Skip to main content
Top
Gepubliceerd in: Psychological Research 5/2021

01-06-2020 | Original Article

Does musical interaction in a jazz duet modulate peripersonal space?

Auteurs: A. Dell’Anna, M. Rosso, V. Bruno, F. Garbarini, M. Leman, A. Berti

Gepubliceerd in: Psychological Research | Uitgave 5/2021

Log in om toegang te krijgen
share
DELEN

Deel dit onderdeel of sectie (kopieer de link)

  • Optie A:
    Klik op de rechtermuisknop op de link en selecteer de optie “linkadres kopiëren”
  • Optie B:
    Deel de link per e-mail

Abstract

Researchers have widely studied peripersonal space (the space within reach) in the last 20 years with a focus on its plasticity following the use of tools and, more recently, social interactions. Ensemble music is a sophisticated joint action that is typically explored in its temporal rather than spatial dimensions, even within embodied approaches. We, therefore, devised a new paradigm in which two musicians could perform a jazz standard either in a cooperative (correct harmony) or uncooperative (incorrect harmony) condition, under the hypothesis that their peripersonal spaces are modulated by the interaction. We exploited a well-established audio-tactile integration task as a proxy for such a space. After the performances, we measured reaction times to tactile stimuli on the subjects’ right hand and auditory stimuli delivered at two different distances, (next to the subject and next to the partner). Considering previous literature’s evidence that integration of two different stimuli (e.g. a tactile and an auditory stimulus) is faster in near space compared to far space, we predicted that a cooperative interaction would have extended the peripersonal space of the musicians towards their partner, facilitating reaction times to bimodal stimuli in both spaces. Surprisingly, we obtained complementary results in terms of an increase of reaction times to tactile-auditory near stimuli, but only following the uncooperative condition. We interpret this finding as a suppression of the subject’s peripersonal space or as a withdrawal from the uncooperative partner. Subjective reports and correlations between these reports and reaction times comply with that interpretation. Finally, we determined an overall better multisensory integration competence in musicians compared to non-musicians tested in the same task.
Bijlagen
Alleen toegankelijk voor geautoriseerde gebruikers
Literatuur
go back to reference Badino, L., D’Ausilio, A., Glowinsky, D., Camurri, A., & Fadiga, L. (2014). Sensorimotor communication in professional quartets. Neuropsychologia, 55, 98–104.PubMedCrossRef Badino, L., D’Ausilio, A., Glowinsky, D., Camurri, A., & Fadiga, L. (2014). Sensorimotor communication in professional quartets. Neuropsychologia, 55, 98–104.PubMedCrossRef
go back to reference Bangert, M., Peschel, T., Schlaug, G., Rotte, M., Drescher, D., Hinrichs, H., et al. (2006). Shared networks for auditory and motor processing in professional pianists: Evidence from fMRI conjunction. NeuroImage, 30, 917–926.PubMedCrossRef Bangert, M., Peschel, T., Schlaug, G., Rotte, M., Drescher, D., Hinrichs, H., et al. (2006). Shared networks for auditory and motor processing in professional pianists: Evidence from fMRI conjunction. NeuroImage, 30, 917–926.PubMedCrossRef
go back to reference Berti, A., & Frassinetti, F. (2000). When far becomes near: Remapping of space by tool use. Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience, 12, 415–420.PubMedCrossRef Berti, A., & Frassinetti, F. (2000). When far becomes near: Remapping of space by tool use. Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience, 12, 415–420.PubMedCrossRef
