. Group Dynamics: Theory, Research, and Practice, 5(3), 163.CrossRef
go back to reference Akdoğan, B., & Balcı, F. (2017). Are you early or late?: Temporal error monitoring. Journal of Experimental Psychology: General, 146(3), 347–361.CrossRef Akdoğan, B., & Balcı, F. (2017). Are you early or late?: Temporal error monitoring. Journal of Experimental Psychology: General, 146(3), 347–361.CrossRef
go back to reference Brainard, D. H. (1997). The psychophysics toolbox. Spatial Vision, 10(4), 433–436.CrossRef Brainard, D. H. (1997). The psychophysics toolbox. Spatial Vision, 10(4), 433–436.CrossRef
go back to reference Brocas, I., Carrillo, J. D., & Tarrasó, J. (2018). Self-awareness of biases in time perception. Journal of Economic Behavior & Organization, 148, 1–19.CrossRef Brocas, I., Carrillo, J. D., & Tarrasó, J. (2018). Self-awareness of biases in time perception. Journal of Economic Behavior & Organization, 148, 1–19.CrossRef
go back to reference Chib, V. S., Adachi, R., & O’doherty, J. P. (2018). Neural substrates of social facilitation effects on incentive-based performance. Social Cognitive and Affective Neuroscience, 13(4), 391–403.CrossRef Chib, V. S., Adachi, R., & O’doherty, J. P. (2018). Neural substrates of social facilitation effects on incentive-based performance. Social Cognitive and Affective Neuroscience, 13(4), 391–403.CrossRef
go back to reference Conty, L., George, N., & Hietanen, J. K. (2016). Watching eyes effects: When others meet the self. Consciousness and Cognition, 45, 184–197.CrossRef Conty, L., George, N., & Hietanen, J. K. (2016). Watching eyes effects: When others meet the self. Consciousness and Cognition, 45, 184–197.CrossRef
go back to reference Conty, L., Gimmig, D., Belletier, C., George, N., & Huguet, P. (2010a). The cost of being watched: Stroop interference increases under concomitant eye contact. Cognition, 115(1), 133–139.CrossRef Conty, L., Gimmig, D., Belletier, C., George, N., & Huguet, P. (2010a). The cost of being watched: Stroop interference increases under concomitant eye contact. Cognition, 115(1), 133–139.CrossRef
go back to reference Conty, L., Russo, M., Loehr, V., Hugueville, L., Barbu, S., Huguet, P., et al. (2010b). The mere perception of eye contact increases arousal during a word-spelling task. Social Neuroscience, 5(2), 171–186.CrossRef Conty, L., Russo, M., Loehr, V., Hugueville, L., Barbu, S., Huguet, P., et al. (2010b). The mere perception of eye contact increases arousal during a word-spelling task. Social Neuroscience, 5(2), 171–186.CrossRef
go back to reference Corston, R., & Colman, A. M. (1996). Gender and social facilitation effects on computer competence and attitudes toward computers. Journal of Educational Computing Research, 14(2), 171–183.CrossRef Corston, R., & Colman, A. M. (1996). Gender and social facilitation effects on computer competence and attitudes toward computers. Journal of Educational Computing Research, 14(2), 171–183.CrossRef
go back to reference Cottrell, N. B. (1972). Social facilitation. In C. G. McClintock (Ed.), Experimental social psychology. New York: Holt. Cottrell, N. B. (1972). Social facilitation. In C. G. McClintock (Ed.), Experimental social psychology. New York: Holt.
go back to reference Doenyas, C., Mutluer, T., Genç, E., & Balcı, F. (2019). Error monitoring in decision-making and timing is disrupted in autism spectrum disorder. Autism Research, 12(2), 239–248.CrossRef Doenyas, C., Mutluer, T., Genç, E., & Balcı, F. (2019). Error monitoring in decision-making and timing is disrupted in autism spectrum disorder. Autism Research, 12(2), 239–248.CrossRef
go back to reference Duyan, Y. A., & Balcı, F. (2018). Numerical error monitoring. Psychonomic Bulletin & Review, 25(4), 1549–1555.CrossRef Duyan, Y. A., & Balcı, F. (2018). Numerical error monitoring. Psychonomic Bulletin & Review, 25(4), 1549–1555.CrossRef
go back to reference Duyan, Y. A., & Balcı, F. (2019). Metric error monitoring in the numerical estimates. Consciousness and Cognition, 67, 69–76.CrossRef Duyan, Y. A., & Balcı, F. (2019). Metric error monitoring in the numerical estimates. Consciousness and Cognition, 67, 69–76.CrossRef
go back to reference Duyan, Y. A., & Balcı, F. (2020). Monitoring line length reproduction errors. Consciousness and Cognition, 77, 102831.CrossRef Duyan, Y. A., & Balcı, F. (2020). Monitoring line length reproduction errors. Consciousness and Cognition, 77, 102831.CrossRef
go back to reference Flavell, J. H., & Wellman, H. M. (1977). Metamemory. In R. V. Kail Jr. & J. W. Hagen (Eds.), Perspectives on the development of memory and cognition. Hillsdale, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum. Flavell, J. H., & Wellman, H. M. (1977). Metamemory. In R. V. Kail Jr. & J. W. Hagen (Eds.), Perspectives on the development of memory and cognition. Hillsdale, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum.
