Skip to main content
Top
Gepubliceerd in: Psychological Research 5/2021

04-08-2020 | Original Article

The impact of implicit and explicit suggestions that ‘there is nothing to learn’ on implicit sequence learning

Auteurs: Luc Vermeylen, Elger Abrahamse, Senne Braem, Davide Rigoni

Gepubliceerd in: Psychological Research | Uitgave 5/2021

Log in om toegang te krijgen
share
DELEN

Deel dit onderdeel of sectie (kopieer de link)

  • Optie A:
    Klik op de rechtermuisknop op de link en selecteer de optie “linkadres kopiëren”
  • Optie B:
    Deel de link per e-mail

Abstract

We can sometimes efficiently pick up statistical regularities in our environment in the absence of clear intentions or awareness, a process typically referred to as implicit sequence learning. In the current study, we tried to address the question whether suggesting participants that there is nothing to learn can impact this form of learning. If a priori predictions or intentions to learn are important in guiding implicit learning, we reasoned that suggesting participants that there is nothing to learn in a given context should hamper implicit learning. We introduced participants to random contexts that indicated that there was nothing to learn, either implicitly (i.e., by presenting blocks of random trials in “Experiment 1”), or explicitly (i.e., by explicitly instructing them in “Experiment 2”). Next, in a subsequent learning phase, participants performed an implicit sequence learning task. We found that these implicit or explicit suggestions that ‘there was nothing to learn’ did not influence the emergence of implicit knowledge in the subsequent learning phase. Although these findings seem consistent with simple associative or Hebbian learning accounts of implicit sequence learning (i.e., not steered by predictions), we discuss potential limitations that should inform future studies on the role of a priori predictions in implicit learning.
Bijlagen
Alleen toegankelijk voor geautoriseerde gebruikers
Literatuur
go back to reference Bar, M. (2007). The proactive brain: using analogies and associations to generate predictions. Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 11(7), 280–289.CrossRef Bar, M. (2007). The proactive brain: using analogies and associations to generate predictions. Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 11(7), 280–289.CrossRef
go back to reference Cacioppo, J. T., & Petty, R. E. (1982). The need for cognition. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 42(1), 116–131.CrossRef Cacioppo, J. T., & Petty, R. E. (1982). The need for cognition. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 42(1), 116–131.CrossRef
go back to reference Dale, R., Duran, N. D., & Morehead, J. R. (2012). Prediction during statistical learning, and implications for the implicit/explicit divide. Advances in Cognitive Psychology, 8(2), 196.CrossRef Dale, R., Duran, N. D., & Morehead, J. R. (2012). Prediction during statistical learning, and implications for the implicit/explicit divide. Advances in Cognitive Psychology, 8(2), 196.CrossRef
go back to reference Hebb, D. O. (1949). The organization of behavior: a neuropsychological theory. New York: Wiley. Hebb, D. O. (1949). The organization of behavior: a neuropsychological theory. New York: Wiley.
go back to reference Howard, D. V., & Howard, J. H. (2001). When it does hurt to try: Adult age differences in the effects of instructions on implicit pattern learning. Psychonomic Bulletin and Review, 8(4), 798–805.CrossRef Howard, D. V., & Howard, J. H. (2001). When it does hurt to try: Adult age differences in the effects of instructions on implicit pattern learning. Psychonomic Bulletin and Review, 8(4), 798–805.CrossRef
go back to reference Howard, J. H., & Howard, D. V. (1997). Age differences in implicit learning of higher order dependencies in serial patterns. Psychology and Aging, 12(4), 634–656.CrossRef Howard, J. H., & Howard, D. V. (1997). Age differences in implicit learning of higher order dependencies in serial patterns. Psychology and Aging, 12(4), 634–656.CrossRef
