Skip to main content
Top
Gepubliceerd in: Psychological Research 7/2021

22-11-2020 | Original Article

Sustained attention and the experience of flow

Auteurs: Jeremy Marty-Dugas, Laura Howes, Daniel Smilek

Gepubliceerd in: Psychological Research | Uitgave 7/2021

Log in om toegang te krijgen
share
DELEN

Deel dit onderdeel of sectie (kopieer de link)

  • Optie A:
    Klik op de rechtermuisknop op de link en selecteer de optie “linkadres kopiëren”
  • Optie B:
    Deel de link per e-mail

Abstract

The state of flow—often referred to as being “in the zone”—is characterized by the experience of deep, effortless concentration on the activity one is engaged in. While much of the flow literature seems to imply a tight link between flow and attention processes, relatively little work has assessed this question empirically. In the present study, we explored how the experience of flow relates to behavioural performance on the Sustained Attention to Response Task (SART). Flow was indexed at the state level using thought-probes and at the trait level via questionnaires. The main finding was that those who experienced more state-level flow during the SART made fewer commission errors during the task, indicating that flow is linked to better sustained attention. Interestingly, the correlation between flow and sustained attention performance was found to increase in the second half of the task. While trait flow was not related to SART performance, it was found to be predictive of state flow during the task, such that those who tended to experience higher levels of flow in their everyday lives also experienced more flow during the experiment.
Bijlagen
Alleen toegankelijk voor geautoriseerde gebruikers
Voetnoten
1
Clearly, the authors’ findings support the conclusion that flow is not related to intelligence. However, it is less clear that the same measures can be interpreted as measures of ‘effortful’ or ‘sustained’ attention.
 
2
We do not mean to imply that intelligence and sustained attention abilities are orthogonal—merely that there are other available metrics which are perhaps better suited for assessing sustained attention (i.e. that have been designed specifically to assess sustained attention abilities) than an intelligence test.
 
3
Following the completion of the d2 test, participants completed four five-minute blocks of Raven’s Matrices and reported their experience of flow after each block. However, the authors did not report the correlation between the experience of state flow and performance on the Raven’s matrices.
 
4
There were two participant who either declined to answer or left the question blank.
 
5
Four participants from Sample 1 and six participants from Sample 2 removed their earmuffs at some point during the experiment and are noted in the associated R script. These participants are included in all our analyses.
 
6
Including the 97 participants who participated in the laboratory portion of the study.
 
7
Indeed, this further highlights the importance of assessing sustained attention in longer task than had been used in prior studies.
 