go back to reference Fleming, S. M., & Daw, N. D. (2017). Self-evaluation of decision-making: A general Bayesian framework for metacognitive computation. Psychological Review, 124(1), 91–114.CrossRef Fleming, S. M., & Daw, N. D. (2017). Self-evaluation of decision-making: A general Bayesian framework for metacognitive computation. Psychological Review, 124(1), 91–114.CrossRef
go back to reference Fleming, S. M., & Dolan, R. J. (2012). The neural basis of metacognitive ability. Philosophical Transactions of the Royal Society B: Biological Sciences, 367(1594), 1338–1349.CrossRef Fleming, S. M., & Dolan, R. J. (2012). The neural basis of metacognitive ability. Philosophical Transactions of the Royal Society B: Biological Sciences, 367(1594), 1338–1349.CrossRef
go back to reference Fleming, S. M., Huijgen, J., & Dolan, R. J. (2012b). Prefrontal contributions to metacognition in perceptual decision making. Journal of Neuroscience, 32(18), 6117–6125.CrossRef Fleming, S. M., Huijgen, J., & Dolan, R. J. (2012b). Prefrontal contributions to metacognition in perceptual decision making. Journal of Neuroscience, 32(18), 6117–6125.CrossRef
go back to reference Gibbon, J., Church, R. M., & Meck, W. H. (1984). Scalar timing in memory. Annals of the New York Academy of Sciences, 423, 52–77.CrossRef Gibbon, J., Church, R. M., & Meck, W. H. (1984). Scalar timing in memory. Annals of the New York Academy of Sciences, 423, 52–77.CrossRef
go back to reference Kononowicz, T., Roger, C., & van Wassenhove, V. (2019). Temporal metacognition as the decoding of self-generated brain dynamics. Cerebral Cortex, 29(10), 4366–4380.CrossRef Kononowicz, T., Roger, C., & van Wassenhove, V. (2019). Temporal metacognition as the decoding of self-generated brain dynamics. Cerebral Cortex, 29(10), 4366–4380.CrossRef
go back to reference Kononowicz, T. W., & van Wassenhove, V. (2019). Evaluation of Self-generated Behavior: Untangling Metacognitive Readout and Error Detection. Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience, 31(11), 1641–1657.CrossRef Kononowicz, T. W., & van Wassenhove, V. (2019). Evaluation of Self-generated Behavior: Untangling Metacognitive Readout and Error Detection. Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience, 31(11), 1641–1657.CrossRef
go back to reference Lake, J. I., LaBar, K. S., & Meck, W. H. (2016). Emotional modulation of interval timing and time perception. Neuroscience & Biobehavioral Reviews, 64, 403–420.CrossRef Lake, J. I., LaBar, K. S., & Meck, W. H. (2016). Emotional modulation of interval timing and time perception. Neuroscience & Biobehavioral Reviews, 64, 403–420.CrossRef
go back to reference Orr, J. M., & Carrasco, M. (2011). The role of the error positivity in the conscious perception of errors. Journal of Neuroscience, 31(16), 5891–5892.CrossRef Orr, J. M., & Carrasco, M. (2011). The role of the error positivity in the conscious perception of errors. Journal of Neuroscience, 31(16), 5891–5892.CrossRef
go back to reference Rattat, A. C., & Droit-Volet, S. (2012). What is the best and easiest method of preventing counting in different temporal tasks? Behavior Research Methods, 44(1), 67–80.CrossRef Rattat, A. C., & Droit-Volet, S. (2012). What is the best and easiest method of preventing counting in different temporal tasks? Behavior Research Methods, 44(1), 67–80.CrossRef
go back to reference Riemer, M., Kubik, V., & Wolbers, T. (2019). The effect of feedback on temporal error monitoring and timing behavior. Behavioural Brain Research, 369, 111929.CrossRef Riemer, M., Kubik, V., & Wolbers, T. (2019). The effect of feedback on temporal error monitoring and timing behavior. Behavioural Brain Research, 369, 111929.CrossRef
go back to reference Triplett, N. (1898). The dynamogenic factors in pacemaking and competition. The American Journal of Psychology, 9(4), 507–533.CrossRef Triplett, N. (1898). The dynamogenic factors in pacemaking and competition. The American Journal of Psychology, 9(4), 507–533.CrossRef
go back to reference Uziel, L. (2007). Individual differences in the social facilitation effect: A review and meta-analysis. Journal of Research in Personality, 41(3), 579–601.CrossRef Uziel, L. (2007). Individual differences in the social facilitation effect: A review and meta-analysis. Journal of Research in Personality, 41(3), 579–601.CrossRef
Metagegevens
Titel
Temporal error monitoring with directional error magnitude judgements: a robust phenomenon with no effect of being watched
Auteurs
Tutku Öztel
Terry Eskenazi
Fuat Balcı
Publicatiedatum
04-07-2020
Uitgeverij
Springer Berlin Heidelberg
Gepubliceerd in
Psychological Research / Uitgave 5/2021
Print ISSN: 0340-0727
Elektronisch ISSN: 1430-2772
DOI
https://doi.org/10.1007/s00426-020-01379-0

Andere artikelen Uitgave 5/2021

Psychological Research 5/2021 Naar de uitgave