go back to reference Biggio, M., Bisio, A., Avanzino, L., Ruggeri, P., & Bove, M. (2017). This racket is not mine: The influence of the tool-use on peripersonal space. Neuropsychologia, 103, 54–58.PubMedCrossRef Biggio, M., Bisio, A., Avanzino, L., Ruggeri, P., & Bove, M. (2017). This racket is not mine: The influence of the tool-use on peripersonal space. Neuropsychologia, 103, 54–58.PubMedCrossRef
go back to reference Bisio, A., Garbarini, F., Biggio, M., Fossataro, C., Ruggeri, P., & Bove, M. (2017). Dynamic shaping of the defensive peripersonal space through predictive motor mechanisms: when the ‘near’ becomes ‘far’. Journal of Neuroscience, 37, 2415–2424.PubMedCrossRef Bisio, A., Garbarini, F., Biggio, M., Fossataro, C., Ruggeri, P., & Bove, M. (2017). Dynamic shaping of the defensive peripersonal space through predictive motor mechanisms: when the ‘near’ becomes ‘far’. Journal of Neuroscience, 37, 2415–2424.PubMedCrossRef
go back to reference Canzoneri, E., Magosso, E., & Serino, A. (2012). Dynamic sounds capture the boundaries of peripersonal space representation in humans. PLoS ONE, 7, e44306.PubMedPubMedCentralCrossRef Canzoneri, E., Magosso, E., & Serino, A. (2012). Dynamic sounds capture the boundaries of peripersonal space representation in humans. PLoS ONE, 7, e44306.PubMedPubMedCentralCrossRef
go back to reference Cheong Y.J. & Will U. (2018). Music, Space and Body: the evolutionary history of vocal and instrumental music. Conference: ICMPC15-ESCOM10 Cheong Y.J. & Will U. (2018). Music, Space and Body: the evolutionary history of vocal and instrumental music. Conference: ICMPC15-ESCOM10
go back to reference Clayton, M. (2012). What is entrainment? Definition and applications in musical research. Empirical Musicology Review, 7(1–2), 49–56.CrossRef Clayton, M. (2012). What is entrainment? Definition and applications in musical research. Empirical Musicology Review, 7(1–2), 49–56.CrossRef
go back to reference Cochrane, T. (2017). Group Flow. In M. Lesaffre, P.-J. Maes, & M. Leman (Eds.), The Routledge Companion to Embodied Music Interaction (pp. 133–140). NY: Routledge.CrossRef Cochrane, T. (2017). Group Flow. In M. Lesaffre, P.-J. Maes, & M. Leman (Eds.), The Routledge Companion to Embodied Music Interaction (pp. 133–140). NY: Routledge.CrossRef
go back to reference Cross, I. (2014). Music and communication in music psychology. Psychology of Music, 42(6), 809–819.CrossRef Cross, I. (2014). Music and communication in music psychology. Psychology of Music, 42(6), 809–819.CrossRef
go back to reference Csikszentmihalyi, M. (1990). Flow: The psychology of optimal experience. New York: Harper & Row. Csikszentmihalyi, M. (1990). Flow: The psychology of optimal experience. New York: Harper & Row.
go back to reference D’Ausilio, A., Badino, L., Li, Y., Tokay, S., Craighero, L., Canto, R., et al. (2012). Leadership in orchestra emerges from the causal relationships of movement kinematics. PLoS ONE, 7, e35757.PubMedPubMedCentralCrossRef D’Ausilio, A., Badino, L., Li, Y., Tokay, S., Craighero, L., Canto, R., et al. (2012). Leadership in orchestra emerges from the causal relationships of movement kinematics. PLoS ONE, 7, e35757.PubMedPubMedCentralCrossRef
go back to reference D’Ausilio, A., Novembre, G., Fadiga, L., & Keller, P. E. (2015). What can music tell us about social interaction? Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 19(3), 111–114.PubMedCrossRef D’Ausilio, A., Novembre, G., Fadiga, L., & Keller, P. E. (2015). What can music tell us about social interaction? Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 19(3), 111–114.PubMedCrossRef
go back to reference Davis, M. H. (1980). A multidimensional approach to individual differences in empathy. JSAS Catalog of Selected Documents in Psychology, 10, 85. Davis, M. H. (1980). A multidimensional approach to individual differences in empathy. JSAS Catalog of Selected Documents in Psychology, 10, 85.