Literatuur
go back to reference Aherne, C., Moran, A. P., & Lonsdale, C. (2011). The effect of mindfulness training on athletes’ flow: An initial investigation. The Sport Psychologist, 25(2), 177–189.CrossRef Aherne, C., Moran, A. P., & Lonsdale, C. (2011). The effect of mindfulness training on athletes’ flow: An initial investigation. The Sport Psychologist, 25(2), 177–189.CrossRef
go back to reference Asakawa, K. (2010). Flow experience, culture, and well-being: How do autotelic Japanese college students feel, behave, and think in their daily lives? Journal of Happiness Studies, 11(2), 205–223.CrossRef Asakawa, K. (2010). Flow experience, culture, and well-being: How do autotelic Japanese college students feel, behave, and think in their daily lives? Journal of Happiness Studies, 11(2), 205–223.CrossRef
go back to reference Brickenkamp, R. (2002) Test d2. Aufmerksamkeits-Belastungs-Test (8. Auflage). Goettingen: Hogrefe. Brickenkamp, R. (2002) Test d2. Aufmerksamkeits-Belastungs-Test (8. Auflage). Goettingen: Hogrefe.
go back to reference Brown, K. W., & Ryan, R. M. (2003). The benefits of being present: Mindfulness and its role in psychological well-being. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 84(4), 822.PubMedCrossRef Brown, K. W., & Ryan, R. M. (2003). The benefits of being present: Mindfulness and its role in psychological well-being. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 84(4), 822.PubMedCrossRef
go back to reference Carriere, J. S. A., Seli, P., & Smilek, D. (2013). Wandering in both mind and body: Individual differences in mind wandering and inattention predict fidgeting. Canadian Journal of Experimental Psychology/Revue Canadienne De Psychologie Expérimentale, 67(1), 19–31. https://doi.org/10.1037/a0031438CrossRefPubMed Carriere, J. S. A., Seli, P., & Smilek, D. (2013). Wandering in both mind and body: Individual differences in mind wandering and inattention predict fidgeting. Canadian Journal of Experimental Psychology/Revue Canadienne De Psychologie Expérimentale, 67(1), 19–31. https://​doi.​org/​10.​1037/​a0031438CrossRefPubMed
go back to reference Cermakova, L., Moneta, G. B., & Spada, M. M. (2010). Dispositional flow as a mediator of the relationships between attentional control and approaches to studying during academic examination preparation. Educational Psychology, 30(5), 495–511.CrossRef Cermakova, L., Moneta, G. B., & Spada, M. M. (2010). Dispositional flow as a mediator of the relationships between attentional control and approaches to studying during academic examination preparation. Educational Psychology, 30(5), 495–511.CrossRef
go back to reference Chen, J. H., Tsai, P. H., Lin, Y. C., Chen, C. K., & Chen, C. Y. (2019). Mindfulness training enhances flow state and mental health among baseball players in Taiwan. Psychology Research and Behavior Management, 12, 15.PubMedCrossRef Chen, J. H., Tsai, P. H., Lin, Y. C., Chen, C. K., & Chen, C. Y. (2019). Mindfulness training enhances flow state and mental health among baseball players in Taiwan. Psychology Research and Behavior Management, 12, 15.PubMedCrossRef
go back to reference Cheyne, J. A., Solman, G. J., Carriere, J. S., & Smilek, D. (2009). Anatomy of an error: A bidirectional state model of task engagement/disengagement and attention-related errors. Cognition, 111(1), 98–113.CrossRef Cheyne, J. A., Solman, G. J., Carriere, J. S., & Smilek, D. (2009). Anatomy of an error: A bidirectional state model of task engagement/disengagement and attention-related errors. Cognition, 111(1), 98–113.CrossRef
go back to reference Csikszentmihalyi, M. (1975/2000). Beyond boredom and anxiety. Jossey-Bass. (Original work published 1975). Csikszentmihalyi, M. (1975/2000). Beyond boredom and anxiety. Jossey-Bass. (Original work published 1975).