go back to reference Lubow, R. E. (1973). Latent inhibition. Psychological Bulletin, 79(6), 398.CrossRef Lubow, R. E. (1973). Latent inhibition. Psychological Bulletin, 79(6), 398.CrossRef
go back to reference Miyashita, K., Rand, M. K., Miyachi, S., & Hikosaka, O. (1996). Anticipatory saccades in sequential procedural learning in monkeys. Journal of Neurophysiology, 76(2), 1361–1366.CrossRef Miyashita, K., Rand, M. K., Miyachi, S., & Hikosaka, O. (1996). Anticipatory saccades in sequential procedural learning in monkeys. Journal of Neurophysiology, 76(2), 1361–1366.CrossRef
go back to reference Reber, A. S. (1976). Implicit learning of synthetic languages: The role of instructional set. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Learning and Memory, 2(1), 88. Reber, A. S. (1976). Implicit learning of synthetic languages: The role of instructional set. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Learning and Memory, 2(1), 88.
go back to reference Reber, A. S. (1989). Implicit learning and tacit knowledge. Journal of Experimental Psychology General, 118(3), 219–235.CrossRef Reber, A. S. (1989). Implicit learning and tacit knowledge. Journal of Experimental Psychology General, 118(3), 219–235.CrossRef
go back to reference Rebuschat, P., & Williams, J. N. (2012). Implicit and explicit knowledge in second language acquisition. Applied Psycholinguistics, 33(4), 829–856.CrossRef Rebuschat, P., & Williams, J. N. (2012). Implicit and explicit knowledge in second language acquisition. Applied Psycholinguistics, 33(4), 829–856.CrossRef
go back to reference Rescorla, R. A., & Wagner, A. R. (1972). A theory of Pavlovian conditioning: Variations in the effectiveness of reinforcement and nonreinforcement. Classical Conditioning II: Current Research and Theory, 2, 64–99. Rescorla, R. A., & Wagner, A. R. (1972). A theory of Pavlovian conditioning: Variations in the effectiveness of reinforcement and nonreinforcement. Classical Conditioning II: Current Research and Theory, 2, 64–99.
go back to reference Song, S., Howard, J. H., & Howard, D. V. (2007). Implicit probabilistic sequence learning is independent of explicit awareness. Learning and Memory, 14(3), 167–176.CrossRef Song, S., Howard, J. H., & Howard, D. V. (2007). Implicit probabilistic sequence learning is independent of explicit awareness. Learning and Memory, 14(3), 167–176.CrossRef
go back to reference Thiessen, E. D. (2017). What's statistical about learning? Insights from modelling statistical learning as a set of memory processes. Philosophical Transactions of the Royal Society of London Series B, 372(1711), 20160056.CrossRef Thiessen, E. D. (2017). What's statistical about learning? Insights from modelling statistical learning as a set of memory processes. Philosophical Transactions of the Royal Society of London Series B, 372(1711), 20160056.CrossRef
go back to reference Tran, D. M. D., Harris, J. A., Harris, I. M., & Livesey, E. J. (2020). Motor conflict: Revealing involuntary conditioned motor preparation using transcranial magnetic stimulation. Cerebral Cortex, 30(4), 2478–2488.CrossRef Tran, D. M. D., Harris, J. A., Harris, I. M., & Livesey, E. J. (2020). Motor conflict: Revealing involuntary conditioned motor preparation using transcranial magnetic stimulation. Cerebral Cortex, 30(4), 2478–2488.CrossRef
Metagegevens
Titel
The impact of implicit and explicit suggestions that ‘there is nothing to learn’ on implicit sequence learning
Auteurs
Luc Vermeylen
Elger Abrahamse
Senne Braem
Davide Rigoni
Publicatiedatum
04-08-2020
Uitgeverij
Springer Berlin Heidelberg
Gepubliceerd in
Psychological Research / Uitgave 5/2021
Print ISSN: 0340-0727
Elektronisch ISSN: 1430-2772
DOI
https://doi.org/10.1007/s00426-020-01385-2

Andere artikelen Uitgave 5/2021

Psychological Research 5/2021 Naar de uitgave