go back to reference Dijkerman, H. C., & Farnè, A. (2015). Sensorimotor and social aspects of peripersonal space. Neuropsychologia, 70, 309–312.PubMedCrossRef Dijkerman, H. C., & Farnè, A. (2015). Sensorimotor and social aspects of peripersonal space. Neuropsychologia, 70, 309–312.PubMedCrossRef
go back to reference Dissanayake, E. (2000). Antecedents of the temporal arts in early mother–infant interaction. In N. L. Wallin, B. Merker, & S. Brown (Eds.), The Origins of Music (pp. 389–410). Cambridge: MIT Press. Dissanayake, E. (2000). Antecedents of the temporal arts in early mother–infant interaction. In N. L. Wallin, B. Merker, & S. Brown (Eds.), The Origins of Music (pp. 389–410). Cambridge: MIT Press.
go back to reference Doffman, M.R. (2008). Feeling the Groove: Shared Time and Its Meanings for Three Jazz Trios. Ph.D. Dissertation, Music Department, Open University. Doffman, M.R. (2008). Feeling the Groove: Shared Time and Its Meanings for Three Jazz Trios. Ph.D. Dissertation, Music Department, Open University.
go back to reference Eerola, T., Jakubowski, K., Moran, N., Keller, P., & Clayton, M. (2018). Shared periodic performer movements coordinate interactions in duo improvisations. Royal Society Open Science, 5(2), 171520.PubMedPubMedCentralCrossRef Eerola, T., Jakubowski, K., Moran, N., Keller, P., & Clayton, M. (2018). Shared periodic performer movements coordinate interactions in duo improvisations. Royal Society Open Science, 5(2), 171520.PubMedPubMedCentralCrossRef
go back to reference Faul, F., Erdfelder, E., Buchner, A., & Lang, A.-G. (2009). Statistical power analyses using G*Power 3.1: Tests for correlation and regression analyses. Behavior Research Methods, 41, 1149–1160.PubMedCrossRef Faul, F., Erdfelder, E., Buchner, A., & Lang, A.-G. (2009). Statistical power analyses using G*Power 3.1: Tests for correlation and regression analyses. Behavior Research Methods, 41, 1149–1160.PubMedCrossRef
go back to reference Faul, F., Erdfelder, E., Lang, A.-G., & Buchner, A. (2007). G*Power 3: A flexible statistical power analysis program for the social, behavioral, and biomedical sciences. Behavior Research Methods, 39, 175–191.PubMedCrossRef Faul, F., Erdfelder, E., Lang, A.-G., & Buchner, A. (2007). G*Power 3: A flexible statistical power analysis program for the social, behavioral, and biomedical sciences. Behavior Research Methods, 39, 175–191.PubMedCrossRef
go back to reference Freeman, W. J. (2000). A neurobiological role of music in social bonding. In N. L. Wallin, B. Merker, & S. Brown (Eds.), The Origins of Music (pp. 411–424). Cambridge: The MIT Press. Freeman, W. J. (2000). A neurobiological role of music in social bonding. In N. L. Wallin, B. Merker, & S. Brown (Eds.), The Origins of Music (pp. 411–424). Cambridge: The MIT Press.
go back to reference Fuchs, T., & De Jaegher, H. (2009). Enactive Intersubjectivity: Participatory sense-making and mutual incorporation. Phenomenology and the Cognitive Sciences, 8(4), 465–486.CrossRef Fuchs, T., & De Jaegher, H. (2009). Enactive Intersubjectivity: Participatory sense-making and mutual incorporation. Phenomenology and the Cognitive Sciences, 8(4), 465–486.CrossRef
go back to reference Geeves, A., & Sutton, J. (2014). Embodied cognition, perception, and performance in music. Empirical Musicology Review, 9(3–4), 247–253. Geeves, A., & Sutton, J. (2014). Embodied cognition, perception, and performance in music. Empirical Musicology Review, 9(3–4), 247–253.