go back to reference Csikszentmihalyi, M. (1978). Attention and the Holistic Approach to Behaviour. In K. S. Pope & J. L. Singer (Eds.), The Stream of Consciousness (pp. 335–358). New York: Plenum.CrossRef Csikszentmihalyi, M. (1978). Attention and the Holistic Approach to Behaviour. In K. S. Pope & J. L. Singer (Eds.), The Stream of Consciousness (pp. 335–358). New York: Plenum.CrossRef
go back to reference Csikszentmihalyi, M. (1988). The flow experience and its significance for human psychology. In M. Csikszentmihalyi & I. S. Csikszentmihalyi (Eds.), Optimal experience: Psychological studies of flow in consciousness (pp. 15–35). Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.CrossRef Csikszentmihalyi, M. (1988). The flow experience and its significance for human psychology. In M. Csikszentmihalyi & I. S. Csikszentmihalyi (Eds.), Optimal experience: Psychological studies of flow in consciousness (pp. 15–35). Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.CrossRef
go back to reference Csikszentmihalyi, M., & Nakamura, J. (2010). Effortless attention in everyday life: A systematic phenomenology. In B. Bruya (Ed.), Effortless attention: A new perspective in the cognitive science of attention and action (pp. 179–190). Cambridge: The MIT Press.CrossRef Csikszentmihalyi, M., & Nakamura, J. (2010). Effortless attention in everyday life: A systematic phenomenology. In B. Bruya (Ed.), Effortless attention: A new perspective in the cognitive science of attention and action (pp. 179–190). Cambridge: The MIT Press.CrossRef
go back to reference Davies, D. R., & Parasuraman, R. (1982). The Psychology of Vigilance. London: Academic Press. Davies, D. R., & Parasuraman, R. (1982). The Psychology of Vigilance. London: Academic Press.
go back to reference Deci, E. L. (1971). Effects of externally mediated rewards on intrinsic motivation. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 18(1), 105.CrossRef Deci, E. L. (1971). Effects of externally mediated rewards on intrinsic motivation. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 18(1), 105.CrossRef
go back to reference Dember, W. N., Galinsky, T. L., & Warm, J. S. (1992). The role of choice in vigilance performance. Bulletin of the Psychonomic Society, 30(3), 201–204.CrossRef Dember, W. N., Galinsky, T. L., & Warm, J. S. (1992). The role of choice in vigilance performance. Bulletin of the Psychonomic Society, 30(3), 201–204.CrossRef
go back to reference Derryberry, D., & Reed, M. A. (2002). Anxiety-related attentional biases and their regulation by attentional control. Journal of Abnormal Psychology, 111(2), 225.PubMedCrossRef Derryberry, D., & Reed, M. A. (2002). Anxiety-related attentional biases and their regulation by attentional control. Journal of Abnormal Psychology, 111(2), 225.PubMedCrossRef
go back to reference de Sampaio Barros, M. F., Araújo-Moreira, F. M., Trevelin, L. C., & Radel, R. (2018). Flow experience and the mobilization of attentional resources. Cognitive, Affective, & Behavioral Neuroscience, 18(4), 810–823.CrossRef de Sampaio Barros, M. F., Araújo-Moreira, F. M., Trevelin, L. C., & Radel, R. (2018). Flow experience and the mobilization of attentional resources. Cognitive, Affective, & Behavioral Neuroscience, 18(4), 810–823.CrossRef
go back to reference Han, S. (1988). The relationship between life satisfaction and flow in elderly Korean immigrants. In M. Csikszentmihalyi & I. S. Csikszentmihalyi (Eds.), Optimal experience: Psychological studies of flow in consciousness (pp. 138–149). Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.CrossRef Han, S. (1988). The relationship between life satisfaction and flow in elderly Korean immigrants. In M. Csikszentmihalyi & I. S. Csikszentmihalyi (Eds.), Optimal experience: Psychological studies of flow in consciousness (pp. 138–149). Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.CrossRef
go back to reference Jackson, S. A., & Eklund, R. C. (2002). Assessing flow in physical activity: The flow state scale–2 and dispositional flow scale–2. Journal of Sport and Exercise Psychology, 24(2), 133–150.CrossRef Jackson, S. A., & Eklund, R. C. (2002). Assessing flow in physical activity: The flow state scale–2 and dispositional flow scale–2. Journal of Sport and Exercise Psychology, 24(2), 133–150.CrossRef
go back to reference Jackson, S. A., & Marsh, H. W. (1996). Development and validation of a scale to measure optimal experience: The Flow State Scale. Journal of Sport and Exercise psychology, 18(1), 17–35.CrossRef Jackson, S. A., & Marsh, H. W. (1996). Development and validation of a scale to measure optimal experience: The Flow State Scale. Journal of Sport and Exercise psychology, 18(1), 17–35.CrossRef
go back to reference Johnson, K. A., Kelly, S. P., Bellgrove, M. A., Barry, E., Cox, M., Gill, M., & Robertson, I. H. (2007). Response variability in attention deficit hyperactivity disorder: evidence for neuropsychological heterogeneity. Neuropsychologia, 45(4), 630–638.PubMedCrossRef Johnson, K. A., Kelly, S. P., Bellgrove, M. A., Barry, E., Cox, M., Gill, M., & Robertson, I. H. (2007). Response variability in attention deficit hyperactivity disorder: evidence for neuropsychological heterogeneity. Neuropsychologia, 45(4), 630–638.PubMedCrossRef
go back to reference Johnson, K. A., Robertson, I. H., Kelly, S. P., Silk, T. J., Barry, E., Dáibhis, A., & Gill, M. (2007). Dissociation in performance of children with ADHD and high-functioning autism on a task of sustained attention. Neuropsychologia, 45(10), 2234–2245.PubMedPubMedCentralCrossRef Johnson, K. A., Robertson, I. H., Kelly, S. P., Silk, T. J., Barry, E., Dáibhis, A., & Gill, M. (2007). Dissociation in performance of children with ADHD and high-functioning autism on a task of sustained attention. Neuropsychologia, 45(10), 2234–2245.PubMedPubMedCentralCrossRef
go back to reference Kaufman, K. A., Glass, C. R., & Pineau, T. R. (2018). Mindful sport performance enhancement: Mental training for athletes and coaches. American Psychological Association. Kaufman, K. A., Glass, C. R., & Pineau, T. R. (2018). Mindful sport performance enhancement: Mental training for athletes and coaches. American Psychological Association.
go back to reference Lee, I. A., & Preacher, K. J. (2013). Calculation for the test of the difference between two dependent correlations with no variable in common [Computer software]. Available from https://quantpsy.org. Lee, I. A., & Preacher, K. J. (2013). Calculation for the test of the difference between two dependent correlations with no variable in common [Computer software]. Available from https://​quantpsy.​org.
go back to reference Lepper, M. R., Greene, D., & Nisbett, R. E. (1973). Undermining children’s intrinsic interest with extrinsic reward: A test of the" overjustification" hypothesis. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 28(1), 129.CrossRef Lepper, M. R., Greene, D., & Nisbett, R. E. (1973). Undermining children’s intrinsic interest with extrinsic reward: A test of the" overjustification" hypothesis. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 28(1), 129.CrossRef
go back to reference Macbeth, J. (1988). Ocean cruising. In M. Csikszentmihalyi & I. S. Csikszentmihalyi (Eds.), Optimal experience: Psychological studies of flow in consciousness (pp. 214–231). Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.CrossRef Macbeth, J. (1988). Ocean cruising. In M. Csikszentmihalyi & I. S. Csikszentmihalyi (Eds.), Optimal experience: Psychological studies of flow in consciousness (pp. 214–231). Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.CrossRef
go back to reference Mackworth, N. H. (1948). The breakdown of vigilance during prolonged visual search. Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology, 1(1), 6–21.CrossRef Mackworth, N. H. (1948). The breakdown of vigilance during prolonged visual search. Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology, 1(1), 6–21.CrossRef