go back to reference Gescheider, G. A. (1997). The classical psychophysical methods. Psychophysics: The Fundamentals (3rd ed., pp. 45–72). Mahwah: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates. Gescheider, G. A. (1997). The classical psychophysical methods. Psychophysics: The Fundamentals (3rd ed., pp. 45–72). Mahwah: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates.
go back to reference Goebl, W., & Palmer, C. (2009). Synchronization of timing and motion among performing musicians. Music Perception, 26, 427–438.CrossRef Goebl, W., & Palmer, C. (2009). Synchronization of timing and motion among performing musicians. Music Perception, 26, 427–438.CrossRef
go back to reference Graziano, M. S. A., Hu, X. T., & Gross, C. G. (1997). Visuospatial properties of ventral premotor cortex. Journal of Neurophysiology, 77, 2268–2292.PubMedCrossRef Graziano, M. S. A., Hu, X. T., & Gross, C. G. (1997). Visuospatial properties of ventral premotor cortex. Journal of Neurophysiology, 77, 2268–2292.PubMedCrossRef
go back to reference Herbert, R. (2011). Everyday Music Listening: Absorption, Dissociation and Trancing. Farnham: Ashgate. Herbert, R. (2011). Everyday Music Listening: Absorption, Dissociation and Trancing. Farnham: Ashgate.
go back to reference Hove, M. J., & Risen, J. L. (2009). It's all in the timing: Interpersonal synchrony increases affiliation. Social Cognition, 27, 949–960.CrossRef Hove, M. J., & Risen, J. L. (2009). It's all in the timing: Interpersonal synchrony increases affiliation. Social Cognition, 27, 949–960.CrossRef
go back to reference Iachini, T., Coello, Y., Frassinetti, F., & Ruggiero, G. (2014). Body space in social interactions: A comparison of reaching and comfort distance in immersive virtual reality. PLoS ONE, 9, e111511.PubMedPubMedCentralCrossRef Iachini, T., Coello, Y., Frassinetti, F., & Ruggiero, G. (2014). Body space in social interactions: A comparison of reaching and comfort distance in immersive virtual reality. PLoS ONE, 9, e111511.PubMedPubMedCentralCrossRef
go back to reference Iriki, A., Tanaka, M., & Iwamura, Y. (1996). Coding of modified body schema during tool use by macaque postcentral neurones. NeuroReport, 7, 2325–2330.PubMedCrossRef Iriki, A., Tanaka, M., & Iwamura, Y. (1996). Coding of modified body schema during tool use by macaque postcentral neurones. NeuroReport, 7, 2325–2330.PubMedCrossRef
go back to reference Iyer, V. (2002). Embodied mind, situated cognition, and expressive microtiming in African–American music. Music Perception, 19, 387–414.CrossRef Iyer, V. (2002). Embodied mind, situated cognition, and expressive microtiming in African–American music. Music Perception, 19, 387–414.CrossRef
go back to reference Jäncke, L. (2012). The dynamic audio-motor system in pianists. Annals of the New York Academy of Sciences, 1252, 246–252.PubMedCrossRef Jäncke, L. (2012). The dynamic audio-motor system in pianists. Annals of the New York Academy of Sciences, 1252, 246–252.PubMedCrossRef
go back to reference Keller, P. E., Knoblich, G., & Repp, B. H. (2007). Pianists duet better when they play with themselves: On the possible role of action simulation in synchronization. Consciousness and Cognition, 16, 102–111.PubMedCrossRef Keller, P. E., Knoblich, G., & Repp, B. H. (2007). Pianists duet better when they play with themselves: On the possible role of action simulation in synchronization. Consciousness and Cognition, 16, 102–111.PubMedCrossRef
go back to reference Keller, P. E., Novembre, G., & Hove, M. J. (2014). Rhythm in joint action: psychological and neurophysiological mechanisms for real-time interpersonal coordination. Philosophical Transactions of the Royal Society B, 369, 20130394.CrossRef Keller, P. E., Novembre, G., & Hove, M. J. (2014). Rhythm in joint action: psychological and neurophysiological mechanisms for real-time interpersonal coordination. Philosophical Transactions of the Royal Society B, 369, 20130394.CrossRef