go back to reference Marty-Dugas, J., & Smilek, D. (2019). Deep, effortless concentration: Re-examining the flow concept and exploring relations with inattention, absorption, and personality. Psychological Research Psychologische Forschung, 83(8), 1760–1777.PubMedCrossRef Marty-Dugas, J., & Smilek, D. (2019). Deep, effortless concentration: Re-examining the flow concept and exploring relations with inattention, absorption, and personality. Psychological Research Psychologische Forschung, 83(8), 1760–1777.PubMedCrossRef
go back to reference Moore, B. A. (2013). Propensity for experiencing flow: The roles of cognitive flexibility and mindfulness. The Humanistic Psychologist, 41(4), 319.CrossRef Moore, B. A. (2013). Propensity for experiencing flow: The roles of cognitive flexibility and mindfulness. The Humanistic Psychologist, 41(4), 319.CrossRef
go back to reference Mrazek, M. D., Smallwood, J., Franklin, M. S., Chin, J. M., Baird, B., & Schooler, J. W. (2012). The role of mind-wandering in measurements of general aptitude. Journal of Experimental Psychology: General, 141(4), 788.CrossRef Mrazek, M. D., Smallwood, J., Franklin, M. S., Chin, J. M., Baird, B., & Schooler, J. W. (2012). The role of mind-wandering in measurements of general aptitude. Journal of Experimental Psychology: General, 141(4), 788.CrossRef
go back to reference Nakamura, J., & Csikszentmihalyi, M. (2002). The concept of flow. In C. R. Snyder & S. J. Lopez (Eds.), Handbook of positive psychology (pp. 89–105). New York: Oxford University Press. Nakamura, J., & Csikszentmihalyi, M. (2002). The concept of flow. In C. R. Snyder & S. J. Lopez (Eds.), Handbook of positive psychology (pp. 89–105). New York: Oxford University Press.
go back to reference Raven. J. C. (1962). Advanced Progressive Matrices. Set II. London: H. K. Lewis & Co. Distributed in the USA by The Psychological Corporation. San Antonio. Texas. Raven. J. C. (1962). Advanced Progressive Matrices. Set II. London: H. K. Lewis & Co. Distributed in the USA by The Psychological Corporation. San Antonio. Texas.
go back to reference Raven. J. C. (1965). Advanced Progressive Matrices. Sets I and II. London: H. K. Lewis & Co. Distributed in the USA by The Psychological Corporation. San Antonio, Texas. Raven. J. C. (1965). Advanced Progressive Matrices. Sets I and II. London: H. K. Lewis & Co. Distributed in the USA by The Psychological Corporation. San Antonio, Texas.
go back to reference Risko, E. F., Anderson, N., Sarwal, A., Engelhardt, M., & Kingstone, A. (2012). Everyday attention: Variation in mind wandering and memory in a lecture. Applied Cognitive Psychology, 26(2), 234–242.CrossRef Risko, E. F., Anderson, N., Sarwal, A., Engelhardt, M., & Kingstone, A. (2012). Everyday attention: Variation in mind wandering and memory in a lecture. Applied Cognitive Psychology, 26(2), 234–242.CrossRef
go back to reference Robertson, I. H., Manly, T., Andrade, J., Baddeley, B. T., & Yiend, J. (1997). Oops!: Performance correlates of everyday attentional failures: the Sustained Attention to Response Task (SART). Neuropsychologia, 35, 747–758.PubMedCrossRef Robertson, I. H., Manly, T., Andrade, J., Baddeley, B. T., & Yiend, J. (1997). Oops!: Performance correlates of everyday attentional failures: the Sustained Attention to Response Task (SART). Neuropsychologia, 35, 747–758.PubMedCrossRef
go back to reference Schiefele, U., & Raabe, A. (2011). Skills-demands compatibility as a determinant of flow experience in an inductive reasoning task. Psychological Reports, 109(2), 428–444.PubMedCrossRef Schiefele, U., & Raabe, A. (2011). Skills-demands compatibility as a determinant of flow experience in an inductive reasoning task. Psychological Reports, 109(2), 428–444.PubMedCrossRef