go back to reference Kirschner, S., & Tomasello, M. (2009). Joint drumming: Social context facilitates synchronization in preschool children. Journal of Experimental Child Psychology, 102(3), 299–314.PubMedCrossRef Kirschner, S., & Tomasello, M. (2009). Joint drumming: Social context facilitates synchronization in preschool children. Journal of Experimental Child Psychology, 102(3), 299–314.PubMedCrossRef
go back to reference Koelsch, S., Fritz, T., & Schlaug, G. (2008). Amygdala activity can be modulated by unexpected chord functions during music listening. NeuroReport, 19(18), 1815–1819.PubMedCrossRef Koelsch, S., Fritz, T., & Schlaug, G. (2008). Amygdala activity can be modulated by unexpected chord functions during music listening. NeuroReport, 19(18), 1815–1819.PubMedCrossRef
go back to reference Landry, S. P., & Champoux, F. (2017). Musicians react faster and are better multisensory integrators. Brain and Cognition, 111, 156–162.PubMedCrossRef Landry, S. P., & Champoux, F. (2017). Musicians react faster and are better multisensory integrators. Brain and Cognition, 111, 156–162.PubMedCrossRef
go back to reference Lee, D. J., Chen, Y., & Schlaug, G. (2003). Corpus callosum: Musician and gender effects. NeuroReport, 14, 205–209.PubMedCrossRef Lee, D. J., Chen, Y., & Schlaug, G. (2003). Corpus callosum: Musician and gender effects. NeuroReport, 14, 205–209.PubMedCrossRef
go back to reference Leman, M. (2007). Embodied music cognition and mediation technology. Cambridge: MIT Press.CrossRef Leman, M. (2007). Embodied music cognition and mediation technology. Cambridge: MIT Press.CrossRef
go back to reference Leman, M. (2012). Musical entrainment subsumes bodily gestures—Its definition needs a spatiotemporal dimension. Empirical Musicology Review, 7, 63–67.CrossRef Leman, M. (2012). Musical entrainment subsumes bodily gestures—Its definition needs a spatiotemporal dimension. Empirical Musicology Review, 7, 63–67.CrossRef
go back to reference Lindenberger, U., Li, S. C., Gruber, W., & Müller, V. (2009). Brains swinging in concert: Cortical phase synchronization while playing guitar. BMC Neuroscience, 10, 22.PubMedPubMedCentralCrossRef Lindenberger, U., Li, S. C., Gruber, W., & Müller, V. (2009). Brains swinging in concert: Cortical phase synchronization while playing guitar. BMC Neuroscience, 10, 22.PubMedPubMedCentralCrossRef
go back to reference Loehr, J. D., Kourtis, D., Vesper, C., Sebanz, N., & Günther, K. (2013). Monitoring individual and joint action outcomes in duet music performance. Journal of cognitive neuroscience, 25(7), 1049–1061.PubMedCrossRef Loehr, J. D., Kourtis, D., Vesper, C., Sebanz, N., & Günther, K. (2013). Monitoring individual and joint action outcomes in duet music performance. Journal of cognitive neuroscience, 25(7), 1049–1061.PubMedCrossRef
go back to reference Loehr, J. D., & Palmer, C. (2011). Temporal coordination between performing musicians. Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology, 64(11), 2153–2167.CrossRef Loehr, J. D., & Palmer, C. (2011). Temporal coordination between performing musicians. Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology, 64(11), 2153–2167.CrossRef
go back to reference Müller, V., Delius, J. A. M., & Lindenberger, U. (2018). Complex networks emerging during choir singing. Annals of the New York Academy of Sciences, 1431(1), 85–101.PubMedCrossRef Müller, V., Delius, J. A. M., & Lindenberger, U. (2018). Complex networks emerging during choir singing. Annals of the New York Academy of Sciences, 1431(1), 85–101.PubMedCrossRef
go back to reference Munte, T., Altenmüller, E., & Jäncke, L. (2002). The musician’s brain as a model of neuroplasticity. Nature Reviews Neuroscience, 3, 473–478.PubMedCrossRef Munte, T., Altenmüller, E., & Jäncke, L. (2002). The musician’s brain as a model of neuroplasticity. Nature Reviews Neuroscience, 3, 473–478.PubMedCrossRef
go back to reference Naveda, L., & Leman, M. (2010). The spatiotemporal representation of dance and music gestures using topological gesture analysis (TGA). Music Perception, 28(1), 93–111.CrossRef Naveda, L., & Leman, M. (2010). The spatiotemporal representation of dance and music gestures using topological gesture analysis (TGA). Music Perception, 28(1), 93–111.CrossRef
go back to reference Nijs, L. (2017). The merging of musician and musical instrument: an internal model-based approach. In M. Lesaffre, M. Leman, & P. J. Maes (Eds.), Routledge Companion to Embodied Musical Interaction. London: Routledge. Nijs, L. (2017). The merging of musician and musical instrument: an internal model-based approach. In M. Lesaffre, M. Leman, & P. J. Maes (Eds.), Routledge Companion to Embodied Musical Interaction. London: Routledge.
go back to reference Pellencin, E., Paladino, M. P., Herbelin, B., & Serino, A. (2018). Social perception of others shapes one's own multisensory peripersonal space. Cortex, 104, 163–179.PubMedCrossRef Pellencin, E., Paladino, M. P., Herbelin, B., & Serino, A. (2018). Social perception of others shapes one's own multisensory peripersonal space. Cortex, 104, 163–179.PubMedCrossRef
go back to reference Phillips-Silver, J., & Keller, P. E. (2012). Searching for roots of entrainment and joint action in early musical interactions. Frontiers in Human Neuroscience, 6, 26.PubMedPubMedCentralCrossRef Phillips-Silver, J., & Keller, P. E. (2012). Searching for roots of entrainment and joint action in early musical interactions. Frontiers in Human Neuroscience, 6, 26.PubMedPubMedCentralCrossRef
go back to reference Schäfer, T., Fachner, J., & Smukalla, M. (2013). Changes in the representation of space and time while listening to music. Frontiers in Psychology, 4, 508.PubMedPubMedCentral Schäfer, T., Fachner, J., & Smukalla, M. (2013). Changes in the representation of space and time while listening to music. Frontiers in Psychology, 4, 508.PubMedPubMedCentral
go back to reference Schiavio, A., & De Jaegher, H. (2017). Participatory sense-making in Joint musical practice. In M. Lesaffre, P. J. Maes, & M. Leman (Eds.), Routledge Companion to Embodied Music Interaction (pp. 31–39). New York and London: Routledge.CrossRef Schiavio, A., & De Jaegher, H. (2017). Participatory sense-making in Joint musical practice. In M. Lesaffre, P. J. Maes, & M. Leman (Eds.), Routledge Companion to Embodied Music Interaction (pp. 31–39). New York and London: Routledge.CrossRef
go back to reference Serino, A., Bassolino, M., Farnè, A., & Làdavas, E. (2007). Extended multisensory space in blind cane users. Psychological Science, 18(7), 642–648.PubMedCrossRef Serino, A., Bassolino, M., Farnè, A., & Làdavas, E. (2007). Extended multisensory space in blind cane users. Psychological Science, 18(7), 642–648.PubMedCrossRef
go back to reference Shrem, T., Murray, M., & Deouell, L. (2017). Auditory-visual integration modulates location-specific repetition suppression of auditory responses. Psychophysiology, 54(11), 1663–1675.PubMedCrossRef Shrem, T., Murray, M., & Deouell, L. (2017). Auditory-visual integration modulates location-specific repetition suppression of auditory responses. Psychophysiology, 54(11), 1663–1675.PubMedCrossRef
go back to reference Soliman, T. M., Ferguson, R., Dexheimer, M. S., & Glenberg, A. M. (2015). Consequences of joint action: Entanglement with your partner. Journal of Experimental Psychology: General, 144(4), 873–888.CrossRef Soliman, T. M., Ferguson, R., Dexheimer, M. S., & Glenberg, A. M. (2015). Consequences of joint action: Entanglement with your partner. Journal of Experimental Psychology: General, 144(4), 873–888.CrossRef
go back to reference Spence, C., Pavani, F., & Driver, J. (1998). What crossing the hands can reveal about crossmodal links in spatial attention. Abstracts of the Psychonomic Society, 3, 13. Spence, C., Pavani, F., & Driver, J. (1998). What crossing the hands can reveal about crossmodal links in spatial attention. Abstracts of the Psychonomic Society, 3, 13.