go back to reference Scott-Hamilton, J., Schutte, N. S., & Brown, R. F. (2016). Effects of a mindfulness intervention on sports-anxiety, pessimism, and flow in competitive cyclists. Applied Psychology: Health and Well-Being, 8(1), 85–103. Scott-Hamilton, J., Schutte, N. S., & Brown, R. F. (2016). Effects of a mindfulness intervention on sports-anxiety, pessimism, and flow in competitive cyclists. Applied Psychology: Health and Well-Being, 8(1), 85–103.
go back to reference Seli, P., Cheyne, J. A., & Smilek, D. (2012). Attention failures versus misplaced diligence: Separating attention lapses from speed–accuracy trade-offs. Consciousness and Cognition, 21(1), 277–291.PubMedCrossRef Seli, P., Cheyne, J. A., & Smilek, D. (2012). Attention failures versus misplaced diligence: Separating attention lapses from speed–accuracy trade-offs. Consciousness and Cognition, 21(1), 277–291.PubMedCrossRef
go back to reference Seli, P., Schacter, D. L., Risko, E. F., & Smilek, D. (2019). Increasing participant motivation reduces rates of intentional and unintentional mind wandering. Psychological Research Psychologische Forschung, 83(5), 1057–1069.PubMedCrossRef Seli, P., Schacter, D. L., Risko, E. F., & Smilek, D. (2019). Increasing participant motivation reduces rates of intentional and unintentional mind wandering. Psychological Research Psychologische Forschung, 83(5), 1057–1069.PubMedCrossRef
go back to reference Smallwood, J., Davies, J. B., Heim, D., Finnigan, F., Sudberry, M., O’Connor, R., & Obonsawin, M. (2004). Subjective experience and the attentional lapse: Task engagement and disengagement during sustained attention. Consciousness and Cognition, 13(4), 657–690.PubMedCrossRef Smallwood, J., Davies, J. B., Heim, D., Finnigan, F., Sudberry, M., O’Connor, R., & Obonsawin, M. (2004). Subjective experience and the attentional lapse: Task engagement and disengagement during sustained attention. Consciousness and Cognition, 13(4), 657–690.PubMedCrossRef
go back to reference Smallwood, J., McSpadden, M., & Schooler, J. W. (2007). The lights are on but no one’s home: Meta-awareness and the decoupling of attention when the mind wanders. Psychonomic Bulletin & Review, 14(3), 527–533.CrossRef Smallwood, J., McSpadden, M., & Schooler, J. W. (2007). The lights are on but no one’s home: Meta-awareness and the decoupling of attention when the mind wanders. Psychonomic Bulletin & Review, 14(3), 527–533.CrossRef
go back to reference Smilek, D., Carriere, J. S., & Cheyne, J. A. (2010). Out of mind, out of sight: eye blinking as indicator and embodiment of mind wandering. Psychological Science, 21(6), 786–789.PubMedCrossRef Smilek, D., Carriere, J. S., & Cheyne, J. A. (2010). Out of mind, out of sight: eye blinking as indicator and embodiment of mind wandering. Psychological Science, 21(6), 786–789.PubMedCrossRef
go back to reference Swann, C., Keegan, R. J., Piggott, D., & Crust, L. (2012). A systematic review of the experience, occurrence, and controllability of flow states in elite sport. Psychology of Sport and Exercise, 13(6), 807–819.CrossRef Swann, C., Keegan, R. J., Piggott, D., & Crust, L. (2012). A systematic review of the experience, occurrence, and controllability of flow states in elite sport. Psychology of Sport and Exercise, 13(6), 807–819.CrossRef
go back to reference Szalma, J. L., & Hancock, P. A. (2006, October). Performance, workload, and stress in vigilance: The power of choice. In Proceedings of the Human Factors and Ergonomics Society Annual Meeting (Vol. 50, No. 16, pp. 1609–1613). Los Angeles: SAGE Publications. Szalma, J. L., & Hancock, P. A. (2006, October). Performance, workload, and stress in vigilance: The power of choice. In Proceedings of the Human Factors and Ergonomics Society Annual Meeting (Vol. 50, No. 16, pp. 1609–1613). Los Angeles: SAGE Publications.
go back to reference Tse, D. C., Lau, V. W. Y., Perlman, R., & McLaughlin, M. (2018). The Development and Validation of the Autotelic Personality Questionnaire. Journal of personality assessment, 1–14. Tse, D. C., Lau, V. W. Y., Perlman, R., & McLaughlin, M. (2018). The Development and Validation of the Autotelic Personality Questionnaire. Journal of personality assessment, 1–14.
go back to reference Ullén, F., de Manzano, Ö., Almeida, R., Magnusson, P. K., Pedersen, N. L., Nakamura, J., & Madison, G. (2012). Proneness for psychological flow in everyday life: Associations with personality and intelligence. Personality and Individual Differences, 52(2), 167–172.CrossRef Ullén, F., de Manzano, Ö., Almeida, R., Magnusson, P. K., Pedersen, N. L., Nakamura, J., & Madison, G. (2012). Proneness for psychological flow in everyday life: Associations with personality and intelligence. Personality and Individual Differences, 52(2), 167–172.CrossRef
go back to reference Ullén, F., de Manzano, Ö., Theorell, T., & Harmat, L. (2010). 10 The Physiology of Effortless Attention: Correlates of State Flow and Flow Proneness (p. 205). Effortless attention: A new perspective in the cognitive science of attention and action.CrossRef Ullén, F., de Manzano, Ö., Theorell, T., & Harmat, L. (2010). 10 The Physiology of Effortless Attention: Correlates of State Flow and Flow Proneness (p. 205). Effortless attention: A new perspective in the cognitive science of attention and action.CrossRef
go back to reference Ullén, F., Harmat, L., Theorell, T., & Madison, G. (2016). Flow and individual differences–a phenotypic analysis of data from more than 10,000 twin individuals. In L. Harmat, F. O. Anderson, F. Ullén, J. Wright, & G. Sadlo (Eds.), Flow Experience (pp. 267–288). New York: Springer International Publishing.CrossRef Ullén, F., Harmat, L., Theorell, T., & Madison, G. (2016). Flow and individual differences–a phenotypic analysis of data from more than 10,000 twin individuals. In L. Harmat, F. O. Anderson, F. Ullén, J. Wright, & G. Sadlo (Eds.), Flow Experience (pp. 267–288). New York: Springer International Publishing.CrossRef
go back to reference Vuorre, M., & Metcalfe, J. (2016). The relation between the sense of agency and the experience of flow. Consciousness and Cognition, 43, 133–142.PubMedCrossRef Vuorre, M., & Metcalfe, J. (2016). The relation between the sense of agency and the experience of flow. Consciousness and Cognition, 43, 133–142.PubMedCrossRef
go back to reference Wammes, J. D., Boucher, P. O., Seli, P., Cheyne, J. A., & Smilek, D. (2016). Mind wandering during lectures I: Changes in rates across an entire semester. Scholarship of Teaching and Learning in Psychology, 2(1), 13.CrossRef Wammes, J. D., Boucher, P. O., Seli, P., Cheyne, J. A., & Smilek, D. (2016). Mind wandering during lectures I: Changes in rates across an entire semester. Scholarship of Teaching and Learning in Psychology, 2(1), 13.CrossRef
go back to reference Wammes, J. D., & Smilek, D. (2017). Examining the influence of lecture format on degree of mind wandering. Journal of Applied Research in Memory and Cognition, 6(2), 174–184.CrossRef Wammes, J. D., & Smilek, D. (2017). Examining the influence of lecture format on degree of mind wandering. Journal of Applied Research in Memory and Cognition, 6(2), 174–184.CrossRef
go back to reference Warm, J. S. (1984). Sustained attention in human performance. Hoboken: John Wiley & Sons Inc. Warm, J. S. (1984). Sustained attention in human performance. Hoboken: John Wiley & Sons Inc.
Metagegevens
Titel
Sustained attention and the experience of flow
Auteurs
Jeremy Marty-Dugas
Laura Howes
Daniel Smilek
Publicatiedatum
22-11-2020
Uitgeverij
Springer Berlin Heidelberg
Gepubliceerd in
Psychological Research / Uitgave 7/2021
Print ISSN: 0340-0727
Elektronisch ISSN: 1430-2772
DOI
https://doi.org/10.1007/s00426-020-01433-x

Andere artikelen Uitgave 7/2021

Psychological Research 7/2021 Naar de uitgave