go back to reference Stupacher, J., Maes, P. J., Witte, M., & Wood, G. (2017). Music strengthens prosocial effects of interpersonal synchronization—If you move in time with the beat. Journal of Experimental Social Psychology, 72, 39–44.CrossRef Stupacher, J., Maes, P. J., Witte, M., & Wood, G. (2017). Music strengthens prosocial effects of interpersonal synchronization—If you move in time with the beat. Journal of Experimental Social Psychology, 72, 39–44.CrossRef
go back to reference Teneggi, C., Canzoneri, E., di Pellegrino, G., & Serino, A. (2013). Social modulation of peripersonal space boundaries. Current Biology, 23(5), 406–411.PubMedCrossRef Teneggi, C., Canzoneri, E., di Pellegrino, G., & Serino, A. (2013). Social modulation of peripersonal space boundaries. Current Biology, 23(5), 406–411.PubMedCrossRef
go back to reference Walton, A., Richardson, M. J., Langland-Hassan, P., & Chemero, A. (2015). Improvisation and the self-organization of multiple musical bodies. Frontiers in Psychology., 6, 313.PubMedPubMedCentralCrossRef Walton, A., Richardson, M. J., Langland-Hassan, P., & Chemero, A. (2015). Improvisation and the self-organization of multiple musical bodies. Frontiers in Psychology., 6, 313.PubMedPubMedCentralCrossRef
go back to reference Zamm, A., Pfordresher, P. Q., & Palmer, C. (2015). Temporal coordination in joint music performance: Effects of endogenous rhythms and auditory feedback. Experimental Brain Research, 233, 607–615.PubMedCrossRef Zamm, A., Pfordresher, P. Q., & Palmer, C. (2015). Temporal coordination in joint music performance: Effects of endogenous rhythms and auditory feedback. Experimental Brain Research, 233, 607–615.PubMedCrossRef
go back to reference Zimmerman, E., & Lahav, A. (2012). The multisensory brain and its ability to learn music. Annals of the New York Academy of Sciences, 1252, 179–184.PubMedCrossRef Zimmerman, E., & Lahav, A. (2012). The multisensory brain and its ability to learn music. Annals of the New York Academy of Sciences, 1252, 179–184.PubMedCrossRef
Metagegevens
Titel
Does musical interaction in a jazz duet modulate peripersonal space?
Auteurs
A. Dell’Anna
M. Rosso
V. Bruno
F. Garbarini
M. Leman
A. Berti
Publicatiedatum
01-06-2020
Uitgeverij
Springer Berlin Heidelberg
Gepubliceerd in
Psychological Research / Uitgave 5/2021
Print ISSN: 0340-0727
Elektronisch ISSN: 1430-2772
DOI
https://doi.org/10.1007/s00426-020-01365-6

Andere artikelen Uitgave 5/2021

Psychological Research 5/2021 Naar de uitgave